<<

Anne Mari Steigen | Social support in nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems

Social support in nature-based services for Social support in nature-based young adults with mental health problems services for young adults with There is increasing interest in the potential for using nature and animals in services and interventions for people with mental health problems. However, the knowledge about nature-based services and participants is limited. The mental health problems overall aim of this thesis is to examine nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems in Norway, focusing on the meaning of different dimensions of social support.

The results indicate that social aspects are important in nature-based services. The young adults participating in the services exhibit more symptoms of mental health problems than a sample of young adults from the general population, Anne Mari Steigen but fewer than a sample of clinical mental health care in-patients. Furthermore, the results show that a variety of dimensions of social support is found in the services and that nature-based services add specific qualities to these dimensions, for instance, through support from animals. For participants struggling with mental health problems and social insecurity, emotional and esteem support, and the opportunity to provide care and support to others may be of particular importance in strengthening their self-esteem and social confidence. 2018:43

ISBN 978-91-7063-879-4 (Print)

ISBN 978-91-7063-974-6 (pdf) Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences

ISSN 1403-8099 Social Work

DOCTORAL THESIS | Karlstad University Studies | 2018:43 DOCTORAL THESIS | Karlstad University Studies | 2018:43 Social support in nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems

Anne Mari Steigen

DOCTORAL THESIS | Karlstad University Studies | 2018:43 Social support in nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems

Anne Mari Steigen

DOCTORAL THESIS

Karlstad University Studies | 2018:43 urn:nbn:se:kau:diva-69390

ISSN 1403-8099

ISBN 978-91-7063-879-4 (Print)

ISBN 978-91-7063-974-6 (pdf)

© The author

Distribution: Karlstad University Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences Department of Social and Psychological Studies SE-651 88 Karlstad, Sweden +46 54 700 10 00

Print: Universitetstryckeriet, Karlstad 2018

WWW.KAU.SE Abstract

Aim: The overall aim of this thesis is to examine nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems, focusing on the meaning of different dimensions of social support.

Methods: The thesis includes four studies that use a variety of data sources and methods: an integrative literature review of nature-based services in the Nordic countries; a quantitative cross-sectional survey of 93 young adults participating in nature-based services in Norway; and qualitative data comprising 20 interviews with nine participants in nature-based services. In addition, a sample of clinical mental health care in-patients and a sample of young adults from the general popula- tion are used for comparative analysis related to mental health prob- lems. Data are analysed using a variety of statistical analyses and qual- itative content analysis. Rasch analysis is employed to analyse the psy- chometric of the Social Provisions Scale (SPS-10).

Results: The majority of young adults in nature-based services strug- gles with mental health problems. The results indicate that they show more symptoms of mental health problems than the sample from the general population, but fewer than the sample of clinical mental health care in-patients. Previous research papers highlight the importance of social support in nature-based services. However, knowledge about so- cial support in these services is limited. Rasch analysis of the SPS-10 supports the division of social support into functional and structural support and the SPS-10 is revised for use in the nature-based sample. Participants receive emotional, esteem, informational and instrumen- tal support in the services, and experience social integration and op- portunities for nurturance. The results also indicate that nature-based services add specific qualities to the dimensions of social support, for instance, through support from animals.

Conclusions: Social support is important in nature-based services, and various dimensions of social support are provided in the services. Emotional and esteem support, along with opportunities for nurtur- ance, may be of particular importance for participants with mental health problems. 1

Sammendrag

Sosial støtte i naturbaserte tjenester for unge voksne med psykiske problemer

Bakgrunn: Interessen for å bruke natur og dyr innen helsefrem- mende arbeid, som et pedagogisk verktøy (i f.eks. skoler og barneha- ger), i sosialt arbeid og i rehabilitering og behandling, har økt de siste 20 årene. Det finnes naturbaserte tjenester over hele verden, for en rekke ulike brukergrupper. Dette inkluderer blant annet tilbud til barn og ungdom, mennesker med psykiske eller rusrelaterte utfordringer og personer med demens. Aktivitetene som tilbys varierer i forhold til hva som er hensikten med tilbudet og brukergruppene. En overordnet mål- setning for alle de naturbaserte tiltakene er å bidra til helsefremming og mestring blant deltagere som av ulike grunner trenger støtte til dette. Tidligere forskning viser at naturbaserte tjenester bidrar til gode resultater for deltagerne, knyttet til bedret psykisk helse, mestrings- opplevelser relatert til aktivitetene på tiltaket, og økt sosial kompe- tanse.

En av gruppene som har tilbud om naturbaserte tjenester i Norge er unge voksne med psykiske eller rusrelaterte problemer. Det er en økende bekymring for den psykiske helsen til unge voksne i Norge i dag og det kan se ut til at flere unge voksne har psykiske problemer i dag, enn for noen tiår siden. Samtidig ser det ut til at de psykiske proble- mene får større konsekvenser i dag enn for 10 år siden. Det er en stor økning av unge som mottar uføretrygd, de fleste på grunn av psykiske problemer. 70 % av unge voksne som mottar arbeidsavklaringspenger har psykiske problemer. Unge voksne som faller utenfor skole og ar- beidsliv på grunn av psykiske problemer er derfor en stor folkehelseut- fordring i Norge.

God sosial støtte har vist seg å være viktig for den psykiske helsen, og forskning viser at jo bedre sosial støtte et menneske opplever å ha, de- sto færre symptomer på psykiske problemer. Det sosial miljøet er gjen- nom tidligere forskning funnet å ha en stor betydning i de naturbaserte tjenestene. Tidligere forskning har derimot ikke hatt spesielt fokus på å studere sosial støtte i disse tiltakene. 2

Hensikt: Avhandlingens overordnede hensikt er å undersøke natur- baserte tjenester for unge voksne med psykiske problemer, med fokus på betydningen av ulike dimensjoner av sosial støtte.

Metode: Denne avhandlingen består av fire studier som anvender ulike datakilder og metoder. Studie I er et integrativt review basert på litteratur som omhandler na- turbaserte tjenester for mennesker med psykiske eller rusrelaterte pro- blemer, eller som er utenfor skole og arbeidsliv, i de Nordiske landene. Studie II bruker data fra en kvantitativ tverrsnittsundersøkelse av 93 unge voksne (16-30 år) som er deltagere i ulike naturbaserte tjenester i Norge. Studien ser på psykiske problemer blant deltagerne. I tillegg er data fra et utvalg innlagte pasienter ved en psykiatrisk avdeling i Norge (18-30 år), samt data fra et utvalg unge voksne (18-19 år) fra en generell populasjon i Norge, brukt for å gjøre komparative analyser re- latert til psykiske problemer. Studie III anvender Rasch-analyse for å undersøke de psykometriske egenskapene til Social Provisions Scale (SPS-10), som var en del av spørreskjemaet som ble brukt i tverrsnittsundersøkelsen beskrevet over. Studie IV ser på betydningen av ulike dimensjoner av sosial støtte i na- turbaserte tjenester og bruker data fra tversnittundersøkelsen, samt kvalitative data som består av 20 intervjuer med 9 ulike deltagere (19- 26 år) i naturbaserte tjenester i Norge. Data i de fire studiene er analysert ved hjelp av ulike statistiske analyser og kvalitativ innholdsanalyse.

Hovedresultater: De fleste unge voksne i naturbaserte tjenester som er deltagere i tverrsnittsundersøkelsen sliter med psykiske problemer. Resultatene indikerer at de viser flere symptomer på psykiske proble- mer enn utvalget 18-19- åringer fra den generelle befolkningen, men færre symptomer enn utvalget av innlagte pasienter ved en psykiatrisk avdeling. Tidligere forskning legger vekt på viktigheten av sosial støtte i naturbaserte tjenester. Imidlertid er kunnskap om innholdet i den so- siale støtten i disse tjenestene begrenset. Rasch-analysen av SPS-10

3

støtter tidligere vurderinger rundt at det er riktig å skille mellom funk- sjonell og strukturell sosial støtte. SPS-10 er revidert til en 8-items ver- sjon (SPS-8) som måler kun funksjonell sosial støtte, og som er brukt i analysene. Resultatene viser at deltakere opplever følelsesmessig, verd- settende, informativ og instrumentell støtte (funksjonell støtte) i de na- turbaserte tjenestene. De opplever også sosial integrasjon og mulighe- ter for å gi støtte og omsorg. Resultatene indikerer videre at naturba- serte tjenester synliggjør varierte kvaliteter i dimensjonene av sosial støtte, blant annet kommer det fram at deltagerne også opplever sosial støtte fra dyr.

Konklusjoner: Sosial støtte er viktig i naturbaserte tjenester, og det finnes ulike dimensjoner av sosial støtte i tjenestene. Emosjonell og verdsettende støtte, sammen med mulighetene til å gi støtte og omsorg, er tydelig til stede og ser ut til å være spesielt viktig for deltakere med psykiske problemer.

4 Contents

LIST OF PAPERS ...... 8

INTRODUCTION ...... 10

OVERALL AND SPECIFIC AIMS ...... 14 NATURE-BASED SERVICES AND MENTAL HEALTH PROBLEMS AMONG YOUNG ADULTS IN NORWAY: BACKGROUND AND CONCEPTUAL CLARIFICATIONS ...... 15

NATURE-BASED SERVICES: CONCEPT CLARIFICATIONS ...... 15 NATURE-BASED SERVICES IN NORWAY ...... 19 Extent, organisation, and characteristics of nature-based services in Norway ...... 19 Nature-based services for young adults with mental health- or drug- related problems in Norway ...... 20 THE ROLE OF SOCIAL WORKERS IN NATURE-BASED SERVICES IN NORWAY ...... 21 MENTAL HEALTH PROBLEMS AMONG YOUNG ADULTS – CONCEPTUAL DEFINITIONS AND BACKGROUND ...... 22 Who are the young adults? ...... 22 Mental health problems among young adults ...... 23 What are mental health problems?...... 24 Mental health problems in this thesis ...... 26 NATURE-BASED SERVICES: BACKGROUND AND PREVIOUS RESEARCH ...... 28

A HISTORICAL VIEW OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN NATURE, ANIMALS, AND HUMANS ...... 28 THEORIES ON THE INFLUENCE OF NATURE ON HUMAN WELL-BEING AND HEALTH ...... 29 RESEARCH FIELDS OF RELEVANCE FOR NATURE-BASED SERVICES ...... 30 What do we know about the relation between humans and nature? .. 30 What do we know about the interaction between humans and animals? ...... 32 RESEARCH ON NATURE-BASED SERVICES FOR PEOPLE WITH MENTAL HEALTH PROBLEMS IN FARM MILIEUS ...... 34 Intervention studies in farm milieus for people with mental health problems ...... 36 Research on nature-based services for people with mental health problems ...... 37 Nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems 39 5 SUMMING UP – PREVIOUS RESEARCH ON NATURE-BASED SERVICES IN FARM MILIEUS ...... 40 SALUTOGENESIS AND SOCIAL SUPPORT: THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK AND PREVIOUS RESEARCH ...... 41

SALUTOGENESIS ...... 41 The salutogenic model ...... 42 Sense of coherence ...... 43 Generalised resistance resources ...... 44 SOC and mental health ...... 45 SOC and social support ...... 46 Salutogenesis and nature-based services ...... 47 Critique of salutogenesis ...... 48 SOCIAL SUPPORT ...... 50 Social support and mental health in young adults ...... 52 Social support in nature-based services ...... 53

METHODS ...... 56

METHODS FOR PAPER I ...... 58 Inclusion criteria ...... 58 Literature search ...... 58 Analysis ...... 59 METHODS FOR PAPER II ...... 60 Population and data collection – the nature-based services survey ... 60 The questionnaire in the nature-based services survey ...... 63 Description of the clinical sample ...... 66 Description of the general population sample ...... 66 Analysis of data ...... 67 METHODS FOR PAPER III ...... 67 Population and data collection ...... 67 Analysis of data ...... 67 METHODS FOR PAPER IV ...... 69 Population and data collection ...... 69 Analysis ...... 70 VALIDITY, RELIABILITY, AND GENERALISABILITY ...... 70 ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS ...... 72

RESULTS ...... 74

RESULTS FOR PAPER I ...... 74 RESULTS FOR PAPER II ...... 74 RESULTS FOR PAPER III ...... 76

6 RESULTS FOR PAPER IV ...... 76

DISCUSSION ...... 78

NATURE-BASED SERVICES FOR YOUNG ADULTS WITH MENTAL HEALTH PROBLEMS ...... 79 Previous literature and research papers on nature-based services (focusing on the meaning of social support) ...... 79 Mental health problems among young adults in nature-based services in Norway ...... 81 THE MEANING OF DIMENSIONS OF SOCIAL SUPPORT IN NATURE-BASED SERVICES FOR YOUNG ADULTS WITH MENTAL HEALTH PROBLEMS ...... 83 The concept social support and its dimensions – Conclusions from the psychometric analyses ...... 83 Meaning of dimensions of social support in nature-based services ... 85 METHODOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS ...... 93 Methodological considerations related to data collection for the survey ...... 93 Methodological considerations related to sample size in the survey .. 95 Methodological considerations related to analysis of the survey data 96 Generalisability of the findings from the survey ...... 96 The psychometric properties for the standardized instruments used in the survey ...... 96 Credibility, dependability and transferability for the qualitative data ... 98 Methodological considerations for the literature review ...... 98

CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS ...... 100

FUTURE RESEARCH ...... 101

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ...... 102

REFERENCES ...... 104

APPENDICES

7 List of papers

This thesis is based on the following papers, referred to in the text by their Roman numerals:

I Steigen, A.M., Kogstad, R. & Hummelvoll, J.K. (2016) Green Care services in the Nordic countries: An integrative literature review. European Journal of Social Work, 19:5, 692–715 DOI: 10.1080/13691457.2015.1082983

II Steigen, A.M., Eriksson, B., Kogstad, R.E., Toft, H.P. & Bergh, D. Young adults in nature-based services in Norway – in-group and be- tween-group variations related to mental health problems. (Ac- cepted for publication in Nordic Journal of Social Research)

III Steigen, A.M. & Bergh, D. (2018) The Social Provisions Scale: Psy- chometric properties of the SPS-10 among participants in nature- based services. Disability and Rehabilitation, 1-9 DOI: 10.1080/09638288.2018.1434689

IV Steigen, A.M., Eriksson, B., Kogstad, R.E. & Bergh, D. The meaning of social support in nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems. (Manuscript submitted for publication)

Reprints were made with the publishers’ permission.

8 My contributions to the papers included in this thesis are as follows:

Paper I: The co-authors and I planned the study. I carried out the literature searches. Together with the co-authors, I read and analysed the rele- vant literature. I was responsible for writing the paper, with the co-au- thors reviewing and commenting on the manuscript in the course of the process.

Paper II: The co-authors and I planned and undertook data collection for the na- ture-based survey. Together with one of the co-authors, I conducted the analysis of the three data sets. I was responsible for writing the main body of the paper, with the co-authors reviewing and commenting on the manuscript in the course of the process.

Paper III: The co-author and I analysed the data, and I was responsible for writ- ing the paper in close collaboration with the co-author, who reviewed and commented on the manuscript in the course of the process.

Paper IV: The co-authors and I planned and undertook data collection for the survey, and then analysed the quantitative and qualitative data. I was responsible for writing the main body of the paper, with the co-authors reviewing and commenting on the manuscript in the course of the pro- cess.

9 Introduction

Interest in using nature and animals in health promotion, for pedagog- ical purposes (e.g. in kindergartens or schools), social work services, and rehabilitation and treatment interventions, has increased in the last 20 years. Nature-based services for a range of different groups of people exist worldwide (Artz & Davis, 2017; Berget & Braastad, 2008; Elings & Hassink, 2008; Mallon, 1994; Rappe, Koivunen, & Korpela, 2008; Sahlin, Matuszczyk, Ahlborg, & Grahn, 2012; Sempik, Rickhuss, & Beeston, 2014; Toyoda, 2013). The groups targeted for nature-based services include children and youth, people with mental health- or drug-related difficulties and individuals with dementia. The activities offered in these services vary depending on the purpose of the service and the group participating (Hine, Peacock, & Pretty, 2008; Sempik, Hine, & Wilcox, 2010). Nevertheless, one overall goal of all nature- based services is to promote health and well-being for people who need support (Hassink & Van Dijk, 2006). The principal idea is to promote participants’ experiences of meaningful activities in a health-promot- ing environment. These activities include practical experiences, such as gardening, taking care of animals, and maintenance work. These activ- ities are considered to be meaningful for the participants, and they should be able to take part regardless of their day-to-day condition (Haugan, Nyland, Fjeldavli, Meistad, & Braastad, 2006). Research on nature-based services generally shows promising results as these con- cern improved mental health, experiences of mastery related to activi- ties at the services, and increased social competence (Bragg & Atkins, 2016; Elings & Hassink, 2008; Ellingsen-Dalskau, Berget, Pedersen, Tellnes, & Ihlebæk, 2016; Ellingsen-Dalskau, Morken, Berget, & Pedersen, 2016; Granerud & Eriksson, 2014; Hassink, Elings, Zweekhorst, Van den Nieuwenhuizen, & Smit, 2010; Kogstad, Agdal, & Hopfenbeck, 2014; Mallon, 1994).

One of the groups that is offered nature-based services is young adults with mental health- or drug-related problems. The World Health Or- ganization (WHO) indicates that depression is the third leading cause of illness and disability among adolescents (WHO, 2018). In Norway, mental health problems are widespread (Reneflot et al., 2018), and there is growing concern about young adults’ mental health and the

10 consequences of this for their ability to participate in the society (Nes & Clench-Aas, 2011). The number of young adults receiving disability pensions and work-assessment allowances has increased in Norway in recent years – most of this increase is related to mental health prob- lems. For example, the number of young adults receiving disability pensions doubled between 2009 and 2018. From 7,657 individuals in 2009 to 16,897 in 2018, with most of the increase associated with those with mental health- or behaviour-related problems (Guldvåg, 2017; NAV, 2018c). Furthermore, work assessment allowances given for the medical reason of mental health problems also increased during that time (NAV, 2018b). In August 2018 almost 70% of the 26,260 individ- uals under age 30 who received work-assessment allowances had men- tal health problems (NAV, 2018a). In addition, individuals with mental health problems receive social insurance for a longer period than do other groups receiving social insurance (Brage & Bragstad, 2011). Var- ious explanations have been suggested for this trend, some of which are related to a labour market that is more difficult to enter, with higher demands for social and communicative competencies, which can be difficult for individuals who struggle with mental health problems. There has also been speculation about whether this increase in social insurance associated with mental health problems reflects a general de- terioration in young adults’ mental health, or whether the increase is related to how people in society view and treat mental health problems (Hyggen & Hammer, 2013).

Previous research on nature-based services for people with mental health problems includes intervention studies focusing on various spe- cific aspects of nature-based services. For instance, the effects of con- tact with animals in a farm setting have been the focus of several stud- ies (Berget, Ekeberg, & Braastad, 2008; Berget, Ekeberg, Pedersen, & Braastad, 2011; Pedersen, Martinsen, Berget, & Braastad, 2012; Pedersen, Nordaunet, Martinsen, Berget, & Braastad, 2011). Research has also focused on nature-based services that exist independently of research interventions for people with mental health problems (Elings & Hassink, 2008; Granerud & Eriksson, 2014; Hassink et al., 2010; Kogstad et al., 2014; Leck, Upton, & Evans, 2015). In addition, previous research has focused on people in prevocational training on care farms (Ellingsen-Dalskau, Berget, et al., 2016; Ellingsen-Dalskau, Morken, et 11

al., 2016), the majority of whom struggle with mental health problems. However, there is limited research on nature-based services that exist independently of research interventions, both in Norway and interna- tionally. Hence, knowledge of these nature-based services and their participants is limited. In previous research, hypotheses about why na- ture-based services are beneficial for humans are related to theories of social support, biophilia, self-determination, and salutogenesis (Cutcliffe & Travale, 2016; Grahn, Ivarsson, Stigsdotter, & Bengtsson, 2010; Granerud & Eriksson, 2014; Sempik et al., 2010). Some of these perspectives are described later in this introductory chapter.

Social support, as a core component of nature-based services, is high- lighted in several studies, and is linked to the service leader, the other participants, and the animals as possible providers of different dimen- sions of support (Elings & Hassink, 2008; Hassink et al., 2010; Pedersen, Ihlebæk, & Kirkevold, 2012). However, limited research has focused specifically on social support in nature-based services, differ- ent dimensions of social support, and what social support in nature- based services means. For instance, how does social support feature in the services, what types of social support are found there, by whom, and how is it provided?

