Alexithymia from Wikipedia, the Free Encyclopedia

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Alexithymia from Wikipedia, the Free Encyclopedia Alexithymia From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Alexithymia (pronounced /əˌlɛksəˈθaɪmiə/) from the Greek words λεξις and θυµος (literally "without words for emotions") is a term coined by Peter Sifneos in 1973[1][2] to describe a state of deficiency in understanding, processing, or describing emotions. Classification Alexithymia is considered to be a personality trait that places individuals at risk for other medical and psychiatric disorders while reducing the likelihood that these individuals will respond to conventional treatments for the other conditions.[3] Alexithymia is not classified as a mental disorder in the DSM IV. It is a personality trait that varies in severity from person to person. A person's alexithymia score can be measured with questionnaires such as the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20), the Bermond-Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire (BVAQ)[4] or the Observer Alexithymia Scale (OAS).[3] Alexithymia is defined by:[5] (i) difficulty identifying feelings and distinguishing between feelings and the bodily sensations of emotional arousal (ii) difficulty describing feelings to other people (iii) constricted imaginal processes, as evidenced by a paucity of fantasies (iv) a stimulus-bound, externally oriented cognitive style. In studies of the general population the degree of alexithymia was found to be influenced by age, but not by gender; the rates of alexithymia in healthy controls have been found at 8.3% (2 of 24 persons) 4.7% (2 of 43), 8.9% (16 of 179), and 7% (4 of 56). Thus, several studies have reported that the prevalence rate of alexithymia is less than 10% in healthy controls.[6] A less common finding suggests that there may be a higher prevalence of alexithymia amongst males than females, which may be accounted for by difficulties they have with 'describing feelings', but not by a difficulty in 'identifying feelings' in which males and females show similar abilities.[7] The alexithymia construct is strongly inversely related to the concepts of psychological mindedness[8] and emotional intelligence[9][10] and M. Bagby and G. Taylor state that there is "strong empirical support for alexithymia being a stable personality trait rather than just a consequence of psychological distress".[11] Other opinions differ and can show evidence that it may be state-dependent.[12] Bagby and Taylor also suggest that there may be two kinds of alexithymia, 'primary alexithymia' which is an enduring psychological trait which does not alter over time, and 'secondary alexithymia' which is state dependent and disappears after the evoking stressful situation has changed. These two manifestations of alexithymia are otherwise called 'trait' or 'state' alexithymia.[13] Typical deficiencies may include problems identifying, describing, and working with one's own feelings, often marked by a lack of understanding of the feelings of others; difficulty distinguishing between feelings and the bodily sensations of emotional arousal;[1] confusion of physical sensations often associated with emotions; few dreams or fantasies due to restricted imagination; and concrete, realistic, logical thinking, often to the exclusion of emotional responses to problems. Those who have alexithymia also report very logical and realistic dreams, such as going to the store or eating a meal.[14] Clinical experience suggests it is the structural features of dreams more than the ability to recall them that best characterizes alexithymia.[1] Some alexithymic individuals may appear to contradict the above mentioned characteristics because they can experience chronic dysphoria or manifest outbursts of crying or rage.[15][16][17] However, questioning usually reveals that they are quite incapable of describing their feelings or appear confused by questions inquiring about specifics of feelings.[5] According to Henry Krystal, individuals suffering from alexithymia think in an operative way and may appear to be superadjusted to reality. In psychotherapy, however, a cognitive disturbance becomes apparent as the patients tends to recount trivial, chronologically ordered actions, reactions, and events of daily life with monotonous detail.[18][19] In general, these individuals lack imagination, intuition, empathy, and drive-fulfillment fantasy, especially in relation to objects. Instead, they seem oriented toward things and even treat themselves as robots. These problems seriously limit their responsiveness to psychoanalytic psychotherapy; psychosomatic illness or substance abuse is frequently exacerbated should these individuals enter psychotherapy.[5] A common misconception about alexithymia is that victims of this construct are totally unable to express emotions verbally and that they may even fail to acknowledge that they experience emotions. Even before coining the term, Sifneos (1967) noted patients often mentioned things like anxiety or depression. The distinguishing factor was their inability to elaborate beyond a few limited adjectives such as "happy" or "unhappy" when describing these feelings.