Previous research indicates that unemployed individuals with higher degrees of social support experience better mental health than those with less social support (Axelsson & Ejlertsson, 2002; McKee-Ryan, Song, Wanberg, & Kinicki, 2005; Milner, Krnjacki, Butterworth, & LaMontagne, 2016). Moreover, social support has been found to have both direct and buffer effects on the mental health of the unemployed (Milner et al., 2016). Social support may also have therapeutic qualities for people with mental health problems (Kogstad, Mønness, & Sørensen, 2013). Positive effects of social support on the mental health in young adults (under the age of 20) has been found in previous stud- ies (Rueger, Malecki, Pyun, Aycock, & Coyle, 2016; Wang, Davis, Wootton, Mottershaw, & Haworth, 2017).

Research problems and needs Social support is potentially an important recovery factor for young adults with mental health- and drug-related problems. Therefore, more 12

knowledge about social support in nature-based services for young adults with mental health- or drug-related problems is required in or- der to evaluate the potential and actual contribution of these services for this group. In addition, overall, there is limited knowledge about nature-based services for young adults with mental health- and drug- related problems, both internationally and in Norway. As research on nature-based services has only recently been initiated, knowledge of previous research and interventions, as well as of participants’ mental health, is scarce. However, such knowledge is crucial for understanding the role of social support in these services.

13

Overall and specific aims

The overall aim of this thesis is to examine nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems, focusing on the meaning of different dimensions of social support.

The specific aims of the studies included in this thesis are:

Study I: To present an overview of the literature on Green Care (nature- based services) in the Nordic countries in relation to people out of work or school, or with mental or drug-related problems, or both.

Study II: To examine mental health problems among young adults par- ticipating in nature-based services in Norway.

Study III: To examine the psychometric properties of the total SPS 10- item version (SPS-10) by means of the polytomous Rasch model, ap- plied to Norwegian data on young adults at risk of marginalisation par- ticipating in nature-based services.

Study IV: To uncover dimensions of social support in relation to sense of coherence (SOC) and mental health among young adults with mental health problems, participating in nature-based services.

14

Nature-based services and mental health problems among young adults in Norway: background and con- ceptual clarifications

In this chapter, the concept of nature-based services is defined and motivation for the use of the concept is provided. Furthermore, the or- ganisation and extent of nature-based services in Norway is presented. In addition, nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems are described in further detail. The role of social workers in nature-based services in Norway is described. Thereafter, the concepts of mental health problems and young adults are defined, and a back- ground to mental health problems among young adults in Norway is presented.

Nature-based services: concept clarifications The concept of nature-based services includes a range of sub-concepts and definitions, some of the most central of which are Green Care, care farming, nature-assisted therapy, social and therapeutic horticulture, ecotherapy, and animal-assisted therapy. Many of these concepts are defined differently in various countries and research traditions. I focus on some of the principal concepts and their definitions, and discuss my choice of the concept used in this thesis. There is an ongoing debate on the use of such concepts in this field, and it has been argued that it would be beneficial to utilise the same system of concepts and defini- tions (Bragg & Atkins, 2016).

Green Care is one of the most frequently used terms to describe services that employ nature, plants, and animals in activities that promote the health and well-being of a range of people. Green Care is the term used by the COST Action 866 (Green Care in agriculture) group, which com- prises researchers from several countries (Sempik et al., 2010). The group broadly defines Green Care as a way to ‘use nature to produce health, social or educational benefits’ (Sempik et al., 2010, p. 27). The group also uses the Green Care concept as an umbrella term that in- cludes social and therapeutic horticulture, animal-assisted interven- tions, care farming, facilitated green-treatment exercises, ecotherapy, wilderness therapy, and nature therapy. Sempik et al. (2010) also pro- vide a rather broad definition of the concept of care. They describe care 15

as including elements of health care, social rehabilitation, education, and work possibilities for various vulnerable groups. The word ‘care’ in Green Care thus not only refers to ‘care’ in the medical sense, but also health promotion and social rehabilitation (Haubenhofer, Elings, Has- sink, & Hine, 2010).

This need to clearly state the meaning of Green Care may have its roots in the ambiguity and asymmetry associated with the concept of care (Aamodt, 2014; H. G. Eriksson, 2003). Care is a relational concept that refers to deeds combined with special qualities that are exchanged be- tween people. There is a giver and a receiver, and care is situated be- tween the two (Aamodt, 2014). Care has both positive and negative connotations. Providing care is viewed as positive, and it can feel good to provide care to someone, making care-givers feel valued (Mayeroff, 1990). However, receiving care is difficult for some people. Depending on the care of others can skew the power distribution in a relationship. Nevertheless, all people are in need of care at some point in lives, and people in vulnerable positions may have a specific need for care.

Returning to the concept of Green Care, Sempik and Bragg (2013, p. 14) conclude that ‘Green Care utilises plants, animals and landscapes to create interventions to improve health and wellbeing (i.e., it does not represent a casual encounter with nature)’. Bragg and Atkins (2016, p. p. 18) further state that ‘Green Care is used to describe a variety of na- ture-based treatment interventions for individuals with a defined med- ical or social need, rather than nature-based health-promotion activi- ties for the general population’. Therefore, they refine their definition to read: ‘Green Care: Nature-based therapy or treatment interventions – specifically designed, structured and facilitated for individuals with a defined need’ (Bragg & Atkins, 2016, p. 18). This definition of Green Care is in line with my understanding of it: it serves as a broad concep- tion covering a range of nature-based activities. However, other defini- tions of Green Care also exist. For instance, Gallis (2013, p. 3) defines it as ‘the use of agricultural farms and the biotic and abiotic elements of nature for health and therapy-promoting interventions as a base for promoting human mental and physical health, as well as quality of life’. What makes this definition different from those above is its mention of

16

the use of agricultural farms – it excludes nature-based interventions that are not associated with an agricultural farm.

In the national strategy for Green Care in Norway, it is defined as:

individually adapted and quality-assured welfare services on farms. The services shall stimulate coping skills, development and well-being. Farms are properties that are used for farming, forestry and gardening. The activ- ities offered in the Green Care service relate to the farm and its daily life and operations. (Norwegian ministry of agriculture and food & Norwegian ministry of local government and regional development, 2012, p. 7)

This association with farms in the definitions leads us to another na- ture-based concept, namely care farming, which is described as the therapeutic use of farming practices to provide health, social, or educa- tional services to vulnerable groups (Care farming UK, 2017). Hau- benhofer et al. (2010) describe care farming as the therapeutic use of agricultural landscapes and farming practices. However, what consti- tutes a care farm differs among countries. The main difference is that, in some countries, care farms must be productive agricultural farms, while in others, they are part of a health care institution (Haubenhofer et al., 2010). Care farming is today a commonly used concept in differ- ent countries, with nature-based services taking place at farms (Ellingsen-Dalskau, 2017; Gorman & Cacciatore, 2017; Hassink, De Bruin, Berget, & Elings, 2017).

Nonetheless, neither Green Care nor care farming is adequate for use in this thesis. One problem with both concepts has already been illus- trated, that is, the concept of ‘care’ itself. Another problem is the incon- sistent use of the definitions, and in some cases, the premise that the services must be linked to a productive farm. With regard to Green Care, the term ‘green’ also raises some issues. The word ‘green’ is eve- rywhere these days, and it is used to describe a range of phenomena related to energy, climate, and politics. Moreover, the term ‘Green Care’ could be confusing for someone without any knowledge of the field (Sempik & Bragg, 2013). Thus, does the term refer to taking care of ‘green’ entities (in an environmental manner), or does it concern pol-

17 lution? There is a need for additional descriptive elements, for exam- ple, nature-based, health-promoting activities for a range of different groups (Sempik & Bragg, 2013).

In Norway, the Into the farmyard concept also is used. This is a brand that quality-checked and approved farms can use, and it carries the same definition found in the National Strategy for Green Care in Nor- way (Norwegian ministry of agriculture and food & Norwegian ministry of local government and regional development, 2012).

I have chosen to use the concept of nature-based interventions’ and ‘nature-based services’ as overall concepts in my thesis. I think that these are broad and do not exclude any of the services from which I have recruited informants, nor services with similar target groups and objectives. These are also concepts that can fit well with social work, with nature-based welfare interventions being one possible aspect of these concepts. Nevertheless, in my studies, I have examined services that would fit within both a broad Green Care definition or a broad care-farming definition. By using the term nature-based, I do not need to consider the term care or the narrow perception of what constitutes a farm. I have chosen to focus on what we are interested in, rather than on the names of these places. Therefore, my definition, based on Bragg and Atkins (2016), is as follows: nature-based services or interventions, often, but not necessarily, use a farm milieu with specifically designed, structured, and facilitated services for individuals with defined needs. An individual’s ‘need’ is in my empirical material defined by public ser- vices such as mental health services or schools, and, in this thesis, the ‘need’ is associated with mental health- or drug-related problems. In my empirical studies, these nature-based services are situated in farm contexts. The farms are not required to be active farming entities, only part of a farm milieu. The principal idea is that there is the potential for interaction with nature, including with plants, animals, and forests, along with the possibility of undertaking many different farm activities, such as taking care of animals, woodworking, carpeting, and cooking. This consciousness of these different conceptions developed in the course of my PhD studies as I developed more knowledge of these ser- vices. Therefore, Green Care is used as the overall concept in my first

18

paper, whereas in my more recent papers, and in this thesis summary, I use nature-based services as an overall concept.

Nature-based services in Norway Norway is one of the most advanced countries when it comes to nature- based services, and it contributes a considerable amount of research in the field. In what follows, the extent, organisation, and characteristics of nature-based services in Norway is presented, followed by a more specific presentation of nature-based services for young adults with mental health- or drug-related problems.

Extent, organisation, and characteristics of nature-based services in Norway In 2012, a national approval system for nature-based services in Nor- wegian farm milieus was introduced, and 403 services are currently (2018) registered in Norway (Matmerk, 2018). In addition, nature- based services are found outside of farm milieus, for example, in the form of wilderness therapy, animal-assisted therapy, and other types of nature-based services that exist independently of farms. However, there is no reliable overview available of these nature-based services and their participants, which makes research in this field challenging. Thus, it is uncertain how many people are participating in nature-based services in Norway at any one time. The focus in this thesis is nature- based services in farm milieus.

The development of nature-based services in Norwegian farm milieus has been driven by the agricultural sector and was later advanced by politicians and bureaucrats in the same field (Vik & Farstad, 2009). Nevertheless, nature-based services have had problems establishing roots in the health sector and in social work. It has been argued that there is a need for more knowledge and research on the effects of such services. At the same time, it has been pointed out that efforts need to be made to facilitate better communication and interaction between the different actors in the field, such as the farmers, users, and buyers of such services (Vik & Farstad, 2009). It has been emphasised that many discursive tensions exist between these different actors. Some main areas of tension include different understandings of the concept

19

of quality, of professionalism in these services, and of the need for sta- ble institutional frameworks for nature-based services (Hummelvoll, Hopfenbeck, & Kogstad, 2012).

Nature-based services for young adults with mental health- or drug-related problems in Norway In Norway, nature-based services for young adults with mental health- or drug-related problems are offered by a range of providers, including the Norwegian Labour and Welfare Administration (NAV)1, schools, and mental-health services. NAV uses the green work concept in its services, while Green Care, care farms, and Into the farmyard often are used by schools and mental health services. However, the nature-based services function in nearly the same manner in all cases, regardless of who recruits the participants. Some services also draw participants from more than one provider. NAV (2011) describes green work as a labour source for people with mental health- or drug-related problems. The services are described as providing work-related physical activities involving contact with nature and animals in a social community on a farm. Experience shows that this is an effective service in terms of strengthening work ability, improving health, and increasing quality of life (NAV, 2011). These three factors comprise the main purpose of these services, which are categorised primarily as ‘work practice in shielded services’ (APS) and exceptional ‘permanent organised work’ (VTA). They need to be situated on a multi-functional farm that affords possibilities for a range of activities that may include caring for ani- mals, working in the forest, working with firewood or plants, using and maintaining machinery, performing maintenance indoors and out-

1 ‘NAV administers a third of the Norwegian national budget through schemes such as un- employment benefit, work assessment allowance, sickness benefit, pensions, child benefit and cash-for-care benefit. The local authorities and central government cooperate to find good solutions for users through 456 NAV offices in municipalities and city boroughs. Each local authority and NAV agree on what local authority services their office should provide. The services provided by a NAV office will thus vary from local authority to local authority. In addition to the local NAV offices, there are more than one hundred special units. The special units perform centralised duties that it would not be appropriate for front line local NAV of- fices to perform’ (NAV, 2017).

20

doors, cooking, physical activity, and other activities related to a par- ticular season. Common meals are viewed as an important social rally- ing point (NAV, 2011). Many opportunities are provided for individual adaptation, and the frequency with which participants visit the farm varies. The nature-based services offered through providers other than NAV for similar target groups do not vary much in terms of the services they offer. However, the main purpose of participation in the services may vary depending on the referring agency. For example, NAV may focus on work rehabilitation, thereby making a return to work an out- come goal, whereas referral sources, such as mental-health agencies, may target improved mental health, well-being, and life satisfaction as outcome goals. Schools using nature-based services may set a goal ac- cording to which participants return to regular schooling or complete their education in other ways.

The role of social workers in nature-based services in Norway Several professions are responsible for running and providing nature- based services, including farmers, nurses, teachers, and social workers. This thesis utilises social work as a frame of reference for research on nature-based services, although the field also extends to other frames such as psychology, health and social care, and pedagogy. Social work is grounded in a humanistic perspective on human life and recognises the potentially positive, unique, social, and changeable aspects of peo- ple. Social work also recognises the importance of significant others in people’s lives and acknowledges that people are influenced by both ma- terial and social conditions (Hansen & Solem, 2017). The main purpose of social work is to help people overcome their social problems, and social workers function at individual, group, and societal levels. Hence, social workers practise in many different places within a variety of frames. Examples of workplaces for social workers in Norway include NAV, child welfare, mental health services, and rehabilitation (Hansen & Solem, 2017). In addition, many social workers in Norway are em- ployed in public administration, and thus work within a bureaucratic frame. Bureaucracy tends to depersonalise cases, in contrast to certain academic and professional values in social work, such as empathy and participation in one’s own problem-solving (Lingås, 2003; Solstad, 2017). There is a need for more realms in which people can be cared for as individuals. 21

Social workers may practise within nature-based services, or they may refer clients to these services while working in NAV, local mental health service agencies, schools, or child welfare services. Nature-based ser- vices may be a resource that provides social workers with an oppor- tunity to practise their profession in line with its basic values. Psycho- social social work emphasises that individuals not should be seen as objects, but understood in relation to their life circumstances and social situation. Indeed, many of the practices related to nature-based ser- vices are in line with the values of psychosocial social work. In this con- text, it is important to integrate a person’s own understanding of her story, life situation and resources with acceptance of the person and the centrality of her experiences (Nissen & Skærbæk, 2014).

Mental health problems among young adults – conceptual defini- tions and background This thesis focuses on young adults with mental health problems who participate in nature-based services in Norway. A definition and back- ground of this population is therefore needed, because various defini- tions and descriptions of the population exist. First, the term young adults is described before a background to mental health problems fo- cusing on young adults with mental health problems in the Norwegian context is provided. The concept of mental health problems is de- scribed and defined and the manner in which the concept is used in this thesis is outlined.

Who are the young adults? The term young adult does not have a clear definition; however, it de- scribes the life phase between youth and adulthood, a phase character- ised by many possibilities, limitations, and challenges. While physical and psychological changes characterise youth, social changes are the most important during the young-adult phase (Hammer & Hyggen, 2013). The term young adult is not delimited by clear age parameters, and the phase may vary among individuals. It is the transitions that characterise this period that are central. Some youths move out of their parents’ homes when they start upper secondary school at the age of 16. Some finish school and start working early in their 20s, while others continue as students until they are nearly 30 years old (Hammer & 22

Hyggen, 2013). Thus, individuals aged 16 to 30 fit the young adult de- mographic, which is the one referred to when young adults are dis- cussed in this thesis.

This transition phase between youth and adulthood has undergone substantial changes in the past 50 years, both in Norway and in other industrialised countries (Arnett, 2004; Fauske, 1996). Hence, in 1961, the average age of first-time mothers in Norway was 24.6 years, while in 2017, the average age was 29.3 years old. For first-time fathers, the respective ages were 28.2 and 31.7 years (Statistics Norway, 2018b). In the period 1961-1965, 90.2% of women and 80.4% of men in Norway were married before they were 30 years old. In 2017, the average age for first-time marriages for women was 32.3 years, and for men, 34.9 years (Statistics Norway, 2018a). In the 1960s, the young-adult phase was therefore relatively short, and many individuals’ were relatively young when they established the path for what they would do in their lives. While society previously had clear expectations about the choices that young adults should make, today this is characterised to a much greater extent by individualisation and freedom (Arnett, 2004; Ziehe, 2012). However, this freedom can be difficult for many young adults, as it presents them with more individual choices and, thus, more re- sponsibilities (Frønes & Strømme, 2014). Freedom of choice also de- mands the competence to make wise choices. Adulthood is no longer based simply on age, but rather on status built on achievements in many areas, such as getting a job, moving away from home, becoming a parent, and establishing stable relationships (Settersten, Furstenberg, & Rumbaut, 2005). In research in the U.S., the term emerging adulthood is used to describe this period in life (Arnett, 2000, 2004).

Mental health problems among young adults Mental health problems are one of the greatest public health challenges in Europe (WHO, 2015). International comparisons indicate that men- tal health problems are less widespread in Norway than in other Euro- pean countries (Nes & Clench-Aas, 2011). However, they are common in both the adolescent and adult population in Norway, and therefore represent a considerable public health issue (Reneflot et al., 2018). Mental health problems may have negative effects on important areas 23

in life, such as quality of life, interpersonal relationships, and school and working life (Nes & Clench-Aas, 2011). Among youth and adults, mental disorders are the most important reason for bad health and im- paired functioning. In Norway, among young adults, mental health problems are the most important reason for receiving a work-assess- ment allowance and a disability pension (NAV, 2014, 2018a, 2018b). Some research indicates that there has been an increase in mental health problems among 16–17 year old adolescents in the UK (Collishaw, Maughan, Natarajan, & Pickles, 2010), and among adoles- cents and young adults (15–25 years old) in Belgium (Van Droogenbroeck, Spruyt, & Keppens, 2018). A systematic review of trends in adolescents’ (10–19 years old) self-reported psychosomatic health complaints reported that Northern Europe was the only region with a clear, significant minor increase in such complaints. However, this region does not have the highest total prevalence of self-reported psychosomatic health complaints at the present time (Potrebny, Wiium, & Lundegård, 2017). Nonetheless, there is limited knowledge of the development of mental health problems over time. However, we have data that indicate that the consequences of mental health prob- lems among young adults have recently become graver. This can be il- lustrated by the increase in the number of young adults receiving social insurance related to mental health problems. The number of teenage girls (15–20 years old) in Norway who have been diagnosed with a mental disorder has also increased by almost 40% the last 5 years (Reneflot et al., 2018). This may indicate that more teenage girls seek help for their mental health problems, or that it is easier to be diag- nosed with a mental disorder in 2018 than it was in 2013. Whether the increase in mental health problems among young adults is related to the challenges they currently face in the young-adult phase has also been discussed (Eckersley, 2011).

What are mental health problems? Health is a widely used notion with many different definitions and un- derstandings. From a biomedical perspective, health is seen as an ab- sence of disease, whereas in the humanistic view it is regarded as some- thing more than – or different to – this (Medin & Alexanderson, 2000). Gadamer (2006) argues that health is not something we experience or thematise before we experience something (e.g. a disease) that disturbs 24

the state of well-being. He sees health as a status of equilibrium. How- ever, it is difficult to know when this status is reached, and easier to know when it is disturbed. Nordenfelt (2018) argues that it is possible to suffer from disease and still experience good health, and sees health as a person’s ability to realise vital goals. The most common definition that reflects that health is more than the absence of disease was pro- vided by the WHO in 1948, according to which health is ‘complete physical, mental and social well-being and not merely the absence of disease or infirmity’ (WHO, 1948). This definition has been criticised for being utopian however, it is ground-breaking because it emphasises function and well-being and points out that health also has a social component (Medin & Alexanderson, 2000). In sociology, health and disease are seen as social constructs, meaning that the way we under- stand them is influenced by societal norms and values, power relation- ships, and theoretical frameworks. Definitions of health and disease are thus not equivocal but rather vary according the time, place, and person (Lian, 2004).

In this regard, one main division is found between medical or bioge- netic explanatory models and contextual or psychosocial explanatory models of mental health problems. There is no distinct boundary be- tween these two perspectives, though it is possible to see a clear divi- sion between them. The humanistic tradition relies on contextual and psychosocial explanatory models for individual problems, unlike med- ical or biogenetic models that find explanations for an individual’s problems in genetic or biological dispositions (Solem, 2017; Walker & Read, 2002). Definitions of some central concepts are discussed below in order to illustrate different understandings of mental health prob- lems.

The concept of mental disorders is used when a person experiences problems consistent with symptoms described in diagnostic manuals for mental disorders (von Soest & Hyggen, 2013). The most common mental disorders among young adults are anxiety and depression, along with behavioural difficulties, attention deficit hyperactivity dis- order, substance abuse, and learning disabilities (Reneflot et al., 2018). As the diagnostic criteria change over time, it is difficult to assess how

25

the occurrence of mental disorders develops over time (von Soest & Hyggen, 2013).

Mental health problems are defined as ailments that are troublesome, and that reduce life quality and function, though not necessarily to the extent that they fulfil all the requirements for a mental disorder. Mental health problems are often measured using questionnaires, such as the Hopkins Symptoms Checklist (HSCL-10) that is used in this thesis (von Soest & Hyggen, 2013).

Mental health problems in this thesis This thesis is grounded in values outside the medical paradigm and re- lies on contextual or psychosocial explanatory models of mental health problems. The participants included in the empirical material in this thesis have discontinued school or work due to mental health- or drug- related problems. No criteria for participants requiring a diagnosed mental disorder were set, yet many of the participants are likely to have been diagnosed. The inclusion of the participants in the study was un- dertaken through third parties, such as service leaders and responsible managers from NAV. It is therefore their assessment of participants in terms of the inclusion criteria that influenced who was offered partici- pation in the study. Furthermore, the participants in the survey are asked to answer on whether problems related to their mental health or substance abuse were a reason for their participation in the services. Diagnostic criteria were not set here either. This thesis further uses the HSCL-10 to measure the symptoms of mental health problems, and uses the cut-off score of this instrument (which is common practice) to ascertain who has symptoms of mental health problems. HSCL-10 is widely used to measure mental health problems, specifically symptoms of anxiety and depression. Mental health problems in this thesis are therefore mainly self-reported. Even if we do not know what kind of problems the participants have or their severity, we know their poten- tial consequences – namely, discontinuing school or work. However, we do not have data on whether there is a causal connection operating here. It may be that it is the situation of being outside important areas of life such as school and work that led to their mental health problems (Görlich, Pless, Katznelson, & Olsen, 2011).

26 Drug-related problems The inclusion criteria for the studies in this thesis also concerned young adults with drug related problems. Substance abuse is a considerable public health issue, both in Norway and globally (Institute for Health Metrics and Evaluation (IHME), 2016; Knudsen et al., 2017). It is often – though not necessarily – coupled with mental health problems. Pre- vious research in Norway has shown that 9 out of 10 people undergoing drug rehabilitation had one or more mental disorders (Bakken, Landheim, & Vaglum, 2003). It is unclear whether mental health prob- lems develop because of substance abuse, or vice versa; or whether the same risk factors lead to both mental health problems and drug-related problems (Chassin, Sher, Hussong, & Curran, 2013; Schulenberg & Maggs, 2002). Furthermore, substance abuse is often placed under the wide umbrella mental health problems in the documents of the Norwe- gian Institute of Public Health and in public documents in Norway (Reneflot et al., 2018). In this thesis, mental health problems are there- fore used as a broad overall conception that also includes substance abuse. In the empirical material, all of the participants but one answer- ing that drug-related problems were a contributing reason for their participation in the nature-based service also answered that mental health problems were a contributing reason for their participation.

27

Nature-based services: background and previous re- search

This chapter describes the background and previous research on na- ture-based services relevant to this thesis. First, a historical view of the relation between nature, animals, and humans is presented. This is fol- lowed by an overview of the central theoretical perspectives previously used in research on the relation between nature, animals, and humans. In addition, research areas relevant to nature-based services are briefly described, namely research focusing on the relation between animals and humans, and on the relation between nature and humans. Lastly, previous research on nature-based services in farm milieus is pre- sented.

A historical view of the relationship between nature, animals, and humans The relationship between humans and nature goes back to the origin of humankind. Nature has historically represented a threat – yet also a premise for human existence. Working with or in nature and living close to animals was essential for humans to find food and shelter; hence, this relation with nature was essential for human survival (Roszak, Gomes, & Kanner, 1995). In modern Western society, many people live at a distance from nature and relatively few people work with or in it. These changes have occurred gradually; however, in the last century, they have increased in scale and speed due to the indus- trialisation and urbanisation of the Western world. Today, many chil- dren in Western society grow up without a close connection to nature and some may never have seen a cow in real life (Børresen, 2006; Katcher & Beck, 1987). At the same time, many people have personal experiences with nature and animals that exert a positive influence on their quality of life, health and well-being. Indeed, companion animals such as dogs, cats, and rabbits are common, and many people use na- ture for outdoor recreation. Among Norwegians, this use of nature is ubiquitous (Statistics Norway, 2017).

The awareness of nature’s potential positive effects on human health is not new. Hippocrates (4th century BC) was the first to underline the im- portance of nature to human health and in therapy (Gallis, 2013). In 28

the last half of the 19th century, outdoor surroundings and animals were used in rehabilitation and nursing, and were viewed as beneficial as- pects of ‘treatment’ (Nightingale, 1859; Tuke, 1882). Today, there is re- search evidence of potential benefits to humans from interacting with nature (Annerstedt & Wahrborg, 2011; Bragg & Atkins, 2016).

The use of nature in promoting human health is not new. Nature, in- cluding plants, gardens, and animals, has been used for this purpose for many years. Many early hospitals and other such institutions were traditionally built in rural areas, surrounded by nature, gardens, and animals. Tuke (1882) describes one of the earliest examples of using nature in an active way in rehabilitation. At York Retreat, a mental health care institution in England, interaction with animals and walks in quiet outdoor surroundings were part of the facility’s treatments (Tuke, 1882). In 1859, pioneering nurse Florence Nightingale wrote in her book Notes on Nursing that ‘a small pet is often an excellent com- panion for the sick’ (Nightingale, 1859).

Theories on the influence of nature on human well-being and health Various theories exist concerning the influence of nature on human well-being and health. The biophilia hypothesis developed from evolu- tionary psychology and emphasises that humans have an innate and predisposed connection to nature. This leads to humans having an urge to affiliate with other forms of life. Being in nature therefore fulfils a basic need in human life and, hence, nature is good for humans (Kellert & Wilson, 1993; Wilson, 1984). One of the other central theories focus- ing on why nature is beneficial to human health, and also deriving from , is attention restoration theory (ART). This theory highlights the restorative effects of nature, making nature a good location for restoring human attention (Kaplan, 1995). Attention restoration theory is widely used in research and interventions; how- ever, a recent systematic review concludes that ‘there is uncertainty re- garding which aspects of attention may be affected by exposure to nat- ural environments’ (Ohly et al., 2016, p. 305). Ulrich (1993) developed psychophysiological stress recovery theory, which also derives from evolutionary psychology, and which asserts that nature has a stress-re- ducing effect on humans. These stress-reducing qualities are due to 29

natural surroundings providing an environment free from the stressors found in more urban surroundings. These theories principally focus on the effects of looking at nature, which is identified and described by Pretty (2004) as one of three levels of exposure to nature. The other levels are being in and close to nature, and actively participating and being involved in nature. The effects of the two latter, more interactive levels of exposure to nature are related to the aforementioned theories, as well as to theories of social support, self-efficacy, connectedness, the benefits of physical activity, occupation/work as therapy, recovery, and salutogenesis (Cutcliffe & Travale, 2016; Grahn et al., 2010; Granerud & Eriksson, 2014; Kogstad et al., 2014; Sempik et al., 2010). This thesis focuses on aspects not specifically related to the theories that derive from evolutionary psychology (e.g. ART and biophilia). The focus in this thesis is on nature-based services that allow individuals to actively participate and be involved in nature. Theories of salutogenesis and so- cial support are central to social science and social work and are there- fore used in this thesis to expand knowledge of nature-based services. These theories are presented later in this thesis.

Research fields of relevance for nature-based services A variety of research traditions are of special interest when studying nature-based services. Two research fields are of particular interest, namely research focusing on the interaction between animals and hu- mans, and that focusing on the relation between nature and humans. Both of these research fields are grounded on various theories, includ- ing biophilia, ART, and psychophysiological stress recovery theory, as well as on other theories, including salutogenesis and social support. Interactions between humans and nature and between humans and an- imals are both present in nature-based services in farm milieus. These two research fields are therefore briefly discussed below.

What do we know about the relation between humans and nature? A considerable amount of research has concluded that nature and green spaces are good for a person’s mental and physical health (Beyer et al., 2014; De Vries, Verheij, Groenewegen, & Spreeuwenberg, 2003; Maas, Verheij, Groenewegen, De Vries, & Spreeuwenberg, 2006; Pretty, 2004; Van den Berg, Maas, Verheij, & Groenewegen, 2010). In a systematic review of nature-assisted therapy, Annerstedt and 30 Wahrborg (2011, p. 15) conclude that the literature ‘supports the effec- tiveness and appropriateness of nature-assisted therapy as a relevant resource for public health. Significant improvements were found for varied outcomes in diverse diagnoses’.

From a philosophical perspective, Heidegger (2007) emphasises the existential element of connectedness to nature, in which humans expe- rience being part of nature. This experience provides humans with a deep cognition of their own existence (Heidegger, 2007). Tordsson (2003) introduces the notion of ‘Nature’s open appeal’, which states that nature allows each individual to form his or her own interpretation and understanding of it. Tordsson (2003) also reflects on how nature has been understood in the past century: it has been viewed as an anti- type of modern society, that is, nature is what society is not, and vice versa. Hence, nature relieves humans of the strains and limitations of everyday life, giving them the opportunity to be on the same level as the challenges they meet because they seek these challenges, which are related to their own assumptions, themselves (Tordsson, 2003). This contrasts with the demands of urban life, in which challenges come from the outside and are assessed by scales, with diffuse criteria for fulfilment, over which humans have no influence. Nature also is cred- ited with freeing humans from society’s roles and expectations (Bele, 2013).

Connectedness to nature involves a sense of meaningful involvement in something larger than oneself (Howell, Dopko, Passmore, & Buro, 2011; Howell, Passmore, & Buro, 2013). Some researchers see connect- edness as a trait, defining it as ‘an individual’s affective, experiential connection to nature’ (Mayer & Frantz, 2004, p. 504). A significant cor- relation has been found between the trait of connectedness to nature and life satisfaction (Mayer & Frantz, 2004). A connectedness to nature has also been found to correlate positively with psychological and social well-being (Howell et al., 2011). The theory of nature-connectedness is primarily used by researchers interested in environmental issues to de- termine whether connections exist between the trait of nature-connect- edness and engagement in environmental management issues (Restall & Conrad, 2015), which is not a focus of this thesis.

31

Research shows that gardening interventions benefit mental health (Clatworthy, Hinds, & M. Camic, 2013). An intervention study in Nor- way found reduced symptoms of depression after a 12-week horticul- tural therapy intervention for those with depression (Gonzalez, Hartig, Patil, Martinsen, & Kirkevold, 2009, 2011a, 2011b). Improvements in social interaction have been reported in a study using social and thera- peutic horticulture for people experiencing mental health problems (Sempik et al., 2014).

The Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences has a rehabilitation garden for people with stress-related illnesses. The rehabilitation gar- den combines the use of restorative natural environments, horticul- tural therapy, and traditional occupational therapy, physiotherapy, and psychotherapy (Grahn et al., 2010). Several research projects have been conducted on this rehabilitation garden (see e.g. Grahn, Pálsdóttir, Ottosson, & Jonsdottir, 2017; Pàlsdòttir, Persson, Persson, & Grahn, 2014; Pálsdóttir, Stigsdotter, Persson, Thorpert, & Grahn, 2018). The theory of supportive environments (SET) (Grahn et al., 2010) has been used to describe qualities in natural surroundings, for example, in relation to this rehabilitation garden in Sweden.

What do we know about the interaction between humans and an- imals? The potential positive effects on humans from interaction with animals have been a subject of research for many years, and several positive ef- fects on human as a result of interacting with animals have been re- ported. Research on the potential effects of animals on humans has fo- cused on: 1) physiological effects; 2) social support and animals as cat- alysts for promoting communication between humans; 3) promotion of self-esteem and mastery; and 4) promotion of caring ability and em- pathy (Berget & Braastad, 2008).

One of the earliest research articles on this topic was published in 1944. Bossard (1944) emphasises the important role of companion animals for human mental health. He argues that especially children benefit from growing up with animals, which can provide an unlimited source of love and strengthen children’s sense of responsibility and social 32

competence. Levinson (1962), who used his dog in therapy with chil- dren, recounts how the children talked to him through the dog, which made it easier for them to discuss difficult issues. The company of pet animals is important for many humans and can, in some cases, be equal to relations with other people (Garrity & Stallones, 1998). The company of a dog also increases the number of positive interactions an individual has with other people (McNicholas & Collis, 2000). Moreover, animals can help humans to more easily express and communicate their feel- ings (McNicholas & Collis, 2006), which can contribute to more effec- tive social and emotional learning for young people experiencing social and behavioural challenges. Specifically, interaction with animals can contribute to better impulse control among children and adolescents (Mallon, 1994).

Research on the effects of horses on children, adolescents, and adult women with mental health problems demonstrates that horses can strengthen self-esteem and social competence (Burgon, 2003; Koren & Træen, 2003; Rothe, Vega, Torres, Soler, & Pazos, 2005). A study of adolescents (aged 12–15) in Norway who participated in an interven- tion with horses at farm-based stables found a significant increase in perceived social support compared with the waiting-list control group (Hauge, Kvalem, Berget, Enders-Slegers, & Braastad, 2014). However, no differences in self-esteem and general self-efficacy between the groups were found. A lower level of perceived social support prior to the intervention predicted an increase in the mastering of skills during the intervention (Hauge et al., 2014).

Several physiological effects have been identified in humans from in- teractions and physical contact with dogs, such as a decline in blood pressure and heart rate, along with decreased levels of stress hormones (Cole, Gawlinski, Steers, & Kotlerman, 2007; Handlin et al., 2011; Odendaal & Meintjes, 2003).

The use of animals in various kinds of animal-assisted interventions is increasing, and we find literature on the topic published both in Nor- way and internationally (Berget, Krøger, & Thorød, 2018; Fine, 2015). Most of knowledge of animal-assisted interventions derives from an- throzoology, a relatively new, interdisciplinary discipline combining 33

knowledge from ethology, medicine, psychology, veterinary medicine, and zoology, and studying all components of the interaction between humans and animals (Berget et al., 2018).

Most research in this field has been conducted on pets, but the effects of interacting with farm animals are expected to be similar (Bokkers, 2006; Mallon, 1994). Nature-based services that use farm milieus often have a mix of pet animals (e.g., dogs, cats, and rabbits) and farm ani- mals (e.g., cows, horses, and sheep). Research on interactions between farm animals and humans supports the view that farm animals have the same qualities as pet animals, and that animals are an important element in nature-based services (Ellingsen-Dalskau, Morken, et al., 2016; Granerud & Eriksson, 2014; Hassink et al., 2017; Pedersen, Ihlebæk, et al., 2012). However, different animal species may have dif- ferent qualities and may appeal to participants in different ways. Larger animals, such as cows and horses, can contribute to participants’ expe- rience of mastery (when they manage to handle them), and can also provide a sense of security. Conversely, large animals may be frighten- ing for some people, who may prefer small animals that they can hold on their laps (Granerud & Eriksson, 2014; Hassink et al., 2017).

In addition, the effects of interaction with animals may vary among hu- mans. Some may not like animals or might be afraid. Animals may therefore not have the same effect on everyone (Berget et al., 2018).

Research on nature-based services for people with mental health problems in farm milieus In recent years, there has been an increase in research on nature-based services and interventions. Results from a variety of nature-based ser- vices generally demonstrate positive changes related to participants’ general condition, self-confidence, ability to manage problems and take responsibility, experience of meaning, and physical health and so- cial skills (Hassink & Van Dijk, 2006; Sempik et al., 2010). However, it should be noted that the results from qualitative and quantitative stud- ies vary. While only a limited number of quantitative studies have shown positive effects from nature-based services in farm milieus, a

34

large volume of qualitative research and case studies shows positive ef- fects from these services (Bragg & Atkins, 2016). However, many quan- titative studies have methodological weaknesses that may result in bi- ased results (Bragg & Atkins, 2016). In a systematic review, Iancu et al. (2015) concluded that the quality of research on the mental health ef- fects of nature-based services in farm milieus is moderate and high- lighted several methodological weaknesses in the research.

Bragg and Atkins (2016, p. 30) identified the following effects on the psychological and social health of participants in the literature: ‘In- crease in: 1) social inclusion, 2) social and work skills, 3) empower- ment, 4) social functioning, 5) coping ability, 6) social rehabilitation, 7) cognitive functioning and well-being, 8) self-esteem and 9) reductions in mood and 10) depressions and anxiety-related symptoms’. However, many areas remain unexplored, and many questions remain unan- swered. Research on nature-based interventions in farm milieus is rel- atively recent, and much of it comes from Norway, the Netherlands, and the United Kingdom (Bragg & Atkins, 2016). Some of the research is specialised, focusing solely on, for example, the role of animals, hor- ticulture, or other isolated aspects of the nature-based services or the interventions. In recent years, several papers reviewing the literature on different types of nature-based services in farm milieus in relation to a variety of participant groups have been published (Artz & Davis, 2017; Gorman & Cacciatore, 2017; Iancu et al., 2015; Salomon, Salomon, & Beeber, 2017). This may reflect the fact that interest in na- ture-based services is increasing, and that more people wish to explore their potential.

In what follows, previous research on nature-based services in farm mi- lieus for people with mental health problems is presented thematically and chronologically. Searches for research studies on nature-based ser- vices were conducted continually during the entire project period. Var- ious databases were used, including PsychInfo, Academic Search Premier, Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature, Medline, ISI Web of Science, Scopus, and Google Scholar. In addition, library databases were used in the searches. The search terms used in- clude ‘green care’, ‘care farm*’, ‘farming for health’, social farm*’, ‘ther- apeutic horticulture’, ‘animal-assisted*’, ‘horticulture therapy’, and 35

‘gardening’. Research studies were also found by reading the reference lists of relevant papers.

Intervention studies in farm milieus for people with mental health problems The first two studies presented are intervention studies. The interven- tions only exist as components of the research project, and hence do not represent actual nature-based services. However, the interventions took place in farm milieus and investigated important elements of na- ture-based services in that context. The first intervention study is a ran- domised controlled trial (RCT) conducted in Norway that included 90 participants (aged 18–58 years) with a variety of mental health prob- lems and investigated the effects of working with farm animals. While no significant effects were found during the intervention, compared to the control group, the intervention group showed significant improve- ments, with fewer symptoms of anxiety (Berget et al., 2011) and im- provements of general self-efficacy (Berget et al., 2008) from baseline to six months after the intervention. In addition, the participants in the treatment group showed significant positive changes in their coping abilities (Berget et al., 2008) and a decrease in depression (Berget et al., 2011) from baseline to the six-month follow-up. A similar RCT study involving 29 adults (aged 23–58 years) with clinical depression found no significant effects between the treatment group and a waiting- list control group. However, the intervention group showed a reduction in the severity of depression during the intervention period compared with a waiting-list control group (Pedersen, Martinsen, et al., 2012). An increase in general self-efficacy within the treatment group during the intervention was also found (Pedersen, Martinsen, et al., 2012). Longi- tudinal behavioural studies were conducted in relation to both of these RCT studies. These longitudinal behavioural studies found that in- crease in working skills was correlated with increase in self-efficacy (Berget, Skarsaune, Ekeberg, & Braastad, 2007; Pedersen et al., 2011) and decrease in symptoms of anxiety (Berget et al., 2007; Pedersen et al., 2011) and depression (Pedersen et al., 2011). Pedersen, Ihlebæk, et al. (2012) also conducted qualitative interviews with eight participants (aged 25–54 years) from the RCT sample. The results of the qualitative study demonstrated that farm animal-assisted interventions could sup- plement mental health rehabilitation. An important element was the 36

possibility for participants to experience an ordinary life – such as un- dertaking ordinary work, being appreciated, being a colleague, and the like. In addition, the flexibility and coping experiences in the interven- tions were highlighted as being important (Pedersen, Ihlebæk, et al., 2012).

Research on nature-based services for people with mental health problems There is a lack of research on all kinds of nature-based services in farm- milieus that exist independently of research projects, including nature- based services in farm-milieus for people with mental health problems. Relevant research studies are presented chronologically below.

Hine et al. (2008) used both qualitative and quantitative methods in a study of 72 participants with a variety of needs. It included people with mental health problems, unemployed people, and disaffected young people. They found significant changes in self-esteem and improve- ments in the overall mood of participants who had spent time in na- ture-based services in farm milieus in the UK. In the qualitative com- ponent, participants indicated that they appreciated being out in the ‘fresh air’, being with animals, being social with other human beings, and having the chance to experience mastery (Hine et al., 2008).