[20] The core issue is that alexithymics have poorly differentiated emotions limiting their ability to distinguish and describe them to others.[1] This contributes to the sense of emotional detachment from themselves and difficulty connecting with others that is typical of the alexithymic's experience.[citation needed] Relational issues According to Vanheule, Desmet and Meganck (2006) alexithymia creates interpersonal problems because these individuals avoid emotionally close relationships, or that if they do form relationships with others they tend to position themselves as either dependent or impersonal, "such that the relationship remains superficial."[21] Inadequate differentiation between self and other by alexithymic individuals has been observed by Blaustein & Tuber (1998) and Taylor et al (1997).[22] In a study, a large group of alexithymic individuals completed the 64-item Inventory of Interpersonal problems (IIP-64) which screens for:[21] (a) Domineering/Controlling, indicating difficulties relinquishing control over others; (b) Vindictive/Self-Centered behavior, which describes problems of hostile dominance and the tendency to fight with others; (c) Cold/Distant behavior, which refers to low degrees of affection for and connection with others; (d) Socially Inhibited, which assesses the tendency to feel anxious and avoidant in the presence of others; (e) Non-Assertiveness, which measures problems in taking initiative in relation to others and coping with social challenges; (f) Overly Accommodating, which indicates an excess of friendly submissiveness; (g) Self-Sacrificing, which indicates a tendency to affiliate excessively; and (h) Intrusive/Needy, which describes problems with friendly dominance. The study found that alexithymic individuals "had significantly higher scores on all IIP-64 subscales than did the nonclinical sample."[21] Chaotic interpersonal relations have also been observed by Sifneos.[23] Due to the inherent difficulties identifying and describing emotional states in self and others, alexithymia also negatively affects relationship satisfaction between couples.[24] In medical and psychiatric illness Alexithymia frequently co-occurs with other disorders, with a representative prevalence of 85% in autism spectrum disorders,[25] 40% in posttraumatic stress disorder,[26] 63% in anorexia nervosa, 56% in bulimia,[27] 45% in major depressive disorder,[12] 34% in panic disorder,[28] and 50% in substance abusers.[29] Research indicates that alexithymia overlaps with Asperger syndrome. In a 2004 study, Uta Frith reported an overlap and that at least half of the Asperger syndrome group obtained scores on the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) that would classify them as severely impaired.[30] Fitzgerald & Bellgrove pointed out that, "Like Alexithymia, Asperger’s syndrome is also characterized by core disturbances in speech and language and social relationships".[31] Hill & Berthoz agreed with Fitzgerald & Bellgrove (2006) and in response stated that "there is some form of overlap between alexithymia and ASDs". They also pointed to studies that revealed impaired Theory of Mind skill in alexithymia, neuroanatomical evidence pointing to a shared etiology and similar social skills deficits.[32] The exact nature of the overlap is uncertain. Alexithymic traits in AS may be linked to depression or anxiety;[30] the mediating factors are unknown and it is possible that alexithymia predisposes to anxiety.[33] Alexithymia involves higher risk of developing certain personality disorders,[34] and is correlated with particular illnesses, such as hypertension,[35] inflammatory bowel disease,[36] functional dyspepsia,[37] sexual disorders,[38] substance use disorders,[39][40] and some anxiety disorders.[41] Alexithymia is further linked with psychosomatic disorders such as migraine headaches, lower back pain, irritable bowel syndrome, asthma, nausea, allergy, and fibromyalgia.[42] An inability to modulate emotions is a possibility in explaining why some alexithymics are prone to discharge tension arising from unpleasant emotional states through impulsive acts or compulsive behaviors such as binge eating,[43] substance abuse,[44] perverse sexual behavior, or the self-starvation of anorexia nervosa.[43] The failure to regulate emotions cognitively might result in prolonged elevations of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) and neuroendocrine systems which can lead to somatic diseases.[42] Alexithymics also show a limited ability to experience positive emotions leading
Recommended publications
  • Alexithymia – Not Autism – Is Associated with Frequency of Social T Interactions in Adults ∗ Alan H