Elings and Hassink (2008) performed focus group interviews with 42 people with mental health- or drug-related problems working in differ- ent nature-based services in farm milieus in the Netherlands. The par- ticipants emphasised the social aspects at the services, such as belong- ing to a group. In addition, feeling at ease and the informality at the services were appreciated, along with being involved in useful activi- ties. Participants described development of self-esteem, self-respect and responsibility.

Hassink et al. (2010) conducted a qualitative study and interviewed participants, farmers, and health professionals in different nature- based services in farm milieus in the Netherlands. One of the partici- pant groups interviewed were people with mental health problems (n = 16). The participants appreciated the community on the farm, the attitude of the farmer, and being treated as a ‘normal’ person. They also 37

appreciated the work they performed in flexible and varied work tasks where they could have contact with animals. In addition, the green en- vironment and the qualities of nature were appreciated (Hassink et al., 2010).

In a qualitative study in Norway that included interviews with 20 par- ticipants with mental health problems participating in nature-based services in farm milieus, similar results were found (Granerud & Eriksson, 2014). The main categories found were ‘a meaningful life’, the participants appreciation of contact with animals, the natural type of work they performed, good leadership, and group processes. The participants experienced mastery of suitable challenges and they felt a sense of community and belonging in the services (Granerud & Eriksson, 2014).

In a qualitative study in the Netherlands, Iancu, Zweekhorst, Veltman, van Balkom, and Bunders (2014) studied people with mental health problems in nature-based services in farm milieus (n = 14) and in day centres (n = 12). The study concluded that all the services included in the study provided recovery for people with such problems. However, different services may be suitable for different participants. Nature- based services appeared to be particularly good for facilitating social inclusion and community integration (Iancu et al., 2014).

A study in the UK using both quantitative and qualitative research methods identified a significant positive relationship between the length of time participants spent at a care farm and measure of well- being. The study also reported that the experiences at the care farm nurtured personal development among participants (Leck et al., 2015). This study comprised 216 participants in the quantitative component, with 137 participating in follow-up data collection, along with 33 inter- views with participants.

A recent research project in Norway focused on people who had dis- continued work and were participating in pre-vocational training on care farms employed both qualitative (Ellingsen-Dalskau, Morken, et al., 2016) and quantitative research methods (Ellingsen-Dalskau, Berget, et al., 2016). The quantitative study included 194 participants 38

aged 18–66 in prevocational training programmes in nature-based ser- vices in farm milieus. The article made use of self-determination theory and found that the services could lead to healthy functioning, motiva- tion, and well-being for the participants. Important elements were the feeling of being a useful colleague, a sense of group-belonging, and so- cial support from the farmer. The latter, including support, under- standing, and acknowledgement, were found to be the most important elements for the positive development of the participants (Ellingsen- Dalskau, Berget, et al., 2016). Results from the qualitative study indi- cated that five elements were important for participants: structure and flexibility, understanding and acknowledgement, guidance and posi- tive feedback, nature and animals, and reflections on personal func- tioning and on the future (Ellingsen-Dalskau, Morken, et al., 2016).

Nature-based services for young adults with mental health prob- lems Research focusing more specifically on young adults with mental health problems participating in nature-based services is even scarcer. A qualitative Danish report on nature-based services for young adults aged 18 to 30 who had discontinued school or work and had mental or physical problems reported that 51.5% of them returned to regular work, school, or other activities following participation. The im- portance of the farmer was emphasised, along with experiences of mas- tery, social support, and contact with animals (Fuglsang, 2011).

Kogstad et al. (2014) followed nine young adults aged 17 to 27 in three different nature-based services over a two-year period. The young adults were not in school or work, and they were in vulnerable situa- tions where they were at risk of social exclusion. The study concluded that these services could be an important supplement to other health and welfare services for this group. Participants indicated that the ser- vices provided recognition, supportive relationships, motivation, meaning, positive coping, self-esteem, confidence, and hope. The im- portant elements mentioned were the leader, varied tasks that led to possibilities for mastery, and nature and animals (Kogstad et al., 2014).

39

Summing up – previous research on nature-based services in farm milieus Previous research on nature-based services in farm milieus is scarce, and the research presented above originates in three European coun- tries: Norway, the Netherlands, and the UK. Most of the research stud- ies are qualitative, though some quantitative studies are to be found, most of which are quantitative intervention studies of animal-assisted therapy with farm animals. Similar results are found in the other re- search papers, and the important dimensions of nature-based services in farm milieus seem to be social aspects relating to being part of a com- munity in the service, being social with other participants and social support from both the service leader and other participants, and being acknowledged and appreciated. In addition, flexible, useful and varied work tasks in natural surroundings and interaction with animals led to experiences of mastery and coping. The results indicate that these fac- tors lead to improvements in participants’ mental health, self-efficacy, self-esteem, well-being, and coping abilities. As can be seen, a variety of dimensions of social support are central in many of the research findings; however, the focus in previous research has not been specifi- cally on social support and different dimensions of social support in nature-based services in farm milieus.

40 Salutogenesis and social support: Theoretical frame- work and previous research

This chapter presents the theoretical framework for this thesis. First, salutogenesis, a theory that in this context is regarded as central to both nature-based services and to social work for people with mental health problems, is discussed. Salutogenesis represents an orientation that fo- cuses on the factors that may promote good health. Furthermore, the- ories and previous research on social support are presented. Social sup- port is an important concept in both the salutogenic model and in na- ture-based services. Social support is shown to be essential for human development and health, and of importance for people with mental health problems. This thesis also includes empirical data about sense of coherence (SOC), a central part of the salutogenic model. Previous research on the relation between SOC, social support, and mental health is presented. Links between the salutogenic model and previous research on nature-based services are also highlighted.

Salutogenesis The word salutogenesis, with its origin in the Latin word salus (health) and the Greek word genesis (origin), means ‘the origin of health’. Salutogenesis may be viewed as an orientation, a model, and a sense of coherence (Mittelmark & Bauer, 2017). The salutogenic orientation fo- cuses on factors that can promote health, rather than those causing dis- ease (Mittelmark & Bauer, 2017). The salutogenic model sees health as a movement along a continuum between ease and dis-ease, and focuses on the factors that promote movement towards the healthy end of the continuum (Mittelmark & Bauer, 2017). Sense of coherence is the core concept in the salutogenic model and representing that which helps elicit movement towards the healthy end of the ease/dis-ease contin- uum (Mittelmark & Bauer, 2017). Salutogenesis is viewed by some as an umbrella concept that covers a range of different theories and con- cepts, all with a salutogenic orientation (Lindström & Eriksson, 2015). This thesis focuses primarily on the model of salutogenesis introduced by Antonovsky (1979), and the core concept of the model, SOC. This is one of the most common and well-known models with a salutogenic orientation, and many research findings are related to Antonovsky’s theories. 41

The salutogenic model Antonovsky is the originator of the salutogenic model, and it all started with him asking the question, ‘What makes people healthy?’ (Lindström & Eriksson, 2015). Antonovsky conducted a study of men- opausal women who had survived extremely stressful life events and found that compared to a control group, these women had more symp- toms of menopause and stress. However, a small group of these women, despite these extremely stressful life events, seemed to func- tion as well as women with more ordinary life cycles. Antonovsky be- came interested in why these women remained healthy and conducted in-depth interviews with them (Antonovsky, 1979). These interviews laid the basis for Antonovsky’s salutogenic theory and the core concept in his model, SOC.

Antonovsky’s salutogenic model was first presented in his book Health, Stress and Coping (Antonovsky, 1979); at that point, no research on the salutogenic model had been undertaken. In his next book, Antonovsky (1987) focused a great deal on the SOC concept, which is presented later in this chapter. Other aspects of the salutogenic model have received less attention. Mittelmark and Bauer (2017) highlight four aspects of the model that they view as having been neglected and in need of more attention: 1) the origins of SOC; 2) answers to the salutogenic question other than SOC; 3) health defined as something other than the absence of disease; and 4) processes linking SOC and health. For the second theme, the potential role of physical and social environments in the development of health is one of the areas of inter- est, and is a subject that requires greater attention (Mittelmark & Bauer, 2017). Thus, environments are not to be viewed as an early force in shaping SOC, but as mediating factors that may facilitate coping (Mittelmark & Bauer, 2017). Mittelmark and Bauer (2017) also point out a discrepancy between the specifications of the salutogenic model and research articles reporting on the relationship between SOC and different disease endpoints. The salutogenic model emphasises the im- portance of focusing on strengths rather than on the symptoms of dis- ease. Hence, that measurement of strengths or positive factors is more appropriate when applying salutogenesis or SOC as a theoretical refer- ence frame.

42

Sense of coherence Sense of coherence is a central concept in Antonovsky’s salutogenic model (Antonovsky, 1979, 1987). Antonovsky viewed SOC as decisive in the development of good health, and for the movement towards good health on the continuum between ease and dis-ease. Sense of coher- ence contains three sub-concepts: comprehensibility, manageability, and meaningfulness. People with a strong SOC experience life as co- herent by virtue of being comprehensible, manageable, and meaning- ful. Antonovsky (1987) meant that a strong SOC leads to inner trust and safety, to the individual possessing inner resources, as well as resources from the surroundings, and to the ability to use these resources in a health-promoting way. Antonovsky (1987) originally defined SOC as:

a global orientation that expresses the extent to which one has a pervasive, enduring, though dynamic feeling of confidence that (1) the stimuli from one’s internal and external environments in the course of living are struc- tured, predictable and explicable; (2) the resources are available to one to meet the demands posed by these stimuli; and (3) these demands are chal- lenges worthy of investment and engagement (p.19).

The sub-concept of comprehensibility highlights the importance of in- ternal or external stimuli being cognitively comprehensible to the indi- vidual; that is, the stimuli are understood as being well-arranged, co- herent, structured, and clear data, rather than being chaotic, unorgan- ised, random, unexpected, and incomprehensible. The sub-concept of manageability emphasises the importance of the individual feeling that he or she has sufficient resources available to manage the demands of the stimuli. The importance of having a strong sense of meaning in life is emphasised, and is represented by the sub-concept of meaningful- ness. Central to SOC is an understanding that problems and demands in life are viewed as challenges to be met, rather than stressors from which the individual would like to be spared – that life is emotionally understandable and that some of life’s problems and demands are worth engagement and involvement (Antonovsky, 1987). The three components in SOC interact with one another; however, the most im- portant motivating factor, according to Antonovsky (1987), is the sub- concept of meaningfulness.

43

Antonovsky (1993) emphasised that SOC is not to be viewed as a coping strategy or a personality trait, but more as a life orientation. Hence, SOC can serve as a coping resource that enables individuals choose be- tween different strategies in order to solve problems or manage inci- dents in life.

Antonovsky (1987) highlighted the fact that an individual does not need to experience the whole world as coherent to have a strong SOC. For some, SOC involves a narrow area, while for others it represents a wider area. What is important is that certain areas in life are important to the individual. If this is not the case, it is unlikely that the person has a strong SOC.

Antonovsky never defined what could be viewed as a normal SOC alt- hough he wrote about strong and weak SOC. This makes it more diffi- cult to interpret the effects of SOC on phenomena such as mental health (Antonovsky, 1979, 1987; M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2006).

Several papers have focused on the possibilities of strengthening the SOC of diverse patient groups in mental health care using various forms of therapy based on Antonovsky’s SOC theory (Joachim, Lyon, & Farrell, 2003; Landsverk & Kane, 1998; Langeland & Vinje, 2017).

Antonovsky (1987) also developed a questionnaire to measure SOC. This questionnaire is widely used globally and is central in much quan- titative research on SOC (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2005). A presen- tation of this questionnaire and its psychometric qualities follows in the methods section of this thesis. In addition, other SOC questionnaires besides Antonovsky’s two original questionnaires (SOC-29 and SOC- 13) have been developed and are in use in research today (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2005).

Generalised resistance resources Generalised resistance resources are viewed as cornerstones for devel- oping a strong SOC. They are connected to each individual’s personality and capacities, and to resources in the individual’s proximal and distal surroundings. The generalised resistance resources involves both ma- terial and non-material qualities, for example, money, housing, self- 44

image, knowledge, contact with inner feelings, social relations, and having a feeling that there is meaning to life (Antonovsky, 1987; Lindström & Eriksson, 2015). According to Antonovsky (1987), at least four of the general resistance resources need to be available in order to develop a strong SOC: meaningful activities, existential thoughts, con- tact with inner feelings, and social relations. In addition, it is not enough for these resistance resources to merely be available; one must be able to use them in a way that promotes health (Lindström & Eriksson, 2015). Antonovsky (1979, p. 99) defined generalised re- sistance resources as ‘every characterisation of a person, group or en- vironment that promotes effective management of tension’. The rela- tion between resistance resources and SOC is mutual (Antonovsky, 1979). When the person’s generalised resistance resources and the challenges that this individual faces are concordant, the individual can cope. Again, this may lead to strengthened SOC (Langeland, Wahl, Kristoffersen, & Hanestad, 2007). Therefore, facilitating positive cop- ing experiences by facing appropriate challenges is essential (Langeland & Vinje, 2017). It is important to strengthen the available general resistance resources and create new kinds of general resistance resources to promote health among individuals (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2006).

SOC and mental health A systematic review concludes that the relation between SOC and health is supported by several empirical findings of studies from around the world, indicating that the higher the SOC, the better one’s health (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2006). There is evidence that the stronger the SOC, the fewer the symptoms of mental health problems. Moreover, a strong SOC has been found to protect against anxiety, de- pression, burnout, and hopelessness (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2006; Grevenstein, Bluemke, & Kroeninger-Jungaberle, 2016; Länsimies, Pietilä, Hietasola-Husu, & Kangasniemi, 2017; Moksnes, Espnes, & Haugan, 2014; K. W. Nilsson, Leppert, Simonsson, & Starrin, 2010). According to M. Eriksson and Lindström (2006), a strong SOC is also associated with health resources such as optimism, robustness, control, and coping. In studies, SOC is used as both an outcome variable and a predictor variable for health outcomes; these studies controlled for

45

confounding variables such as education, age, health status, social sup- port, and marital status (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2006; Länsimies et al., 2017). The strong relation between SOC and mental health has been found in diverse populations (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2006; Länsimies et al., 2017; K. W. Nilsson et al., 2010).

A strong SOC has been found to ease the perception of stress among adolescent girls (aged 13–18 years) (Nielsen & Hansson, 2007). Sense of coherence also has been found to influence health-related behaviour among students; that is, the stronger the SOC, the healthier the behav- iour (Posadzki, Stockl, Musonda, & Tsouroufli, 2010). It has been found to promote resilience (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2006), and seems to have an impact on people’s quality of life; that is, the stronger the SOC, the better the quality of life (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2007).

SOC and social support The concept of social support is elaborated later in this thesis. Here, the relation between SOC and social support is outlined. Antonovsky (1987) describes the relation between SOC and social support, empha- sising that more attention needs to be directed towards why and how social support improves our health. Antonovsky sees social support as one of many variables that are part of a person’s general resistance re- sources (Antonovsky, 1987).

The positive relation between SOC and social support has been found in a range of studies (Holmberg, Thelin, & Stiernström, 2004; Langeland & Wahl, 2009; B. Nilsson, Holmgren, & Westman, 2000; Volanen, Lahelma, Silventoinen, & Suominen, 2004; Wolff & Ratner, 1999). The possibility of a reciprocal relationship between SOC and so- cial support has been raised, suggesting that social support may lead to higher levels of SOC. Higher levels of SOC might also help the individ- ual engaging in social relations and thereby obtain social support (Langeland & Vinje, 2017; Volanen et al., 2004). In addition, social support has been found to be important in restoring a person’s SOC (Langeland & Wahl, 2009; Skärsäter, Langius, & Ågren, 2005). In a study of people with mental health problems, the social-support di- mension of opportunities for nurturance in particular – being able to provide care or support for others – is important in eliciting positive 46

changes in a person’s SOC (Langeland & Wahl, 2009). In a study of adolescents in lower secondary school, higher levels of social support and of neighbourhood cohesion were related to higher levels of SOC (Marsh, Clinkinbeard, Thomas, & Evans, 2007).

Salutogenesis and nature-based services Salutogenic theory is one of many theories used within the nature- based context (Sempik et al., 2010). It has been presented in theoretical papers on nature-based services (Fjeldavli, 2006), and several studies have related their empirical findings to salutogenesis, the salutogenic model, and the concept of SOC (Granerud & Eriksson, 2014; Leck et al., 2015; Schreuder et al., 2014). Meaningfulness has been found to be of importance in several studies of nature-based services (Granerud & Eriksson, 2014; Pedersen, Ihlebæk, et al., 2012). A study in the Neth- erlands (Schreuder et al., 2014) explored the salutogenic mechanisms of an outdoor experimental programme on youth care farms. Schreuder et al. (2014) concluded that several resources on the care farm contributed to young adults’ (17-22 years old) personal develop- ment and SOC: ‘In general, the attitude of the farmer, working with an- imals, the informal atmosphere and being temporarily cut off from the former environment were elements most positively highlighted by the youngsters’ (Schreuder et al., 2014, p. 139).

Restorative and supportive environments Grahn et al. (2010) developed supportive environment theory, which can be linked to salutogenesis (Grahn, 2011). The theory is based on a premise that, over millions of years, humans have developed in a social, cultural, and physical milieu, in which the physical milieu has above all consisted of nature, and the cultural and social milieus have been char- acterised by small groups of humans and settlements. The theory em- phasises that supportive environments are needed for humans to de- velop physically and mentally, resulting in a need for supportive envi- ronments in order to maintain good health (Grahn et al., 2010). A sup- portive environment needs to be perceived as comprehensible, accessi- ble, and safe (Grahn, 2011). Supportive environment theory empha- sises that individuals’ need for supportive environments varies de- pending on their mental and physical health (Grahn et al., 2010). Thus, the importance of other humans and nature may change depending on 47

individuals’ mental and physical resources (Grahn et al., 2010). The need for salutogenic, supportive environments is greater the more troubled an individual is, and supportive environments should be re- storative (Grahn, 2011). Supportive environment theory is used in the research and development of nature-based rehabilitation programmes in the previously mentioned rehabilitation garden in Sweden (Grahn et al., 2010).

The possible relation between restorative or supportive environments and salutogenesis is described in several papers (Bengtsson & Grahn, 2014; Grahn et al., 2010; von Lindern, Lymeus, & Hartig, 2017). Envi- ronmental conditions and interactions between the individual and the environment may be viewed as a generalised resistance resource, as de- scribed by Antonovsky (von Lindern et al., 2017). It is hypothesised that restorative environments may contribute to regular restoration and thereby contribute to a strong SOC. The natural environment has been highlighted as especially interesting in this context in supporting individuals’ possibilities for viewing their circumstances as compre- hensible, manageable, and meaningful (von Lindern et al., 2017).

Critique of salutogenesis The salutogenic model has elicited varying critiques – more specifi- cally, critiques of the concept of SOC. This critiques has been generated both by theorists and through empirical research. The principal com- ponents of the critiques are presented below.

The concept of SOC seems to have a more multidimensional than uni- dimensional character, and has been criticised for being psychometri- cally unclear. It has been suggested that more research is required to clarify its structure and dimensions (Lindström & Eriksson, 2015). Lindström and Eriksson (2015) argue that developing new versions of the SOC scale is not a recommended solution. Instead, they suggest that standardisation of existing measures and the use of qualitative methods to better examine the concept of SOC and its structure should be emphasised (Lindström & Eriksson, 2015). However, this appears to not be an appropriate suggestion, considering that it will only con- ceal the problems with the SOC scale. Lerdal et al. (2017) highlight the importance of further testing of the psychometric characteristics of the 48

scale and recommend that further refinement of the SOC scales is war- ranted.

The concept of SOC also has been criticised for lacking evidence of sta- bility over time (Geyer, 1997; Lindström & Eriksson, 2015). According to Antonovsky, SOC develops during childhood and young adulthood and stabilises around the age of 30 before weakening around the age of retirement (Antonovsky, 1987). Empirical support has not been estab- lished for this framework as SOC has not been found to be as stable as Antonovsky assumed (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2005, 2006). How- ever, Hakanen, Feldt, and Leskinen (2007) found SOC to be relatively stable in individuals with high levels of SOC, and weaker in individuals with low levels of SOC. This is in accordance with Antonovsky (1987) hypothesis. Empirical studies indicate that negative life changes de- crease the level of SOC (B. Nilsson, Holmgren, Stegmayr, & Westman, 2003; Volanen, Suominen, Lahelma, Koskenvuo, & Silventoinen, 2007). In addition, important professional interventions can strengthen a person’s SOC (Langeland et al., 2006; Weissbecker et al., 2002). K. W. Nilsson et al. (2010) concluded that SOC seems to in- crease with age over the entire lifespan.

Antonovsky also has been criticised for not devoting enough attention to the role of emotions in coping processes (Geyer, 1997). Even though Antonovsky (1987) writes about emotions and acknowledges that they have a role in coping processes, a lack of a detailed theory on this aspect may be viewed as a weakness to his concept of SOC (Griffiths, 2010).

The strong association between SOC strength, as measured by the SOC instruments, and psychological distress has also elicited criticism (Breslin, Hepburn, Ibrahim, & Cole, 2006; Geyer, 1997). Some have ar- gued that SOC is largely a measure of psychological distress and nega- tive affect, or just another expression for mental health (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2006; Griffiths, 2010). Griffiths (2010) concludes that even if there is an association between SOC strength and psychological dis- tress, this does not fully explain SOC, which comprises not only psy- chological distress, but also quality of life, social support, and person- ality factors (Griffiths, 2010). M. Eriksson and Lindström (2006) con- clude that SOC has affinities with mental health and mental well-being, 49

but that it is not the same as mental health. This discussion is influ- enced by the fact that different conceptions of theories of mental health exist.

The salutogenic theory is important for this thesis in two principal ways. The theory supports a general humanistic orientation in which the focus is on strengths and resources, and in which belief in the indi- vidual is important. The theory also provides a wider theoretical frame, and is suitable as an analytical tool that may provide a better under- standing of the concept of social support that is the focus of this thesis.

Social support In this section, a theory of social support is presented. Social elements are important aspects of nature-based services; therefore, social sup- port may serve as one of the theoretical underpinnings of such services.

The human need to be connected to other people is fundamental (Berk- man, Glass, Brissette, & Seeman, 2000). People live in a social context and are influenced by the social relationships in which they are en- gaged. Strong social connections with other people and social systems have been found to affect the mental health of adolescents (aged 12–15 years) (Foster et al., 2017). Connectedness is viewed as one of five fac- tors in personal recovery, and is represented by supportive relations in the social network (Leamy, Bird, Le Boutillier, Williams, & Slade, 2011). Others have argued that connectedness and social relations should be even more integrated in definitions and understandings of recovery, with a focus on relational, rather than personal, recovery (Price-Robertson, Obradovic, & Morgan, 2017).

The positive effects of social support on mental and physical health are well-documented (Berkman, 1995; Cobb, 1976; S. Cohen & Wills, 1985; Milner et al., 2016; Rueger et al., 2016; Uchino, 2004; Wang et al., 2017). Various explanatory models exist to help us understand the ef- fects of social support on human health, some of which are related to stress theory. The buffer hypothesis indicates that social support can protect against the negative consequences of stress, while the main-ef- fect hypothesis holds that social support is important regardless of stress levels. Evidence can be found in support of both of these theories 50

– that is, that social support is important regardless of stress levels, but that it is even more important during times of stress (Berkman, 1995; S. Cohen & Wills, 1985; Milner et al., 2016).

Different conceptions are used to describe social support and social re- lationships. Cobb (1976, p. 300) defines social support as ‘information leading the subject to believe that he is cared for and loved, esteemed and a member of a network of mutual obligations’. Common to all the different conceptions and definitions of social support is that social support is described as containing different dimensions, although the number of such dimensions and what they comprise varies (Cutrona & Russell, 1987; Wills, 1991). Some of the social support dimensions de- scribed are emotional support, informational support, instrumental support, and esteem support. Emotional support refers to relationships that offer acceptance, intimacy, and emotional sharing. Informational support refers to the advice or guidance one can receive from support- ive people. Instrumental support refers to help with, for example, goods or services. The fourth dimension, esteem support, represents the strengthening of a person’s sense of competence, for example, through positive feedback on her skills or belief in her coping abilities (Cutrona & Russell, 1987; Cutrona & Russell, 1990; Wills, 1991). Other components of social relationships, such as social integration, reflect- ing a person’s feeling of being part of a group that shares common in- terests and concerns, are also emphasised (Cutrona & Russell, 1990; Wills, 1991). Weiss (1974) emphasises the opportunities for nurturing, and hence giving support or care to others is an important dimension of social support. These benefits are, however, described as accruing through mechanisms that are somewhat different from the mecha- nisms that underlay the other dimensions of social support (Cutrona & Russell, 1990). A distinction often made is between social support and social integration, in terms of which social integration is described as ‘participation in a broad range of social relationships’ (Brissette, Cohen, & Seeman, 2000, p. 54). Hence, the different dimensions of so- cial support are often described as functional support, referring to what others may contribute to a person, both practically and emotionally, and structural support, which is defined as the level of connection a person feels within a social network (social integration) (S. Cohen &

51

Wills, 1985; Uchino, 2004; Wills, 1991; Wills & Shinar, 2000). Re- search confirms that social support in the context of functional support reduces the impact of stressful life events, such as unemployment or illness. Social integration, on the other hand, is related to well-being, but is not necessarily helpful in times of stress (Wills, 1991). There is also a distinction between perceived and received social support, in terms of which perceived support, the belief that support is available, exhibits the greater effect on mental health (S. Cohen & Wills, 1985).

Social support and mental health in young adults The relation between social support and mental health has been re- searched in a number of studies, and the positive effects of social sup- port on mental health are well documented (Caron, 2012; Cobb, 1976; S. Cohen & Wills, 1985; Rueger et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2017). Social support may be an important factor in managing the life changes that characterise the young-adult period, such as moving out of the parents’ home and completing education (Azmitia, Syed, & Radmacher, 2013). Recent research on the relation between mental health and social sup- port in adolescents and young adults (aged 15-25 years) indicates that lower degrees of social support are associated with more mental health problems (Van Droogenbroeck et al., 2018). Lower degrees of social support have been found to be related to suicide risk among adoles- cents (Eskin, 1995). Two meta-analytical studies conclude that social support is associated with positive mental health outcomes, such as fewer depressive symptoms and well-being in children and adolescents (Chu, Saucier, & Hafner, 2010; Rueger et al., 2016). The perceived quality of social support has been found to be more strongly associated with positive mental health outcomes than has the quantity of social support in young adults (aged 18 years)(Wang et al., 2017). Ramsdal, Bergvik, and Wynn (2018) studied the relation between long-term dis- continuation of school and work and mental health in young adults (aged 18-25 years) in Norway. They found that those who had dropped out told of less access to social support than those who had not. For those who had not dropped out, social support was described as playing a major role in their coping with mental health problems.

52

Social support in nature-based services Nature-based services can be assumed to have the potential to provide social support in a variety of ways. The social aspects have previously been described as underpinning the benefits that nature-based services can provide (Leck, Upton, & Evans, 2013). In previous research, the service leader, the other participants, and the animals have been de- scribed as potential providers of social support. These three potential sources of social support in nature-based services are briefly further described based on what has been found in previous research.

The service leader as a source of social support in nature-based ser- vices? The service leader in nature-based services is emphasised as having im- portant qualities related to social support and acceptance (Kogstad et al., 2014). Previous research has indicated that the farmer provides practical and emotional support (Elings & Hassink, 2008; Hassink et al., 2010; Pedersen, Ihlebæk, et al., 2012). In addition, a good relation between helper and user is outlined as being crucial for different types of recovery. Moreover, in social work, the qualities (like trust and re- spect) in the relation between the social worker and the user are highly emphasised (Levin & Ellingsen, 2015). However, the relations both be- tween the social worker and the user and between the service leader and the participant in nature-based services are asymmetrical rela- tions. The user is in some ways dependent on the social worker, as is the participant in nature-based services on the service leader. An awareness of potential implications of these asymmetric relations needs to exist when studying the relation between the service leader and participants in nature-based services.

The other participants as a source of social support in nature-based services? Research on nature-based services has emphasised the importance of participants sharing their thoughts, experiences, and feelings with oth- ers who have similar experiences and problems. The benefits of peer support and the sharing of problems and thoughts with people who have had similar experiences are well-documented (Oades, Deane, & Anderson, 2013). Social inclusion, group belonging, and community are qualities of the nature-based services that have been highlighted in 53

previous research (Elings & Hassink, 2008; Ellingsen-Dalskau, Berget, et al., 2016; Hine et al., 2008; Iancu et al., 2014).

Animals as a source of social support in nature-based services? The importance of animals in nature-based services has been described in previous research (e.g.Granerud & Eriksson, 2014; Hassink et al., 2010; Hine et al., 2008). A number of research studies use the term ‘social support’ when describing the qualities of human-animal inter- action (Leck et al., 2013; Meehan, Massavelli, & Pachana, 2017; Serpell, McCune, Gee, & Griffin, 2017). Use of the term in this context requires some clarification. The word ‘social’ is widely used and have several dif- ferent meanings that depend on the context in which it is used. It de- rives from Old French or Latin, socialis, ‘allied’, from socius ‘friend’, relating to society and humans as members of society (Social, 2018). Social support is therefore primarily used when describing the pres- ence or implication of support received from other human beings (Turner & Brown, 2010). However, the word social is also used in dis- ciplines like ethology (the study of animal behaviour), to describe ani- mal behaviour, in the form of interaction among animals – and for in- teraction between animals and human (Rousing & Wemelsfelder, 2006).

Describing qualities relating to the animal-human relation as support also requires some reflection. Saying that animals give support to hu- mans may imply that animal behaviour is an intentional act that meets a need in a human. Use of the concept of ‘support’ may therefore be problematic in this context. On the other hand, what is important is how the receiver perceive the animal’s behaviour. If the receiver expe- riences it as support, then the animal’s behaviour will have supportive effects – and that is what is relevant.

In this thesis, animals are seen as potentially important when studying social support because they may fulfil various needs in humans, needs that are related to different dimensions of social support– such as emo- tional support. This thesis therefore uses the concept of social support when describing animals’ effects on humans. However, in this thesis, I am aware of the challenges involved in using the concept of social sup- port when referring to the animal-human relation. Whether, and if so, 54

to what degree, animals may provide social support to humans requires further discussion, and is a theme that is important to explore in fur- ther research.

As described earlier, animals may provide support that leads to better social functioning in participants. Animals may also provide partici- pants with the opportunity to give care to other living beings, some- thing that has been described as important by the participants. Animals have also been described as social catalysts that facilitate communica- tion and social interaction between humans (McNicholas & Collis, 2006).

55

Methods

This thesis comprises four papers and, to address the overall aim, a combination of research methods is used. Paper I is an integrative lit- erature review. Paper II is a quantitative study focusing on participants’ in nature-based services suffering from mental health problems; Paper III is a methodological study that analyses the psychometric properties of the SPS; and Paper IV uses both quantitative and qualitative meth- ods to analyse different dimensions of social support in nature-based services in relation to participants’ SOC and mental health. Papers II– IV are based partly on the same data, a survey of participants in nature- based services. Papers II and IV also use other data in addition to data from this survey. Table 1 presents an overview of the methods used in this thesis.

Table 1. Methodological overview of papers I-IV

Paper Study sample Study design Analysis of data I Participants in Review of the ex- Qualitative thematic analysis nature-based ser- isting literature vices in the Nor- dic countries with mental health- or drug-related problems, or both II Young adults Cross-sectional Descriptive statistics, statisti- (aged 16–30 survey, HSCL-10 cal analysis of differences be- years) with men- tween groups tal health prob- lems participat- ing in nature- based services in Norway (n = 93)

General popula- tion of young Cross-sectional adults in Norway survey, nine (aged 18–19 items from years). HSCL-10 (n = 9459)

56

In-patients in a specialised psy- chiatric centre in Survey, HSCL-10 Norway (aged 18–30 years). (n = 31) Young adults Cross sectional Psychometric analysis of SPS- III (aged 16–30 survey, SPS-10 10 using Rasch analysis years) with men- tal health prob- lems participat- ing in nature- based services in Norway (n = 93)

IV Young adults Cross-sectional Descriptive statistics and sta- (aged 16–30 survey tistical analysis; correlations years) with men- tal health prob- Qualitative inter- Qualitative content analysis lems participat- views ing in nature- based services in Norway (n = 93)

The survey of participants in nature-based services was conducted as part of an umbrella project named Recovery-Oriented Green Care Ser- vices (ROGCS)2. The qualitative data used in Paper IV were also col- lected as part of the ROGCS project.

2 The ROGCS project was financed by the Research Council in Norway and aimed at doc- umenting the potential beneficial health effects of nature-based services and the practical implications of this knowledge. The project comprised several sub-projects that applied both qualitative and quantitative methods. The results from these sub-projects are reported in Granerud and Eriksson (2014), Kogstad et al. (2014), Lund, Granerud, and Eriksson (2015), and Hummelvoll et al. (2012). In addition, a handbook for buyers of nature-based services in Norway was developed as part of the project (Kogstad et al., 2012).

57

Methods for Paper I Paper I is an integrative literature review, the aim of which is to present an overview of the literature on nature-based services (Green Care) in Nordic countries. It was considered important to collect and classify existing knowledge of nature-based services as the first part of this the- sis. The paper follows the method of an integrative review as described by Whittemore and Knafl (2005). The integrative review allows for in- clusion of both qualitative and quantitative data, as well as different types of literature (e.g., research articles, reports, etc.) (Whittemore & Knafl, 2005).

Inclusion criteria The focus of the literature review is studies of nature-based services for people who have discontinued work or school, and who have mental health- or drug-related problems, or both. The literature review is lim- ited to the Nordic countries: Norway, Sweden, Denmark, Finland, and Iceland. This limitation was applied in order to make it possible to an- alyse in greater depth different types of literature, for example, reports and evaluations. As the number of studies published in academic jour- nals is limited, it was of great interest to include more literature than merely studies in the form of peer-reviewed research papers.

The paper is based on the literature identified by means of systematic searches of relevant databases. The paper does not necessarily reflect the actual number or types of nature-based services in Nordic coun- tries, but rather reflects the literature published. The literature in- cluded should have been published as reports or evaluations, with a registration number or some other form of verification data, such as scientific articles, doctoral theses, and books or book chapters.

Literature search The literature search covers the period from 1995 to April 2004. The search terms used were ‘green care’, ‘care farm*’, ‘farming for health’, social farm*’, ‘therapeutic horticulture’, ‘animal-assisted*’, ‘horticul- ture therapy’ and ‘gardening’. These search terms were combined with ‘mental health’, ‘recovery’, ‘resilience’, and ‘salutogenesis’; however, for these additional terms, important literature was not included in the re- sults of the searches. In addition, Norwegian, Swedish, and Danish 58

words were used in the searches. For Finland and Iceland, the searches were limited to English words. Broad searches of several databases were conducted in order to reveal where interesting literature could be found. Furthermore, some databases were selected as the most rele- vant: PsychInfo, Academic Search Premier, Cumulative Index to Nurs- ing and Allied Health Literature, Medline, ISI Web of Science, Scopus, and Google Scholar. Searches of library databases in Norway, Sweden, and Denmark were also conducted, along with searches of other rele- vant web pages in these countries.

Quality assessment Related to the aim of this literature review some quality criteria were established. These were not based on any specific model of quality as- sessment, but rather on general scientific criteria relevant to our re- search field. Did the results appear to be trustworthy in terms of their description of the methods and data presentation? Could the results have transferrable value and be compared with other studies? Did the literature thematise our research questions? Were ethical aspects such as anonymity, informed consent, autonomy, and the researchers’ qual- ifications described? Was the literature relevant to nature-based ser- vices (Green Care) and did it inform us about how the services worked? We found it relevant to look more closely at the quality of the literature included only if the results deviated from the rest of the findings, in order to rule out the possibility that these deviating results were due to poor methodological quality (Whittemore & Knafl, 2005).

Analysis The literature included in the review was analysed following the steps presented by Whittemore and Knafl (2005). The literature was sum- marised, and different models of nature-assisted services were de- scribed, along with reasons for the use of these different models. Fi- nally, the results and effects obtained from the various literature sources were analysed and categorised (Whittemore & Knafl, 2005).

Description of literature included Based on the searches and after reading and assessing the literature, 18 articles and seven reports or evaluations were included. The literature

59

originated in Norway, Sweden, Denmark, and Finland, with most of it coming from Norway.

Methods for Paper II The aim of Paper II is to examine the mental health problems of young adults participating in nature-based services using HSCL-10. The HSCL scores were compared with the HSCL scores of both a clinical and general population sample of young adults. The paper therefore uses three different data sets. These are described in the next para- graph.

Population and data collection – the nature-based services survey Paper II uses data from a survey of young adults aged 16 to 30 who participated in nature-based services in Norway. They had discontin- ued school or work (partly or totally) due to mental health- or drug- related problems, or both. Participants were recruited from all parts of Norway, and nature-based services were mapped through NAV, spe- cial-interest organisations, Matmerk (responsible for the ‘Into the farmyard’ authorisation), and through internet searches for places of- fering nature-based services. Approximately 600 services were con- tacted by phone to ascertain whether they had participants that met our inclusion criteria. Of these, 148 nature-based services reported that they had potential informants for our study. The recruitment of partic- ipants was conducted through third parties, that is, service leaders or NAV leaders. The services mostly provided 1 to 10 participants each, with two services providing 20 participants each. Many of the services reported that they had, for example, 10 places at the service, but only five participants at that time. Questionnaires of a number appropriate to the number of participants that the services reported to be available were distributed. In addition, questionnaires for potential new partici- pants were also distributed. In order to keep track of the response rate, the services’ leaders were sent a form on which they could indicate how many participants they had informed of the study. Unfortunately, many of the leaders did not return the forms; hence, response rates could not be calculated in this manner.

60

In all, 47 questionnaires were returned after this first phase of data col- lection. After almost a year, a reminder letter was sent to all 148 ser- vices, and another 10 questionnaires were returned thereafter. There were several reasons for waiting almost a year before sending out the first reminder: practical challenges within the research group, and the consideration that by waiting it may have been the case that new par- ticipants had started in the services and therefore that more partici- pants would potentially have the opportunity to be involved in the study. In June 2014, it was decided to extend the data-collection period for the rest of the year. By August 2014, all 148 services were contacted by phone again to determine whether they still had potential inform- ants. Some of the services did not have any new participants, and many said that after receiving the questionnaires and the information letters that followed these, they had assessed that they did not have partici- pants in the population of interest for the study. Therefore, only 48 ser- vices received questionnaires following these calls, these services had from 1 to 10 participants. This resulted in 23 more questionnaires being returned. In December 2014, a reminder letter was sent out to these 48 services. The letter informed potential participants of the possibility of answering the questionnaire in a structured interview. At the beginning of 2015, there was ongoing contact with the services that had already been contacted, and two new services were added. The first author vis- ited two of the services, and four participants responded to the ques- tionnaire in structured interviews. Following this, 13 more informants answered the questionnaire, including the four who had been inter- viewed, and the survey ended up with a total of 93 participants. In all, 13 of the questionnaires that were received could not be used because the participants either were too old (older than 30 years of age) or too young (younger than 16 years of age) (see Figure 1: Flowchart of the data collection process). The participants came from 16 of the 19 coun- ties in Norway.

61

Figure 1. Flow chart of the data collection process

Response rates Ninety-three informants responded to the questionnaire and hence participated in this study. Due to problems in estimating the actual size of our population, it is difficult to estimate an exact response rate. Based on the data obtained during the data-collection period and from 62

talking to service leaders, the population is likely to be 150-200 people in Norway as a whole.

The questionnaire in the nature-based services survey The questionnaire consisted of three parts. The first set of questions focused on the participants, while a second set referred to the nature- based services they participated in. The last part consisted of three standardised instruments relating to mental health problems, social support, and SOC (appendix 1). The questionnaire was pilot tested by two participants in nature-based services. The participants completed the questionnaire with a researcher present, they discussed the ques- tions, and, following this, some changes were made to the words used, the response categories, and layout.

Hopkins Symptom Checklist-10 The Hopkins Symptom Checklist 10 is a short version of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL-25), which measures symptoms of depres- sion and anxiety (Derogatis, Lipman, Richels, Uhlenhuth, & Covi, 1974; Strand, Dalgard, Tambs, & Rognerud, 2003). The questionnaire con- tains 10 items with four response categories: not troubled (1); slightly troubled (2); much troubled (3); and very much troubled (4). A mean value based on the 10 questions can be computed, with a cut-off value of 1.85 (Strand et al., 2003); that is, those with mean scores above 1.85 have symptoms of anxiety and depression. A Rasch analysis of the HSCL-10 was conducted and the scale showed satisfying psychometric properties, with a person-separation index of 0.869.