    Behaviour Research and Therapy 123 (2019) 103477 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behaviour Research and Therapy journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat Alexithymia – Not autism – is associated with frequency of social T interactions in adults ∗ Alan H. Gerbera, Jeffrey M. Girardb, Stacey B. Scotta, Matthew D. Lernera, a Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA b Language Technologies Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Objective: While much is known about the quality of social behavior among neurotypical individuals and those Alexithymia with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), little work has evaluated quantity of social interactions. This study used Autism ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to quantify in vivo daily patterns of social interaction in adults as a ASD function of demographic and clinical factors. EMA Method: Adults with and without ASD (NASD = 23, NNeurotypical = 52) were trained in an EMA protocol to report Social interaction their social interactions via smartphone over one week. Participants completed measures of IQ, ASD symptom severity and alexithymia symptom severity. Results: Cyclical multilevel models were used to account for nesting of observations. Results suggest a daily cyclical pattern of social interaction that was robust to ASD and alexithymia symptoms. Adults with ASD did not have fewer social interactions than neurotypical peers; however, severity of alexithymia symptoms predicted fewer social interactions
    [Show full text]
  • A Review of Alexithymia and Emotion Perception in Music, Odor, Taste, and Touch
    MINI REVIEW published: 30 July 2021 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.707599 Beyond Face and Voice: A Review of Alexithymia and Emotion Perception in Music, Odor, Taste, and Touch Thomas Suslow* and Anette Kersting Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University of Leipzig Medical Center, Leipzig, Germany Alexithymia is a clinically relevant personality trait characterized by deficits in recognizing and verbalizing one’s emotions. It has been shown that alexithymia is related to an impaired perception of external emotional stimuli, but previous research focused on emotion perception from faces and voices. Since sensory modalities represent rather distinct input channels it is important to know whether alexithymia also affects emotion perception in other modalities and expressive domains. The objective of our review was to summarize and systematically assess the literature on the impact of alexithymia on the perception of emotional (or hedonic) stimuli in music, odor, taste, and touch. Eleven relevant studies were identified. On the basis of the reviewed research, it can be preliminary concluded that alexithymia might be associated with deficits Edited by: in the perception of primarily negative but also positive emotions in music and a Mathias Weymar, University of Potsdam, Germany reduced perception of aversive taste. The data available on olfaction and touch are Reviewed by: inconsistent or ambiguous and do not allow to draw conclusions. Future investigations Khatereh Borhani, would benefit from a multimethod assessment of alexithymia and control of negative Shahid Beheshti University, Iran Kristen Paula Morie, affect. Multimodal research seems necessary to advance our understanding of emotion Yale University, United States perception deficits in alexithymia and clarify the contribution of modality-specific and Jan Terock, supramodal processing impairments.
    [Show full text]
  • Connections Between Sensory Sensitivities in Autism
    PSU McNair Scholars Online Journal Volume 13 Issue 1 Underrepresented Content: Original Article 11 Contributions in Undergraduate Research 2019 Connections Between Sensory Sensitivities in Autism; the Importance of Sensory Friendly Environments for Accessibility and Increased Quality of Life for the Neurodivergent Autistic Minority. Heidi Morgan Portland State University Follow this and additional works at: https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/mcnair Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Morgan, Heidi (2019) "Connections Between Sensory Sensitivities in Autism; the Importance of Sensory Friendly Environments for Accessibility and Increased Quality of Life for the Neurodivergent Autistic Minority.," PSU McNair Scholars Online Journal: Vol. 13: Iss. 1, Article 11. https://doi.org/10.15760/mcnair.2019.13.1.11 This open access Article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial- ShareAlike 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0). All documents in PDXScholar should meet accessibility standards. If we can make this document more accessible to you, contact our team. Portland State University McNair Research Journal 2019 Connections Between Sensory Sensitivities in Autism; the Importance of Sensory Friendly Environments for Accessibility and Increased Quality of Life for the Neurodivergent Autistic Minority. by Heidi Morgan Faculty Mentor: Miranda Cunningham Citation: Morgan H. (2019). Connections between sensory sensitivities in autism; the importance of sensory friendly environments for accessibility and increased quality of life for the neurodivergent autistic minority. Portland State University McNair Scholars Online Journal, Vol. 0 Connections Between Sensory Sensitivities in Autism; the Importance of Sensory Friendly Environments for Accessibility and Increased Quality of Life for the Neurodivergent Autistic Minority.
    [Show full text]
  • Alexithymia, Empathy, and Autism
    Living Autism We find autism services, autism advice and autism support https://livingautism.com Alexithymia, Empathy, and Autism Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by impairment in (a) reciprocal social interaction and communication and (b) restricted and/or repetitive behaviors or interests. These delays or atypicality in social development, communication, neurocognition, and behavior vary in severity of symptoms, age of onset, and association with other disorders. However, it is deficits in social relatedness that are the major source of impairment and the core- defining feature of ASD, regardless of cognitive or language ability. This includes difficulties in communicating with others, processing and integrating emotional information, establishing and maintaining reciprocal social relationships, taking another person's perspective, and inferring the interests of others. An important aspect of social relatedness is the ability to empathize with the feelings of others. Empathy involves two major components: a cognitive component (e.g., theory of mind, perspective taking, or mindreading) and an affective component (emotional processing) which allows us to share the feelings of others. The affective component of sympathy involves having an appropriate emotional reaction to another person’s thoughts and feelings. When engaged in affective empathy, we vicariously experience the emotional states of others, understanding that our feelings are not ours but rather those of the other individual. Alexithymia, Empathy, and Autism While autism has been shown to be associated with a deficit in perspective taking (cognitive empathy), it is much less clear to what degree individuals with ASD also experience deficits in affective empathy. In fact, it is uncertain whether the empathy deficit commonly attributed to individuals with autism is a result of the disorder itself, or if it is a consequence of a comorbid (co- occurring) subclinical condition known as alexithymia.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Alexithymia in Reduced Eye- Fixation in Autism Spectrum Conditions
    The Role of Alexithymia in Reduced Eye- Fixation in Autism Spectrum Conditions Geoffrey Bird, Clare Press & Daniel C. Richardson Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders ISSN 0162-3257 Volume 41 Number 11 J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:1556-1564 DOI 10.1007/s10803-011-1183-3 1 23 Your article is protected by copyright and all rights are held exclusively by Springer Science+Business Media, LLC. This e-offprint is for personal use only and shall not be self- archived in electronic repositories. If you wish to self-archive your work, please use the accepted author’s version for posting to your own website or your institution’s repository. You may further deposit the accepted author’s version on a funder’s repository at a funder’s request, provided it is not made publicly available until 12 months after publication. 1 23 Author's personal copy J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:1556–1564 DOI 10.1007/s10803-011-1183-3 ORIGINAL PAPER The Role of Alexithymia in Reduced Eye-Fixation in Autism Spectrum Conditions Geoffrey Bird • Clare Press • Daniel C. Richardson Published online: 5 February 2011 Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011 Abstract Eye-tracking studies have demonstrated mixed People tend to immediately look at each others’ faces, (e.g. support for reduced eye fixation when looking at social Birmingham et al. 2008; van der Geest et al. 2002; Yarbus scenes in individuals with Autism Spectrum Conditions 1967), and in particular, their eyes (e.g. Pelphrey et al. 2002; (ASC). We present evidence that these mixed findings are Walkersmith et al.
    [Show full text]
  • UNIVERSITY of CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Social
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Social Justice and Autism: Links to Personality and Advocacy A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Education by Steven Kenneth Kapp 2016 © Copyright by Steven Kenneth Kapp 2016 ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION Social Justice and Autism: Links to Personality and Advocacy by Steven Kenneth Kapp Doctor of Philosophy in Education University of California, Los Angeles, 2016 Professor Connie L. Kasari, Chair Autism’s history as an independent condition may originate from “autistic psychopathy”, but autism and psychopathy may entail opposite patterns of personality. Autism may incline people toward moral intuitions in the dimensions of care, loyalty, authority, sanctity, and especially fairness. Yet these may play an unconscious and visceral role that in combination with difficulties with moral reasoning and the understanding of one’s own and others’ emotional and mental states, reduces self- and other awareness of autistic people’s moral drives. Conversely, psychopathic people may have low moral values (particularly for care and fairness), yet usually strong moral reasoning skills, cognitive empathy, and mentalizing abilities. This contrast adds to the literature in part through emphasizing basic sensory and motor differences, in transaction with the social environment and life experience, as underlying these personality-relevant ii distinctions between autism and psychopathy. It thus attempts to embody both conditions, with the understanding that all behavior involves motor activity, and to think of both conditions as neurodevelopmental in their origins and early trajectories. Such an analysis raises the importance of strengths, as a matter of individual differences as well as influences from the environment, that can help to distinguish and even cause the conditions.