Social Provisions Scale (SPS) The SPS is an instrument that measures a person’s perceived social support. The scale is based on Weiss (1974) theory of social provisions and was further developed by Cutrona and Russell (1987). The idea of the SPS is to measure a person’s social provisions related to different dimensions of social support (Cutrona & Russell, 1987). Different ver- sions of the SPS are used, with the 24- and 16-item versions being the most common (Bondevik, 1996; Caron, Tempier, Mercier, & Leouffre, 1998; Cutrona & Russell, 1987). A 10-item version of SPS is also used (Caron, 2013). The results of studies using a factor-analytical classical test-theory approach indicated solid validity and reliability (Caron et 63

al., 1998; Cutrona & Russell, 1987). In associated research, a wide range of scales attempting to measure social support and social provi- sions can be observed (Heitzmann & Kaplan, 1988). One of these is the SPS (Cutrona & Russell, 1987).

In this study, the 10-item version of the SPS was used, although the participants answered all the questions in the 24-item version. The SPS-10 comprises 10 items representing five dimensions, with each item having four response categories. The five dimensions are attach- ment, social integration, reassurance of worth, sense of reliable alli- ance, and obtainment of guidance. The dimensions represent different parts of what may be obtained from relationships with others (Caron, 2013; Cutrona & Russell, 1987). The original SPS had six dimensions, and Weiss (1974) describes all six as being needed for a person to per- ceive adequate social support; however, some of the dimensions may be more important in different circumstances or phases of a person’s life. The opportunity for nurturance dimension is not included in the SPS-10 version (Caron, 2013).

The total SPS score is used in a range of studies (Caron, 2013; Caron et al., 1998; Cutrona & Russell, 1987; Dale, Sævareid, Kirkevold, & Söderhamn, 2010; Langeland & Wahl, 2009). However, use of the 10- item version (SPS-10) and its total score is still rare compared with other versions. The total SPS score is formed by the summation of raw scores using the following response format: strongly agree (4), agree (3), disagree (2), and strongly disagree (1), depending on the wording of the item. A high score indicates a high degree of social provisions (Cutrona & Russell, 1987). The results of studies of the psychometric properties of the total score versions of the SPS are mixed when it con- cerns the number of dimensions suggested (Bondevik, 1996; Caron et al., 1998; Cutrona & Russell, 1987; Mancini & Blieszner, 1992). Good validity and reliability are reported for the total SPS-10 in a study using factor analytical approaches, with Cronbach’s Alpha reported as 0.880 (Caron, 2013).

Sense of coherence (SOC-13) scale The original SOC scale, called the Orientation to Life Questionnaire, was developed by Antonovsky (1987) and consists of 29 items. A short 64

version of the scale consisting of 13 of the original 29 items also is used (Antonovsky, 1987). The 13-item version of the scale was used in this study. The scale comprises items covering all three sub-concepts of SOC: manageability, meaningfulness, and comprehensibility. The re- sponse alternatives range from 1 to 7 for both versions of the scale, with 1 and 7 representing extreme feelings about the questions or state- ments. The total score for the scale is obtained by summing up the raw scores from each of the questions. For the SOC-13, this means that the lowest score available is 13 and the highest is 91 (Antonovsky, 1987). Antonovsky (1987) did not recommend using scores obtained by sum- mating the items belonging to each of the three different sub-concepts of the scale separately; rather he emphasised that it is the total SOC score that is of interest (Antonovsky, 1987; M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2005).

The SOC questionnaires (SOC-29 and the SOC-13) are widely used in- struments globally, and the scales are found in a range of different lan- guages (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2005). A systematic review con- cludes that the SOC scale appears to be a reliable and valid instrument (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2005). A more recent study evaluating the psychometric qualities of the SOC-13 scale using classical test-theory also concludes that the instrument is generally reliable and valid (Rajesh et al., 2016). However, problems related to the construct valid- ity of the SOC scales are found in a range of studies, with recent studies suggesting that the scales seem to be multidimensional rather than uni- dimensional (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2005; M. Eriksson & Mittelmark, 2017). Cronbach’s Alpha ranges from 0.70 to 0.92 in stud- ies using the SOC-13 (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2005). The correlation between the SOC questionnaires and measures of various health as- pects, such as quality of life, well-being, and stressors, range from slight to good (M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2005). More recently, the psycho- metric qualities of the SOC-13 when used in various populations has been evaluated using modern test theory (Rasch analysis) (Hagquist & Andrich, 2004; Holmefur, Sundberg, Wettergren, & Langius-Eklöf, 2015; Lerdal, Fagermoen, Bonsaksen, Gay, & Kottorp, 2014; Lerdal et al., 2017). The results indicate that the rating scale seems to represent a major challenge to the SOC-13 scale (Holmefur et al., 2015; Lerdal et al., 2014; Lerdal et al., 2017). Lerdal et al. (2017) concludes that the 65

SOC-13 does not meet the criteria for validity and reliability in various samples.

Description of the clinical sample The clinical sample comprised 31 people aged 18-30 years who were recruited from a specialised psychiatric centre in Norway. The psychi- atric centre treats patients with long-standing or treatment-resistant trauma, anxiety, eating, and depressive disorders. Data from this sam- ple were collected at the start of a 12-week intervention programme in 2015 and 2016. The data were collected as part of a major study focus- ing on the neuroimmunology of depression and alcohol-use disorders (the DARCY studies) (Toft et al., 2018). The clinical sample responded to the symptom checklist-90-revised (SCL-90-R); the 10 items from the SCL-90-R which correspond to the HSCL-10 questions is used in this thesis (Derogatis, 2008, 2010; Siqveland, Moum, & Leiknes, 2016; Strand et al., 2003). The SCL-90-R has five response categories: not at all (0), slightly (1), medium (2), much (3), and very much (4) (Derogatis, 2010). To allow for a comparison of mean values across the samples, the response categories slightly (1) and medium (2) were col- lapsed, and response categories recoded so that they corresponded to the HSCL-10 in the nature-based sample. Cronbach’s Alpha was 0.869 for the scale with five response categories and 0.872 for that with four response categories. The correlation between the scales with four and five response categories was high, with Pearson’s r = 0.993.

Description of the general population sample The general population sample was sourced from Ungdata (Data on young people). Ungdata is an annual cross-national data-collection scheme conducted by NOVA (Norwegian Social Research) in coopera- tion with regional centres for drug rehabilitation (KoRus). Ungdata is conducted at the municipal level in Norway. The Norwegian Direc- torate of Health, the Ministry of Children, Equality and Social Inclu- sion, and the Ministry of Justice and Public Security finance Ungdata (NOVA, 2016). Results from Ungdata collection in 2014 from young adults in upper secondary school (aged 18–19 years) from throughout Norway were used in this study. Only respondents who responded to all the HSCL questions were included, resulting in a sample of 9,459.

66

More information about Ungdata 2014 is found in a NOVA (2014) re- port. Ungdata has nine items which correspond to nine of the HSCL-10 questions, which were used to compare the Ungdata sample (the gen- eral population sample) with the clinical and nature-based service sam- ple. The statement ‘feeling useless, of little worth’ was not found in the Ungdata sample.

Analysis of data Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 24 software. The analy- sis included descriptive statistics, analysis of the differences in mean values using Mann-Whitney and Kruskal-Wallis tests, and z-tests to calculate differences in proportions. The significance level was set at p < 0.05 for all analyses.

Methods for Paper III

Paper III analyses the psychometric properties of the SPS-10 by means of the polytomous Rasch model (Rasch, 1980). It was considered im- portant to ensure that SPS-10 was suitable for our population and to obtain more knowledge about its strengths and weaknesses.

Population and data collection

Paper III uses data from the survey of young adults with mental health problems participating in nature-based services described above. SPS- 10 was one of the three standardised instruments included in the ques- tionnaire used in this survey.

Analysis of data

The psychometric properties of the SPS-10 were analysed using the pol- ytomous Rasch model (Andrich, 1978; Rasch, 1980). The Rasch model is a mathematical model based on the work of the Danish mathemati- cian, Georg Rasch (1980). It can be used to determine whether items in a set can be used together to access one single latent trait. In its general form, the Rasch model is a dichotomous model, which implies a re- sponse format with only two categories. The polytomous model is an extension of the dichotomous model as it implies a response format with more than two categories (Andrich, 1978). Because SPS-10 has items with four response categories, the polytomous Rasch model is

67

applied in this study. There is no single test that can tell us if our data fit the Rasch model and, hence, multiple tests are therefore conducted. Several of the tests can be analysed both statistically and graphically.

Rasch analysis can indicate how well the SPS-10 captures social provi- sions in a specific population, which is referred to as targeting (Pallant & Tennant, 2007). In addition, the analysis provides information on whether response categories work as intended (Hagquist, Bruce, & Gustavsson, 2009; Pallant & Tennant, 2007). Furthermore, analysis of item fit provides information on how well individual items work in re- lation to other items, as well as on where potential problems are located (Hagquist et al., 2009).

A central requirement for the Rasch model is that an instrument should work in the same way for all persons; this is referred to as the criterion of invariance of measurement (Tennant & Conaghan, 2007, Hagquist et al., 2009). The Rasch analysis provides information on whether an item is working differently between persons due to factors other than perceived social provisions, (e.g. gender or age); this is referred to as differential item functioning (DIF) (Hagquist & Andrich, 2004; Ten- nant & Conaghan, 2007).

One of the general tests in Rasch analysis is the Person separation in- dex (PSI), a reliability indicium analogous to Cronbach’s Alpha (Cronbach, 1951). A high PSI value means high reliability, indicating that the instrument is able to separate the individuals well (Hagquist et al., 2009). The Rasch model also tests whether items are independent of each other and whether correlations between the items are part of the latent trait. If this assumption is not met, it can reflect either mul- tidimensionality or response dependence (Marais & Andrich, 2008; Tennant & Conaghan, 2007). Scales with different sub-dimensions in- tended to measure one global factor is a potential source of multidi- mensionality (Hagquist et al., 2009). As the SPS-10 can be used both as a total score of social provisions and as separate dimensions, anal- yses of multidimensionality are of particular interest. Thus, it is possi- ble to rule out whether it is appropriate to use the total SPS-10 score.

68

All psychometric analyses were conducted using RUMM2030 software (Andrich, Sheridan, & Luo, 2013).

Methods for Paper IV

Paper IV uses both quantitative data and qualitative data to study dif- ferent dimensions of social support in nature-based services in relation to participants’ SOC and mental health.

Population and data collection

Paper IV uses data from the survey of young adults with mental health problems participating in nature-based services in Norway. This survey is described above. Paper IV also uses qualitative data from the same population.

Qualitative data Two researchers from the ROGCS project collected the qualitative data used in Paper IV. The participants were young adults who had discon- tinued school or work, who were struggling with mental health- or drug-related problems, and who participated in nature-based services. Participants were recruited using the same procedure as for the survey. Potential services were mapped and participants were recruited through service leaders. Participants were recruited from two counties in the eastern and southern regions of Norway.

The qualitative interviews were conducted at the nature-based services or at another location chosen by the participants. The data were col- lected in 2012 and 2013. All the interviews followed a semi-structured interview guide about participants’ experiences in the services (appen- dix 2). The interview-guide included questions about expectations, the relationship with the service leader and the other participants, and about animals and nature. The qualitative data were collected as part of a process study, and the data include 20 separate interviews with nine participants. Two of the participants were interviewed once, four were interviewed twice, two were interviewed three times, and one was interviewed four times. The intervals between interviews ranged from three months to almost 18 months. The participants had been in the

69

services for between six weeks and five years at the time of the first in- terview. The sample consisted of eight females and one male between the ages of 19 and 26.

Analysis The qualitative data were analysed using qualitative content analysis based on Graneheim and Lundman (2004). The interviews were read through several times. Meaning units were extracted and later con- densed. The condensed meaning units were further coded and codes were sorted and arranged into sub-categories. The sub-categories were later grouped and three main categories emerged. Graneheim and Lundman (2004) emphasise that such analysis not is a linear process and that the researcher needs to move back and forth between the steps in the process.

Validity, reliability, and generalisability To question the quality of data is central to all scientific research in or- der to evaluate whether results are valid, reliable, and the extent to which they can be generalised. Validity concerns whether the data measures what they intend to measure, for example, whether a scale intended to measure social support really measures social support. Re- liability concerns the precision of the data collection; hence, whether repeated measurements using the same instrument produce the same results. Furthermore, strong reliability is a requirement for strong va- lidity. Generalisability concerns whether the results are valid for popu- lations other than the one that is studied (Kvale, 1997; Ringdal, 2018). The concepts of validity, reliability, and generalisability have tradition- ally been associated with the quantitative research tradition; however, the concepts are also used in qualitative research (Graneheim & Lundman, 2004). There are different opinions on whether this is ap- propriate or not (Kvale, 1997), and some argue that the concepts have the same essential meaning irrespective of the research tradition. Hence, nothing is gained by changing labels (Long & Johnson, 2000). Graneheim and Lundman (2004) suggest the use of concepts linked to the qualitative research tradition, such as credibility, dependability, and transferability, when assessing quality of data, or results from studies using qualitative content analysis. Credibility concerns whether 70

the data and analysis address the intended focus (Polit & Hungler, 1999). Dependability concerns whether data are consistent and repeat- able (Lincoln & Guba, 1985), while transferability concerns whether the findings can be transferred to other populations or settings (Polit & Hungler, 1999).

To ensure the validity, reliability, and generalisability of the data in this thesis, several aspects were taken into account. First, to strengthen the validity and reliability of the data (paper II, III and IV), all nature- based services in Norway were thoroughly mapped to ensure that all services had been invited to participate; hence, that there were no spe- cific types of services that had not been included. Furthermore, the ser- vice leaders received information letters with inclusion criteria to en- sure that all participants in the population of interest were included, and that participants outside this population were excluded. Through knowledge of previous research studies and when mapping the nature- based services, we obtained indications that some of the participants had reading and writing difficulties or struggled with concentration problems. The participants were therefore offered the opportunity to answer the questionnaire in the course of a structured telephone inter- view in order to ensure that no participant groups systematically chose not to participate, something that could have affected the validity of the data. To track the response rate, all service leaders were asked to com- plete a form reporting how many participants they had provided with information about the study. Response rates are relevant for discussing the generalisability of data. The psychometric properties of the stand- ardised scales used in this study were analysed using Rasch analysis, which provided information on the validity and reliability of these scales. Furthermore, the questionnaires were pilot tested among par- ticipants in nature-based services in order to ensure that the question- naires were understandable. Minor language clarifications and some layout editing were effected following the pilot testing.

To ensure the credibility and dependability of the qualitative data (pa- per IV), two researchers were involved in analysing the material, and hence could validate the findings in relation to the transcribed texts. The interview guide consisted of open questions that were not leading, and the researchers used easy and understandable language. To ensure 71

that the reader can assess the transferability of the findings, the whole process from data collection to analysis is thoroughly described.

To ensure validity and reliability in the course of the review (paper I), each step thereof is thoroughly described, including having clear re- search questions, clearly described inclusion and exclusion criteria, and clear descriptions of the search strategy and selection of papers. The quality of papers included was assessed, analysis of data are clearly described, and potential limitations of the study are highlighted.

Ethical considerations The Norwegian Regional Ethics Committee for Southeast Norway (REK) approved the survey of participants in nature-based services (2012/372). Participants were informed that participation was volun- tary, that they could remain anonymous, and that it would be impossi- ble to identify them in the results. Participants had to provide written consent in a form that they returned with the questionnaire. Many questionnaires were returned without written consent, and we learned from the service leaders that participants did not want to provide con- sent because it required that they reveal their names. REK then ruled that the returned questionnaires provided implicit consent, and that signed consent forms were not necessary. Furthermore, after learning that many participants struggled with reading or writing difficulties and concentration problems, we provided participants with the option of answering the questionnaire by phone; however, no one chose this option. Some participants accepted our offer of structured interviews instead of self-administration of the questionnaire. Those who an- swered the questionnaire in a structured interview had to sign written consent forms.

The original ambition was to collect data from several time points from the same participants; however, due to unexpected problems with the data collection, and because fewer people participated in the services than was expected, the procedures for data collection were revised. Originally, we wanted to collect data at three time points from partici- pants who had been in the services for three months or less. We later changed this so that all participants could choose to answer the ques-

72

tionnaire a second time. Nine second-time questionnaires were re- turned, which was too few for the data to be used. Those who agreed to a second-time questionnaire did so at the end of the first questionnaire and provided their names and addresses.

The recruitment of participants was undertaken by third parties, that is, service leaders or NAV leaders. There may have been some ethical issues with this relating to participants feeling obliged to participate, or to answer in a specific way to please the service leaders. We reduced this risk by letting each participant return the questionnaire in a pre- paid envelope. This also made it possible for participants to answer the questionnaire at home.

REK also approved the clinical study (2014/2189) from which Paper II draws data. All the participants in this study received both verbal and written information about the study and had to sign written consent forms.

The Norwegian Social Science Data Services (NSD) approved the Ungdata study, from which Paper II draws data. Ungdata is an anony- mous survey and participation is voluntary (NOVA, 2016).

NSD also approved the qualitative study from which Paper IV draws data. The participants in the qualitative study were recruited through third parties, that is, service leaders, as with the aforementioned ques- tionnaires. All participants signed a written consent form and were in- formed that participation was voluntary, that they could withdraw from the study at any time, and that they could not be identified in the re- sults.

73

Results

The main results from the four studies are presented below.

Results for Paper I The searches resulted in inclusion of 25 papers in the review. Eighteen of these are scientific articles and seven are reports or evaluations. The majority of the literature came from Norway. The literature could be grouped according to different models of nature-based services (Green Care), namely farm work, animal-assisted interventions, nature- and forest-based activities, and gardening and horticulture. Across the dif- ferent models, no clear differences were found between the described effects or results for participants. Not all the papers describe the ex- plicit grounds for the use of a particular model.

Seven main categories describing the effects of the services were found in the literature: mastery and coping, positive effects on mental health, physical activity, structure and meaningfulness, the feeling of dignity produced by performing a decent ordinary job, social gains, and ani- mals and nature.

An unclear division between intervention factors and effects was found in the literature, and a clearer division between these is suggested. The active mechanisms in nature-based (Green Care) services can be de- scribed as: contact with animals, supportive natural environments, the service leader as a significant and important other, social acceptance of and fellowship with the other participants, and meaningful and indi- vidually adapted activities in which mastery can be experienced. These interacted in a holistic manner, with the synergetic effects extending the sum of the active mechanisms.

Results for Paper II The nature-based service sample consisted of 56% female and 44% male participants. Half the participants aged 23 years or older had not completed upper secondary school. Most of the participants were re- cruited through NAV (71.4%), with smaller proportions found through local mental health services (14.3%), schools (9.9%), or other entities,

74

such as child welfare (4.4%). Mental health problems were reported as a contributing reason for participation by 77.5% and drug-related prob- lems as a contributing reason by 11.1%. Almost everyone reporting drug-related problems as a contributing reason also reported mental health problems as a contributing reason. Many participants had posi- tive or no expectations before participating in services (64.1%), while about one third (35.9%) had negative or mixed expectations. The dura- tion of participation at the time of data collection varied from one week to more than 12 months, and how much time participants spent at the farm varied. The mean score on HSCL-10 among participants was 20.88 (SD = 8.46), and just over half the participants scored above the cut-off of 1.85, indicating they had symptoms of mental health prob- lems.

Almost all participants in nature-based services reported being pleased or very pleased with the services, regardless of their expectations before starting or whether they had symptoms of mental health problems.

No differences in the symptoms of mental health problems were found among participants recruited from NAV and those recruited by local mental health services. However, significant differences in symptoms of mental health problems were found among participants recruited from NAV or local mental health services, and those recruited from schools. No significant differences in symptoms of mental health prob- lems were found that related to the duration of the participation in the services.

Participants in nature-based services generally had higher scores for symptoms of mental health problems than the general population sam- ple and lower scores than the clinical sample. Nevertheless, it is diffi- cult to obtain a congruent picture of the nature-based services sample in relation to the other samples. The variation in duration of participa- tion in the nature-based services sample at the time of data collection compared with the clinical sample may have affected differences be- tween the samples. Comparing the standard deviation (SD) in the clin- ical and nature-based samples, we found that the latter had a higher SD, indicating broader variation in this sample. In addition, the SD in- forms us that these two samples overlapped, that is, some participants 75

in nature-based services had the same HSCL-10 scores as some of the participants in the clinical sample, and vice versa.

Results for Paper III The psychometric analysis concluded that there were problems with the SPS-10 relating to targeting and construct validity. Six of the items were problematic with regard to item-threshold ordering, which may be related to the problems with targeting. As the threshold ordering was a severe problem, two of the response categories were collapsed. After collapsing response categories, the PSI value was slightly higher than beforehand.