    [Show full text]
  • Alexithymia Traits Outweigh Autism Traits in the Explanation Of
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Alexithymia traits outweigh autism traits in the explanation of depression in adults with autism Carola Bloch 1,2*, Lana Burghof1, Fritz‑Georg Lehnhardt1, Kai Vogeley1,3 & Christine Falter‑Wagner2,4 When contemplating the alarming depression rates in adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), there is a need to fnd factors explaining heightened symptoms of depression. Beyond the impact of autism traits, markedly increased levels of alexithymia traits should be considered as a candidate for explaining why individuals with ASD report higher levels of depressive symptoms. Here, we aim to identify the extent to which autism or alexithymia traits indicate depressive symptoms in ASD and whether the pattern of association are specifc to ASD. Data of a large (N = 400) representative clinical population of adults referred to autism diagnostics have been investigated and split by cases with a confrmed ASD diagnosis (N = 281) and cases with a ruled out ASD diagnosis (N = 119). Dominance analysis revealed the alexithymia factor, difculties in identifying feelings, as the strongest predictor for depressive symptomatology in ASD, outweighing autism traits and other alexithymia factors. This pattern of prediction was not specifc to ASD and was shared by clinical controls from the referral population with a ruled out ASD diagnosis. Thus, the association of alexithymia traits with depression is not unique to ASD and may constitute a general psychopathological mechanism in clinical samples. Autism traits and depression. Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized as a developmental disorder that entails pervasive peculiarities in communication and social interaction, as well as restricted and repetitive behavior, which exist as main diagnostic categories according to ICD-101.
    [Show full text]
  • Extreme Sensory Processing Patterns and Relation with Autistic And
    2ºCICLO DE ESTUDOS MESTRADO EM PSIQUIATRIA E SAÚDE MENTAL Extreme sensory processing patterns and relation with autistic and alexithymic traits among parents of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Sensory Processing Disorder – an exploratory study Ana Sofia Machado M 2020 DE FACULDADE MEDICINA Agradecimentos Finda esta jornada, apresento a minha gratidão a todos os profissionais da Unidade de Primeira Infância do Departamento de Psiquiatria de Infância e Adolescência em especial a Terapeuta Catarina BarBosa, pela partilha do conhecimento na área das perturBações do processamento sensorial, à Dr.ª Sara Rodrigues e Dr.ª Filipa Lopes pela colaBoração no recrutamento dos participantes e à minha orientadora de estágio, Dr.ª Goretti Dias, pela ajuda no desenho do estudo, facilitação do mesmo em todos os estadios e pela partilha do extenso conhecimento científico. Uma palavra de apreço ao colega Dr. José Manuel Borges, responsável pela tradução portuguesa da Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile, que facilitou todo o processo de autorização do uso da mesma. Agradeço tamBém à Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade do Porto pelo financiamento da licença de uso da escala sem a qual este estudo não seria possível. Assim como, ao Professor Doutor Rui Coelho, por, na qualidade de Diretor do Departamento de Psiquiatria e Saúde Mental, ter incentivado a proposta de financiamento para adquirir a licença de uso do instrumento assim como, por ouvir, considerar e podar as minhas ideias e projetos. Aos meus colegas, familiares (em especial, a minha irmã) e amigos que participaram no estudo como população controlo ou suporte emocional. Por fim, à responsável pela minha orientação nesta tese de mestrado, a Professora Irene Carvalho, pela imensa disponiBilidade, pela paciência nas dúvidas existenciais, por ter acreditado mesmo quando eu duvidava e pelo extenso conhecimento que me transmitiu.
    [Show full text]
  • Running Head: AUTISM and the READING the MIND in the EYES TEST 1
    Running head: AUTISM AND THE READING THE MIND IN THE EYES TEST 1 Submitted to Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 16th February 2016; revisions submitted 25th April 2016 and 17th May 2016; accepted 17th May 2016. ‘Theory of Mind’ is not Theory of Emotion: A cautionary note on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test Beth F. M. Oakley University of Cambridge Rebecca Brewer, Geoffrey Bird and Caroline Catmur King’s College London Author Note Beth F. M. Oakley, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, UK and School of Psychology, University of Surrey, UK. Rebecca Brewer, MRC Social, Genetic & Developmental Psychiatry Centre, King’s College London, UK and School of Psychology, University of East London, UK. Geoffrey Bird, MRC Social, Genetic & Developmental Psychiatry Centre, King’s College London, UK and Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, UK. Caroline Catmur, Department of Psychology, King’s College London, UK and School of Psychology, University of Surrey, UK. This research was supported by the Wellcome Trust [Biomedical Vacation Scholarship to CC supporting BFMO]. AUTISM AND THE READING THE MIND IN THE EYES TEST 2 Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Caroline Catmur, Department of Psychology, King’s College London, London SE1 1UL, UK, [email protected] AUTISM AND THE READING THE MIND IN THE EYES TEST 3 Abstract The ability to represent mental states (‘Theory of Mind’; ToM) is crucial in understanding individual differences in social ability, and social impairments evident in conditions such as Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test (RMET) is a popular measure of ToM ability, validated in part by the poor performance of those with ASD.