According to the statistical test of fit conducted, several items misfit the model; this was confirmed when their graphical presentations were re- viewed. Based on theoretical assumptions and conceptual considera- tions about the concept of social support, two of the items were re- moved in two steps. These assumptions and considerations were grounded in theory about different dimensions of social support, in terms of which it was assumed that the two items that were removed represent a social support dimension different from dimensions meas- ured by the other items. This also corresponded with the results of the statistical tests of fit. The two items that were removed both belonged to the predefined social support dimension of social integration. Once the two items had been removed, the scale showed satisfying psycho- metric properties. No items misfit the model, no items had disordered thresholds, and no DIF was present for age or gender. The PSI value for the new scale was 0.692, and targeting was also improved. The modified SPS (SPS-8) can be described as measuring perceived func- tional support, while the items removed measured the experience of having a social fellowship with others (structural support).

Results for Paper IV Nature-based services appear to provide emotional, esteem, informa- tional, and instrumental support to their participants, as well as social integration and opportunities for nurturance. These dimensions seem

76

to be provided by the service leader, the other participants, and the an- imals. The association between social support and SOC is significant, meaning that high perceived social support among the participants also means that they have a strong SOC. Furthermore, the association be- tween SOC and mental health is even stronger. Moreover, participants described improvement in their mental health. No significant associa- tion was found between social support (SPS-8) and mental health (HSCL-10).

The results indicate that animals may be of especial importance for par- ticipants with low SOC scores. In addition, animals may play an im- portant role for participants who are socially vulnerable. The potential special importance of animals for specific groups of participants in na- ture-based services requires further research. The results indicate that social insecurity and vulnerability were present for many of the partic- ipants; hence, nature-based services may play an important role in strengthening participants’ social safety.

77

Discussion

As presented in the introduction, the overall aim of this thesis is to ex- amine nature-based services for young adults with mental health prob- lems, focusing on the meaning of various dimensions of social support. The discussion is based on the overall and specific aims, which for the reader’s convenience, are repeated here:

I: To present an overview of the literature on Green Care (nature-based services) in the Nordic countries in relation to people out of work or school, or with mental or drug-related problems, or both.

Paper II: To examine mental health problems among young adults par- ticipating in nature-based services in Norway.

Paper III: To examine the psychometric properties of the total Social Provisions Scale 10-item version (SPS-10) by means of the polytomous Rasch model, applied to Norwegian data on young adults at risk of mar- ginalisation participating in nature-based services.

Paper IV: To uncover dimensions of social support in relation to sense of coherence (SOC) and mental health among young adults with mental health problems, participating in nature-based services.

The discussion is organized in two parts based on the overall aim. The first part addresses nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems. The second part addresses the meaning of dimen- sions of social support in nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems.

The first part includes findings from the literature review with a focus on nature-based services as a recent practice. It considers how social support has been studied in previous research. It describes the contri- bution this thesis makes to the field of nature-based services as an arena for social support (referring to paper I). Further, mental health problems among young adults in nature-based services are discussed, and the importance of knowledge in this area (referring to papers II and IV).

78

The second part of the discussion focuses on dimensions of social sup- port. Specifically, the psychometric properties of SPS-10 (referring to paper III) and the dimensions of the social support in nature-based ser- vices are outlined and discussed (referring to paper IV).

Finally, methodological considerations are presented.

Nature-based services for young adults with mental health prob- lems Insights from previous research on nature-based services and knowledge about the participants in these services is a basis for con- ducting research on the role of social support in these services. In the following section, contributions of the current study are discussed in the context of previous research.

Previous literature and research papers on nature-based services (focusing on the meaning of social support) The literature review (paper I) shows that the field of nature-based ser- vices is evolving in all the Nordic countries. There is great diversity in the number and types of services offered. It is evident that much of the published literature originates from research interventions. There is little literature on nature-based services that exist independently of re- search projects. In Norway, nature-based services in farm milieus seem to be the most widespread type. The nature-based services described in the literature were found to provide positive activities for people with mental health or drug-related problems. These findings strengthen the rationale for this thesis. The literature review indicates seven main cat- egories of effects and results for participants in nature-based services. An unclear division was found between intervention factors and effects in the literature.

Nature-based services - a recent practice and research field The literature review illustrates that the field of nature-based services is relatively young in terms of documentation of practice and research. Recently, a number of international reviews focusing on various as- pects of nature-based services have been conducted (Artz & Davis, 2017; Gorman & Cacciatore, 2017; Iancu et al., 2015; Salomon et al., 79

2017). This indicates that interest in nature-based services is growing. More researchers are investigating this phenomenon and its potential for various groups of participants. Paper I describes nature-based ser- vices in the Nordic countries in general. One of the objectives is to iden- tify models for nature-based services in the Nordic countries. Conse- quently, a broad variety of nature-based services is described (e.g. in farm milieus, therapeutic gardens, farm animal-assisted interven- tions). The other papers in this thesis focus specifically on nature-based services in farm milieus. We find there is no clear division between the described impacts on participants in different models of nature-based services. This indicates that although some models of nature-based ser- vices are better suited for some groups of participants, there are com- mon features of these services, resulting in similar results or effects for participants.

Social support in previous research studies The literature review substantiates that no previous literature or re- search in the Nordic countries focuses specifically on social support in nature-based services. Social support is not an explicit aspect of the various nature-based service models and investigating social support has not been an explicit part of the research designs. Research studies presented earlier in this thesis confirm that this is still the case, inter- nationally as well. However, it is evident that there is awareness about the potentially important role of social support in these services (Leck et al., 2013). In addition, the results of the literature review (paper I), paper 4, and other recent research studies all reflect that the social as- pect is important in nature-based services (e.g. Granerud & Eriksson, 2014). The social aspect has been included in recent study designs, such as use of scales to measure social support (Ellingsen-Dalskau, Berget, et al., 2016). One of the primary categories noted in the literature re- view is that of social gains. Social gains include ‘the farmer as a signifi- cant important other’, ‘social support’ and ‘a tendency towards in- creased social activity after intervention’. The social aspect in nature- based services has been described in previous qualitative research studies as: being social with other people (Hine et al., 2008), belonging to a group (Elings & Hassink, 2008), the community at the farm (Hassink et al., 2010), community and belonging at the services

80

(Granerud & Eriksson, 2014), facilitation of social inclusion, and com- munity integration (Iancu et al., 2014). A recent quantitative research study from Norway finds that important social aspects include a sense of group belonging and social support from the farmer (Ellingsen- Dalskau, Berget, et al., 2016). Ellingsen-Dalskau, Berget, et al. (2016) describe social aspects as one of the central factors for participants’ needs to be satisfied by the services.

Contribution of the current study in the context of previous research As discussed, social support and social aspects are potentially among the central factors in nature-based services. This is evident from the literature review (paper I) and supported by the research review in this thesis summary. However, social support has not been specifically studied in recent research. Knowledge about dimensions and meaning of social support in these services remains limited. In that respect, this thesis provides a valuable contribution.

The literature review finds that the potentially positive results of par- ticipation in nature-based services are, to a large extent, related to or justified by theories about why nature and animals are beneficial for humans. These theories are mainly derived from evolutionary psychol- ogy (e.g. the attention restoration theory (ART) and biophilia theory). Some previous studies lack an overall theoretical perspective. Others refer to the self-determination theory to understand nature-based ser- vices (Ellingsen-Dalskau, 2017; Ellingsen-Dalskau, Berget, et al., 2016; Ellingsen-Dalskau, Morken, et al., 2016). Still other studies use a theo- retical foundation comprised of elements from various theories, such as the supportive environment theory (SET), which combines knowledge about restorative environments and salutogenesis. The dis- tinctive approach in this thesis is the combination of theoretical per- spectives on salutogenesis and social support.

Mental health problems among young adults in nature-based ser- vices in Norway More than half of the sample of participants in nature-based services showed symptoms of mental health problems according to HSCL-10 (paper II). In general, they reported fewer symptoms than those in the sample of clinical in-patients receiving mental health care (18-30 81

years), and more symptoms than a general population sample of young adults (18-19 years old). There were no differences found in symptoms of mental health problems between participants recruited via NAV or local mental health services. The qualitative data reveal that partici- pants struggle with social insecurity, vulnerability, and social anxiety, and that they have had previous negative experiences with social inter- actions (paper IV).

Previous research on mental health problems among young adults in nature-based services Overall, there have been few studies on nature-based services existing independently of research interventions, either in Norway or interna- tionally. In addition, there is no central register with information on participants in nature-based services in Norway (Ellingsen-Dalskau, Berget, et al., 2016). Knowledge about participants in these services and their mental health problems is therefore scarce (Ellingsen- Dalskau, 2017). The Ellingsen-Dalskau, Berget, Tellnes, and Ihlebæk (2017) study of participants in prevocational training on care farms in Norway finds that more than 70% of the participants reported anxiety and sadness/depression; a higher score than found in our research pro- ject. Moreover Ellingsen-Dalskau et al. (2017) find that psychological complaints overall were reported by 90% of the participants. This study used ‘the psychological complaints subscale’ of ‘the subjective health complaints inventory’ (Ellingsen-Dalskau et al., 2017) as opposed to HSCL-10. Nevertheless, these results support our finding that many participants in nature-based services struggle with mental health prob- lems. The age span in the Ellingsen-Dalskau et al. (2017) study was broader (19-65), which may explain some of the differences, but the mean age was only 35.7 (SD = 11.9). In addition, the use of different instruments may explain differences in results.

Half of the participants 23 years or older in the current survey had not completed upper secondary school. In comparison, 86.1% of the par- ticipants in the study of Ellingsen-Dalskau et al. (2017) had finished only lower or upper secondary school.

82

The importance of knowledge about mental health problems among participants in nature-based services Social support is important for young adults’ mental health. A high de- gree of social support is associated with fewer mental health problems (Eskin, 1995; Van Droogenbroeck et al., 2018). Social support seems important for young adults in order to cope with mental health prob- lems (Ramsdal et al., 2018).

Participants in nature-based services are, to a large degree, socially vul- nerable. Additional knowledge about the mental health problems among this group, their struggles and needs, and their experiences with social support, may highlight the importance of social support as a health-promoting instrument, and indicate how dimensions of social support may help in various situations. Additionally, Ellingsen- Dalskau (2017) argues that more knowledge about the participants in these services is needed in order to create optimal rehabilitation con- texts.

The meaning of dimensions of social support in nature-based ser- vices for young adults with mental health problems The following section discusses the general concept of social support and dimensions of social support in nature-based services. It also dis- cusses the psychometric properties of SPS-10 as a measure of social support. Following this, the meanings of dimensions of social support in relation to SOC and mental health problems are outlined and dis- cussed.

The concept social support and its dimensions – Conclusions from the psychometric analyses Results from paper III concludes that the SPS-10 has difficulties related to targeting and construct validity. In order to improve the psychomet- ric properties, the scale was modified to include eight items measuring functional support. This modification was based on theoretical and sta- tistical considerations. Hence, the results from paper III gives a better understanding of the concept social support and its dimensions.

83

The results from paper III indicate that including both functional and structural social support dimensions in the same scale is not appropri- ate. These two types of social support do not belong to the same latent trait. Functional support refers to what other people may contribute to an individual, practically, and emotionally. In contrast, structural sup- port refers to the level of connection a person feels within a social net- work. This supports the idea to separate between structural and func- tional social support, as suggested in previous research studies (S. Cohen & Wills, 1985; Uchino, 2004; Wills, 1991; Wills & Shinar, 2000). These findings should be considered when developing new scales for measuring social support, or when using social support scales compris- ing both functional and structural support. To solve this problem, the SPS-10 was modified to an 8-item scale measuring only functional sup- port (emotional, esteem, informational, and instrumental support), and this SPS-8 scale was used in the analysis.

Results from paper III give insights into other shortcomings of the SPS- 10. They reveal that the SPS-10 lacks items measuring lower degrees of perceived social support. Additionally, it may be difficult to satisfacto- rily distinguish between participants with low perceived social support using this scale. The Rasch model emphasises the importance of using scales that include items reflecting a variety of levels of the construct it intends to measure, in this case perceived social support. Results from paper III indicate that the SPS-10 does not provide the information on social support it was intended to collect. This represents a threat to the validity and reliability of this scale, and therefore to the validity of the results when using the SPS-10. However, this risk is reduced by using the SPS-8 instead of SPS-10. The validity and reliability of the SPS-10 and SPS-8 will be further elaborated in the methodological discussion.

The dimension ‘opportunity for nurturance’ is not included in SPS-10 or SPS-8, hence this dimension is not a part of the psychometric anal- ysis. However, empirical support for the importance and opportunity for nurturance in nature-based services is found in the results from the qualitative data in paper IV, and these results are supported by findings in previous studies (Pedersen, Ihlebæk, et al., 2012). When measuring

84

social support in nature-based services, this dimension should be in- cluded. A more in-depth discussion about the dimension ‘opportunity for nurturance’ is presented below.

The SPS-8 used in this study measures social support generally, not specifically in relation to nature-based services. Future research stud- ies may benefit from focusing on social support at these services more specifically. Findings from paper IV indicate the importance of focus- ing on the various dimensions of social support that nature-based ser- vices may provide, regardless from whom this support is received.

In summary, the results from the analysis of psychometric properties of the SPS-10 support the division between structural and functional social support, and indicate it is not appropriate to include these two dimensions in the same scale, at least not among this present popula- tion. Further, the lack of items measuring lower degrees of social sup- port, and the fact that the SPS-10 and SPS-8 lack items measuring the dimension ‘opportunity for nurturance’, are limitations when using these scales among participants in nature-based services. Exploring and potentially developing other scales measuring social support in these services is recommended.

Meaning of dimensions of social support in nature-based services Results from paper IV indicate that all six dimensions of social support as described by Weiss (1974) are found in nature-based services. In this section, the meaning of each of these dimensions specifically, and of social support more generally, will be discussed in relation to partici- pants’ SOC, mental health problems, and experiences on the farm.

Emotional support Emotional support is described as relationships that offer acceptance, intimacy, and emotional sharing (see e.g. Wills, 1991). The results from paper IV indicate that participants encountered service leaders who care for them in a deep sense. This has been found to be important for vulnerable young adults (Blum, McNeely, & Nonnemaker, 2002; Noble-Carr, Barker, McArthur, & Woodman, 2014). The results also in- dicate that participants felt accepted at the services. Service leaders,

85

other participants, and the animals contributed to this sense of ac- ceptance. This is consistent with findings in previous research (Ellingsen-Dalskau, Morken, et al., 2016). Participants also described peer support, sharing problems, and experiences with fellow partici- pants at the services. This may represent a form of emotional sharing (Wills, 1991). Interaction with the animals also allows for emotional sharing and acceptance. Participants described the animals as being non-judgmental, which is a form of acceptance. In addition, partici- pants noted that animals express emotions to the participants and re- flect the participants’ emotions. This may be seen as a form of emo- tional sharing. Animals are described as being pleasant to cuddle with, potentially representing a type of intimacy. Physical contact with the animals has been described as important in previous research (Pedersen, Ihlebæk, et al., 2012). Half of the participants in the current survey reported that animals provide social support on the same level as humans. It can be suggested that animals might have the potential to provide emotional support qualities to the participants on similar levels as fellow humans.

Emotional support may be of specific importance for young adults ex- periencing mental health problems and social insecurity. Negative past experiences and the feeling of being judged by other people may have prevented them in engaging in relationships that could provide emo- tional support. They may have ambivalent feelings related to receiving emotional support. Animals are described as not judging; hence, it may be easier to share this kind of emotional bond with them. Emotional support may be considered one of the basic dimensions of social sup- port. This dimension is associated with feelings of safety, and hence should be incorporated in order to enable the person to benefit from other dimensions of social support.

Esteem support The results from paper IV indicate that participants experienced es- teem support at the services. Esteem support represents a strengthen- ing of a person’s sense of competence, for instance through positive feedback regarding their skills or expressions of belief in their coping abilities (see e.g. Cutrona & Russell, 1987). Esteem support is provided directly and indirectly at the services. Service leaders facilitate work 86

tasks or activities adapted to each individual so that they can experi- ence mastery. This may be seen as a form of indirect esteem support. In addition, by appreciating the work tasks participants perform, and by giving them responsibility, the service leaders demonstrate confi- dence in the participants’ skills and coping abilities. The findings are reported by previous researchers (Ellingsen-Dalskau, Morken, et al., 2016; Pedersen, Ihlebæk, et al., 2012). Animals may also provide es- teem support through positive feedback on participants’ actions (e.g. animals showing appreciation when the barn is cleaned, when they get food, when they are being groomed).

Esteem support such as receiving encouragement, experiencing others’ faith in their abilities, and being able to complete activities or work tasks adapted to their qualifications, may lead to participants experi- encing a sense of mastery. Moreover, mastery in one field promotes mastery in other fields, potentially creating a positive spiral.

Informational and instrumental support Informational support is described as advice or guidance from support- ive people. Instrumental support refers to help with goods or services (see e.g. Cutrona & Russell, 1987). Results from paper IV indicate that participants received informational and instrumental (e.g. practical as- sistance) support at the nature-based services, both from the service leader and the other participants. For instance, they receive advice and support if they are unsure about how to perform a work task. These dimensions of support are also described in previous research (Elings & Hassink, 2008; Ellingsen-Dalskau, 2017).

Participants described peer support as the sharing of experiences and supporting one another. This may include informational support. Pre- vious research indicates that peer support serves as an important re- covery factor for people with mental health problems (Oades et al., 2013).

Social integration Social integration reflects a person’s feeling of being part of a group that shares common interests and concerns (Cutrona & Russell, 1990). The results from paper IV indicate there was strong solidarity among 87

the participants, and that the other participants as well as the service leaders were important for the participants to benefit from the services. There are several factors promoting social integration at the services. One important factor is enabling participants to feel included as ordi- nary workers at nature-based services (Pedersen, Ihlebæk, et al., 2012).

Opportunity for nurturance The dimension ‘opportunity for nurturance’ refers to the possibility for giving support or care to others (Cutrona & Russell, 1987). The results from paper IV illustrate how the animals give participants the oppor- tunity to provide care. In previous research, vulnerable young adults have noted the importance of caring connections with animals, partic- ularly if they are not able to make caring connections with other human beings (Noble-Carr et al., 2014). The prospect of becoming valuable to another living creature by providing care is important (Mayeroff, 1990; Noble-Carr et al., 2014; Thoits, 2011), and caring for living beings may provide a feeling of dignity (Mayeroff, 1990). Similar results related to the positive aspects of giving care are found in other studies on nature- based services (Ellingsen-Dalskau, Morken, et al., 2016; Pedersen, Ihlebæk, et al., 2012). According to Leck et al. (2015), the opportunity for nurturance dimension is relevant in relation to animals and other people.

It has been suggested that the mechanisms for this dimension (giving and receiving care) is somewhat different from other dimensions of so- cial support (Cutrona & Russell, 1990). The results from the studies in- cluded in this thesis do not provide insights into the mechanisms of this dimension, nor if these mechanisms are different from other dimen- sions of social support. However, it is evident from the results in paper IV that the opportunity to care for others is important for the partici- pants in nature-based services. Hence, it is important to focus on this dimension in studies of social support in these services, as noted by previous studies (see e.g. Leck et al., 2015; Pedersen, Ihlebæk, et al., 2012).

Previously it has been suggested that the ‘opportunity for nurturance’ dimension may be perceived differently depending on a person’s age or other life circumstances that involve heavy caring duties (Drageset & 88

Lindstrøm, 2003). For example, an older person who is responsible for caring for a sick spouse may perceive this dimension as stressful, rather than socially supportive. Additional caring duties may hinder them so- cially. For young adults with mental health problems, on the other hand, this dimension seems to represent a health-promoting dimen- sion, as found by Langeland and Wahl (2009). Having mental health problems may mean that the person is dependent or has been depend- ent on others for help. Having the opportunity to give something back may be of particular importance for them.

The findings related to dimensions of social support provide insights into important aspects of nature-based services and their potential contributions. These insights can be applied in the development of new nature-based services, or other services for young adults with mental health problems.

In the following section, the meaning of various dimensions of social support are discussed in relation to SOC and mental health problems.

The meaning of different dimensions of social support in relation to SOC Results from paper IV indicate that there is a strong relation between social support, as measured by SPS-8, and SOC among the participants in nature-based services. This relationship between social support and SOC has also been found in previous studies (see e.g. Holmberg et al., 2004; Langeland & Wahl, 2009). However, previous studies of the re- lationship between social support and SOC among people with mental health problems find that the dimension ‘opportunity for nurturance’ is particularly important for increasing SOC among this population (Langeland & Wahl, 2009). The ‘opportunity for nurturance’ dimen- sion is not included in the SPS-8. This could indicate that if a social support scale including the opportunity for nurturance dimension had been used in this thesis, an even stronger relation between social sup- port and SOC among the participants could have been found. This hy- pothesis could not be tested here, but warrants further research.