    [Show full text]
  • Interoception, Alexithymia and Empathy in Autism
    1 The feeling of me feeling for you: Interoception, alexithymia and empathy in autism. Short title: Interoception, alexithymia and empathy in autism. Cari-lène Mul – Anglia Ruskin University1 Steven D. Stagg – Anglia Ruskin University1 Bruno Herbelin – Center of Neuroprothetics, EPFL2 Jane E. Aspell – Anglia Ruskin University1 1Department of Psychology Faculty of Science and Technology Anglia Ruskin University Cambridge CB1 1PT United Kingdom 2 EPFL Center of Neuroprosthetics Campus Biotech Batiment H4 Chemin Des Mines 9 CH-1202 Geneva Switserland The authors declare no conflict of interest. 1 2 Abstract Following recent evidence for a link between interoception, emotion and empathy, we investigated relationships between these factors in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). 26 adults with ASD and 26 healthy participants completed tasks measuring interoception, alexithymia and empathy. ASD participants with alexithymia demonstrated lower cognitive and affective empathy than ASD participants without alexithymia. ASD participants showed reduced interoceptive sensitivity (IS), and also reduced interoceptive awareness (IA). IA was correlated with empathy and alexithymia, but IS was related to neither. Alexithymia fulfilled a mediating role between IA and empathy. Our findings are suggestive of an alexithymic subgroup in ASD, with distinct interoceptive processing abilities, and have implications for diagnosis and interventions. Keywords: autism, interoception, empathy, alexithymia 2 3 The feeling of me feeling for you: Interoception, alexithymia
    [Show full text]
  • Measuring Alexithymia in Autistic People
    Spectrum | Autism Research News https://www.spectrumnews.org OPINION, VIEWPOINT Measuring alexithymia in autistic people BY KATHERINE GOTHAM, ZACHARY J. WILLIAMS 24 AUGUST 2021 Listen to this story: People vary greatly in their ability to identify and describe the emotions they feel every day. While many people can easily tell when they are happy, sad, angry or frightened, others have great difficulty making sense of their emotional states or describing them to others. People with such challenges are said to have high levels of ‘alexithymia,’ a personality trait that literally translates to “no words for emotions.” In addition to problems identifying and describing feelings, people with high alexithymia prefer to focus their thoughts on things they can see or touch, rather than their own or others’ emotional experiences, a cognitive style that has been termed ‘externally oriented thinking.’ Many people on the autism spectrum are familiar with alexithymia, even if they haven’t heard the term. Studies estimate that nearly half of autistic adults exceed clinical cutoffs for ‘high alexithymia,’ compared with just 5 to 10 percent of the general population. Among autistic people, higher levels of alexithymia predict more significant social communication difficulties, as well as mental health challenges such as anxiety. Additionally, research suggests that certain characteristics commonly associated with autism (such as empathy differences and difficulty recognizing emotional facial expressions) may actually result from alexithymia rather than autism itself. Despite the growing interest in alexithymia in autism research, few studies have looked at whether the tools commonly used to measure this trait work reliably in autistic populations.
    [Show full text]
  • Alexithymia and Autism Spectrum Disorder: a Complex Relationship
    fpsyg-09-01196 July 13, 2018 Time: 16:9 # 1 PERSPECTIVE published: 17 July 2018 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01196 Alexithymia and Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Complex Relationship Jessie Poquérusse1,2, Luigi Pastore3, Sara Dellantonio1 and Gianluca Esposito1,4* 1 Department of Psychology and Cognitive Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy, 2 Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada, 3 Department of Education, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari, Bari, Italy, 4 Psychology Program, School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore Alexithymia is a personality construct characterized by altered emotional awareness which has been gaining diagnostic prevalence in a range of neuropsychiatric disorders, with notably high rates of overlap with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, the nature of its role in ASD symptomatology remains elusive. Here, we distill research at the intersection of alexithymia and ASD. After a brief synopsis of the studies that plaid a pioneering role in the identification of the overlapping fields between alexithymia and ASD, we comb the literature for evidence of its overlap with ASD in terms of prevalence, etiology, and behaviors. Through a formalized framework of the process of emotional interpretation and expression, we explore evidence for where and how deficits arise in Edited by: Lorys Castelli, this complex network of events. We portray how these relate to the dynamic interplay Università degli Studi di Torino, Italy between alexithymic and autistic traits and find emerging evidence that alexithymia is Reviewed by: both a cause and consequence of autistic behaviors. We end with a strategic proposal Indrajeet Patil, Harvard University, United States for future research and interventions to dampen the impacts of alexithymia in ASD.
    [Show full text]