The results in paper IV raise an interesting hypothesis that has not been considered in any previous research study; namely that interaction 89

with animals may be of greater importance to participants with low SOC scores than for participants with high SOC scores. Participants with low SOC scores may appreciate the opportunity to build trust with a living creature that does not condemn or judge them. Animals do not demand anything from the participants other than care. The fact that animals are perceived as beings who do not make demands or condemn is supported by previous research (e.g.Leck et al., 2015). Being with the animals may therefore be of special importance to participants with low SOC scores, who are less likely to view their surroundings as satisfac- torily comprehensible, manageable, and meaningful. This hypothesis should be elaborated in order to provide more knowledge on the spe- cific importance of animals in nature-based services, and whether cer- tain participant groups (e.g. participants with low SOC scores) may es- pecially benefit from being with animals.

As mentioned, interaction with animals may contribute to strengthen- ing the three sub-components of SOC: comprehensibility, manageabil- ity and meaningfulness, due to their traits of not judging and of provid- ing emotional support. It is also likely that other dimensions and pro- viders of social support may strengthen the SOC of participants in na- ture-based services. Previous research studies have found that social support is important for strengthening a person’s SOC (Langeland & Wahl, 2009; Skärsäter et al., 2005).

The results presented in paper IV point to several factors at the nature- based services that may strengthen participants’ feeling of comprehen- sibility, especially related to the social support dimensions of informa- tional and esteem support. Through clear information and esteem sup- port, the environment at the nature-based services may be viewed as coherent, structured, and clear to the participants (Antonovsky, 1987). Further, the dimensions of manageability may be strengthened through informational, esteem, and instrumental support enabling participants to feel that they have adequate resources available to man- age the demands they are facing (Antonovsky, 1987). Participating in activities in which they experience mastery and are part of a mutually supportive social community also has the potential to induce a feeling of meaning.

90

Previous studies support the finding that participation in nature-based services potentially strengthens participants’ SOC (Granerud & Eriksson, 2014; Schreuder et al., 2014). A British study on care farms using qualitative and quantitative methods includes three items that measure SOC in its questionnaire (Leck et al., 2015). The study finds no significant correlation between time spent at the nature-based ser- vice and SOC, but a significant increase in SOC scores is found between the first and second data-collection periods. The authors do not further delve into concrete supportive elements in relation to SOC, but point to meaningful activities, a supportive farmer, interactions with other par- ticipants, and interactions with animals as important for development of strong SOC (Leck et al., 2015).

The results presented in paper IV further indicate a strong association between SOC and mental health problems among the participants in nature-based services. This is in accordance with results from previous studies on the relation between mental health problems and SOC (see e.g. M. Eriksson & Lindström, 2006; K. W. Nilsson et al., 2010). Vari- ous dimensions of social support may strengthen participants’ SOC, and as a consequence may have the potential to improve their mental health. However, there is also the possibility that participants with a strong SOC perceive social support in a different way than do partici- pants with lower SOC scores. In addition, participants with fewer symptoms of mental health problems may have greater potential for receiving or perceiving social support. The results from this thesis do not provide sufficient evidence to draw conclusions regarding these hy- potheses. Further research is needed.

The meaning of different dimensions of social support in relation to mental health No significant statistical association between social support and mental health is found in the results from paper IV. However, it is evident from previous research that social support is potentially important for young adults’ mental health (see e.g. Van Droogenbroeck et al., 2018). One possible explanation as to why no significant association between social support and mental health is found in this thesis is that the SPS-8 measures dimensions of social support that are not important for the mental health of participants in nature-based services. Qualitative 91

findings suggest that dimensions of social support in nature-based ser- vices improve participants’ mental health. The potential meaning of specific dimensions of social support are discussed above. In summary, emotional support, esteem support, and opportunity for nurturance may have particular importance for young adults with mental health problems. Since they are socially vulnerable, receiving emotional sup- port may provide a sense of safety. Esteem support may strengthen their self-esteem. Opportunity for nurturance may enable them to feel valuable. Emotional and esteem support are also found to be strongly positively related to quality of life (Caron, 2012).

Related to social support and participants’ mental health, it is relevant to discuss the definition of social support used in this thesis. Social sup- port is defined by Cobb (1976, p. 300) as: ‘information leading the sub- ject to believe that he is cared for and loved, esteemed and a member of a network of mutual obligations’. The aspect of mutual obligations requires some consideration. Results discussed above show that many of the young adults in nature-based services struggle with mental health problems and are socially insecure. It may be difficult for these young adults to fulfil mutual obligations with other people. They need social support, but may not have the capability to provide it. However, as discussed above, it may be that interaction with animals, who do not make demands or judgements, give them opportunities to fulfil obliga- tions. Alternatively, the mutual obligations they face at the services may be easier for them to fulfil.

The meaning of the context in relation to social support It is necessary to discuss the potential effect or influence that context has on dimensions of social support in nature-based services. That is, do nature-based services provide a context that influences the presence or quality of various dimensions of social support? None of the studies cited in this thesis has such a focus, and no results directly address this question. However, it is clear from the results that the services offer a variety of activities that are flexible and have different levels of diffi- culty, making it possible for participants to experience mastery. The services allow participants to work together with fellow participants and the service leader; these conditions support social integration.

92

Summary of the discussion Previous research indicates that nature-based services provide positive activities for people with mental health problems and highlights the importance of the social context, enabling participants to experience social gains. However, no previous research study has focused system- atically on studying the meaning of various dimensions of social sup- port in nature-based services. The results indicate that nature-based services provide all dimensions of social support to participants. In ad- dition, these dimensions may positively influence their SOC and men- tal health. The results indicate that animals may be of special im- portance for participants with low SOC scores.

Methodological considerations Several considerations were considered to ensure the quality of the data included in this thesis, e.g. the validity (credibility), reliability (de- pendability), and generalisability (transferability), as described in the methods chapter. First, a discussion of the quality of the data in the survey is presented, followed by a discussion on the trustworthiness of the qualitative data and methodological considerations regarding the literature review.

Methodological considerations related to data collection for the survey Several challenges were faced while collecting data for the survey. The main challenges resulted from not having a comprehensive overview of the field. There is no register of the number or types of nature-based services in Norway.3 Matmerk (the organisation responsible for the Inn på Tunet authorisation) operates with a register, but this only contains the services that are Inn på Tunet-approved. Furthermore, this register does not distinguish between services that are active and currently have participants, and those that have been approved but have not yet started, or those without any current active services. This register has some data on what kind of participant groups the different services are intended to serve. However, many of the services cater to a varied

3 This also applies for Sweden

93

group of participants, and are listed as offering services to the elderly, children, and adults receiving work training. As a result, it seemed that some services would have potential participants for our research sam- ple, but in fact had no relevant participants. Characteristics of individ- ual participants in nature-based services are not registered anywhere. Therefore, mapping nature-based services was a time-consuming and challenging endeavour. There may be a risk of selection bias at the farm level, meaning that some types of farms are systematically not invited to participate. However, the thorough mapping of nature-based ser- vices in this study reduced this risk. There is no information about the exact number of nature-based services that have participants included in the survey. However, several phone calls to service-leaders revealed that this number is likely to be between 45 and 55. Ellingsen-Dalskau (2017) reports that 65 care farms were included in their study of prev- ocational rehabilitation in Norway. This study included participants between 18-65 years old. It is likely that a wider age span leads to more services being suitable for participation.

There is also a risk of selection bias at the participant level. Because recruitment of participants was done by service leaders and leaders from NAV, there is a risk that participants with most severe mental health problems were assessed as not being able to participate in the survey. Alternatively, there is a risk that participants with the most se- vere mental health problems chose not to participate because they felt it would be too demanding to answer the questionnaire. It is also pos- sible that participants with a positive attitude towards the services were more likely to be invited to participate. This represents a potential non- response bias, in that people who choose to participate or were invited to participate may be different from those who did not participate in the study. To prevent this, participants were invited to answer the ques- tionnaire by telephone. However, none of the participants chose this option. Four of the participants did choose to answer the questionnaire in a structured interview. Because there is no register of participants in nature-based services in Norway, it is difficult to obtain information about non-responders. This makes it difficult to calculate a response rate. The intention was to calculate response rates based on service leaders completing a form indicating the number of participants they invited to participate in the study. This did not succeed as intended, as 94

only a few of the service leaders returned this form. However, knowledge about the size of the actual population was obtained through contact with service leaders, and this knowledge was later used for estimating the size of the population. This estimation, however, has a large degree of uncertainty. Additionally, the data collection period lasted for approximately two years, and the size of the population may have varied during this time.

The fact that the service leaders or leaders from NAV distributed the questionnaires to the participants raises some ethical considerations. The participants are somewhat dependent on these leaders, and may therefore have felt obliged to participate. To prevent this, the infor- mation letter clearly indicated that participation was voluntary. In ad- dition, pre-paid envelopes were provided with the questionnaires, so participants could complete and return the questionnaire at their con- venience, and not were dependent on the service leader.

A strength of this study is the exploration of the significant methodo- logical challenges when performing research in this field.

Methodological considerations related to sample size in the sur- vey In the quantitative analysis, the trustworthiness of statistical analysis is dependent upon sample size. Larger sample sizes are preferable to small ones. The sample size potentially affects the power of the statis- tical analysis (J. Cohen, 1992). This is especially relevant when consid- ering the generalisability of quantitative data. The sample size in the survey is rather small. No exact preferable size of a sample for statisti- cal analysis is suggested, and preferred size is related to the type of sta- tistical analysis being applied. In this present study, great efforts were made to achieve the largest sample size possible. However, this turned out to be a challenge. Because the total population is estimated to be rather small, it is likely that even if the whole population was included, the sample would still be relatively small. As the aim was to examine various dimensions of social support, rather than drawing statistical interferences, no sample size calculation was conducted.

95

Methodological considerations related to analysis of the survey data The responses to many of the items in the survey focusing on the par- ticipants and their experiences at the nature-based services are skewed. That is, most of the participants responded positively to the items. This might reflect the fact that the participants were generally positive re- garding the nature-based services. However, future research studies should consider how to collect richer data from participants in nature- based services. Due to the skewed responses to many of the questions, the questions used in paper IV where dichotomized. This dichotomiza- tion was done to determine if there were differences between the peo- ple strongly agreeing with a statement, and the participants who chose other response categories.

The low number of participants, as discussed earlier, had consequences for which types of statistical analysis were appropriate or possible to conduct. Many of the analyses of the quantitative data, therefore, are descriptive. Significance testing was not seen as appropriate in some of the analyses.

Generalisability of the findings from the survey Several factors are considered in judging generalisability of the survey findings. First, it is not possible to provide an analysis of non-respond- ers. This makes it difficult to determine if there are differences between people who chose to participate in the survey and those who did not. In other words, it is not possible to determine if the sample is truly repre- sentative of the population. Further, it is difficult to understand exactly who the population is, because there is such limited information avail- able about participants in nature-based services. However, similar re- sults are found in studies among similar populations in Norway and internationally. This supports the assumption that the findings may be valid for other populations participating in nature-based services in Norway and in other countries.

The psychometric properties for the standardized instruments used in the survey Scales used to measure a specific concept need to be of high quality. The items included should accurately describe the concept they intend 96 to explore, and only that concept. Further, the scales should be equally valid across different groups (e.g. male and female). The outcomes of the Rasch analysis address these criteria, and hence contribute knowledge about validity, reliability, and generalisability of the scales used (Wolfe & Smith, 2007a, 2007b). Rasch analysis was used to ana- lyse the psychometric properties of the standardised measures in- cluded in the survey: HSCL-10, SPS-10 and SOC-13.

Although none of the included scales were found to have perfect psy- chometric properties, they all met the general criteria of measurement according to the Rasch model. However, the SPS-8 and SOC-13 were revised to improve their psychometric properties. These revisions for the SPS-8 are thoroughly presented in paper III. The revisions of re- sponse categories for SOC-13 are described in paper IV.

As mentioned in the discussion of the results presented in paper III, problems with targeting in the SPS-8 scale may represent a threat to the validity of the scale. Targeting assesses whether scales are appro- priately targeted to the population. Results indicate that the SPS scale does not have the potential to reflect the whole spectrum of social sup- port that the participants may perceive that they receive or do not re- ceive. There is a lack of items measuring lower degrees of social sup- port. Hence, the distribution is reduced. This might affect the associa- tions between SPS-8, SOC-13, and HSCL-10.

Methodological considerations in relation to use of HSCL-10 The survey study design and the chosen questionnaires also may be limitations for this thesis. The use of HSCL-10 in a study claiming to be salutogenic may represent a discrepancy with the salutogenic model (Mittelmark & Bauer, 2017), since HSCL-10 measures only symptoms of anxiety and depression. However, using the HSCL-10 measure made it possible to compare participants in nature-based services with other populations, and this contributes to eliciting knowledge about partici- pants’ characteristics, which is an important objective of the research.

97

Credibility, dependability and transferability for the qualitative data Even though the qualitative data originally were collected for another purpose than use in this thesis, the interview guide includes questions highly relevant to social support. There might be some threat to the credibility of the qualitative data related to recruitment of participants, but virtually the entire group in a service was interviewed. The leaders did not pick, for example, participants with more or less severe mental health problems. The skewed gender distribution among the interview- ees weakened the variety in the sample, but mirrors to some degree the gender distribution at the services.

The fact that two researchers conducted the interviews at several na- ture-based services, and that a third researcher did the main analysis of the data strengthens the credibility. The analysis process with the selection of meaning units followed strict qualitative guidelines with profound readings and fellow-researcher validation. Nevertheless, there is a risk of choosing meaning units that are too wide or too nar- row. In qualitative research, the best quality assurance is transparency and detailed description of the different stages of the analysis. In addi- tion, the use of quotations in the text strengthens the credibility. Fi- nally, the fact that several researchers with thorough knowledge about nature-based services were involved in the process assures credibility.

Dependability may be threatened when data collection stretches over time, with a risk that the focus of the researcher changes. This may be a risk in the qualitative data, because they are process data. On the other hand, the two researchers conducting the interviews could sup- port each other in staying true to the theme. In the end, only the reader can assess the transferability of the data. Effort was made in describing the entire process thoroughly, so that the reader has the opportunity to evaluate the transferability.

Methodological considerations for the literature review The searches conducted for the literature review were thorough and in- cluded a wide range of databases, web-pages, and hand-searching in reference lists, to minimise the risk of selection bias related to literature inclusion. Thorough searches including exploration of multiple search 98 terms reduced the risk that central search terms were omitted. Never- theless, when selecting items to be included in the literature review, there still is a risk for selection bias. This risk was reduced by having clear inclusion criteria. All the three authors read the literature and as- sessed which items to include. By using identical data extraction schemes for all the included literature, it was ensured that data relevant for the aim and comparable data from all the literature were extracted. Also, validity was strengthened by involving all three researchers in the analysis process.

99 Conclusions and implications

 Previous research indicates that nature-based services provide positive activities for people with mental health problems.  No other research studies have specifically focused on the mean- ing of various dimensions of social support in nature-based ser- vices, although the importance of social factors in these services is well documented.  Young adults participating in nature-based services in Norway struggle with mental health problems and social vulnerability.  Nature-based services provide emotional, esteem, informa- tional, and instrumental support. Further, the social support di- mensions of social integration and opportunity for nurturance are found.  It is appropriate to separate between structural and functional social support, since measuring these forms of social support us- ing the same scale seems inappropriate.  The service leader, other participants, and animals provide social support to the participants.  Results indicate that the social support dimensions may posi- tively influence participants’ mental health and SOC.  Interaction with animals may be of special importance for par- ticipants with low SOC.

100

Future research

Research on nature-based services is still in its early stages. Many im- portant issues need attention as an extension of this thesis. Some are listed below.

 Research that compares nature-based services with other types of health and welfare services for the same populations is needed to procure more knowledge about the special qualities of nature- based services, as well as to deepen the knowledge about which groups of participants benefit from these services, and which do not.

 In future research, there is a need for close collaboration among researchers, participants, nature-based services and ‘buyers’ (e.g., welfare and work authorities, mental health services and schools) in research projects that ensure data quality and availa- bility, and reduce time spent on data collection. This, in turn, will make it easier to assess results from such services, e.g., conduct research with multiple measurement times.

 Results from this thesis indicates that animals may be of specific importance for participants with low SOC. There is a need for additional knowledge on the potential impacts of interacting with animals for specific participant groups.

 This thesis is based on empirical data collected in Norway and (in paper I) from the Nordic countries. Comparative studies in- cluding countries outside the Nordic region would increase knowledge about the variety as well as strengths and weaknesses of the world-wide phenomenon of nature-based services.

101

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank everyone who supported and believed in me dur- ing the work with this thesis. In particular, I would like to show my appreciation to:

My supervisors Bengt G. Eriksson, Ragnfrid Eline Kogstad, and Daniel Bergh. I am sincerely grateful for having you as my supervisors. Thanks for sharing your knowledge and experience and for always supporting and believing in me.

Karlstad University for accepting me as a PhD student, and to all the people at the University who have given me help and support along the way, from commenting on manuscripts to helping with practical issues.

Inland Norway University of Applied Sciences for giving me the oppor- tunity to work towards my PhD; ROGCS project leader Ragnfrid Eline Kogstad and former Head of the Department of Mental Health Gunvor Volla Stensrud for employing me as a PhD student; Former Dean of the Department of Public Health Sven Inge Sunde; former vice Dean of re- search Ingeborg Hartz; former Head of the Department of Nursing and Mental Health, Marianne Stensvehagen; current Dean of the faculty of Health and Social Sciences, Ingrid Guldvik; and Head of the Depart- ment of Health Randi Martinsen for facilitating my work.

Inland Norway University of Applied Sciences and the Norwegian Re- search Council for financing the project.

To all my fellow doctoral students in Elverum and Karlstad, thanks for your support. To Tuva Sandsdalen, Hanne Søberg Finbråten, Annette Løvehim Kleppang, Marie Dahlen Granrud, and Anne Kjersti Myhrene, thanks for all the trips to Karlstad, for sharing your knowledge and ex- perience, and for always supporting me.

To all my colleagues in Elverum. Thanks for your support.

The rest of the ROGCS project members Jan Kåre Hummelvoll, Mark Hopfenbeck, Rita Agdal and Arild Granerud for the collaboration at the

102 beginning of this work. Arild Granerud, thanks for always keeping your door open and for your support along the way. Jan Kåre Hummelvoll, thank you for your support as a supervisor in the beginning of my PhD work, and for your contribution on my first paper.

Librarians Elin Opheim and Eva Backström thanks for valuable help with EndNote and references. Thanks to Bente Giset for administrative help.

Helge Prytz Toft and Vanja Sollesnes Knudsen for sharing your data with me. The Norwegian Social Research (NOVA) institute and re- gional centres for drug rehabilitation (KoRus) for allowing me to use data from Ungdata.

Curt Hagquist for always being supportive and interested in my work.

All the participants in the nature-based services and the service leaders for contribution in this project.

Family and friends. Thanks for all your support. Especially thanks to my parents and parents-in-law. I could not have done this without you; your support and helping out with the children are invaluable.

Eirik, Erle, Eskil my beloved children, thanks for always reminding me of what is most important in life. Thanks for giving me distraction and love. Dag, my wonderful husband, thanks for always supporting and believing in me. I am truly grateful.

103

References

Aamodt, L. G. (2014). Den gode relasjonen: Støtte, omsorg eller anerkjennelse? [The good relation: Support, care or acknowledgement] (2. ed.). Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal akademisk. Andrich, D. (1978). A rating formulation for ordered response categories. Psychometrika, 43, 561-573. Annerstedt, M., & Wahrborg, P. (2011). Nature-assisted therapy: Systematic review of controlled and observational studies. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 39(4), 371-388. Antonovsky, A. (1979). Health, stress, and coping. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unraveling the mystery of health: How people manage stress and stay well. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Antonovsky, A. (1993). The structure and properties of the Sense of Coherence Scale. Social Science & Medicine, 36(6), 725-733. Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55(5), 469-480. Arnett, J. J. (2004). Emerging adulthood : The winding road from the late teens through the twenties. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Artz, B., & Davis, D. B. (2017). Green care: A review of the benefits and potential of animal-assisted care farming globally and in rural America. Animals : An Open Access Journal from MDPI, 7(4), 1-13. Axelsson, L., & Ejlertsson, G. (2002). Self‐reported health, self‐esteem and social support among young unemployed people: A population‐based study. International Journal of Social Welfare, 11(2), 111-119. Azmitia, M., Syed, M., & Radmacher, K. (2013). Finding your niche: Identity and emotional support in emerging adults' adjustment to the transition to college. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 23(4), 744-761. Bakken, K., Landheim, A., & Vaglum, P. (2003). Primary and secondary substance misusers: Do they differ in substance- induced and substance-independent mental disorders? Alcohol and Alcoholism, 38(1), 54-59. Bele, M. (2013). "I naturen kommer jeg liksom på plass i meg selv" : Sansende nærvær i naturopplevelser som helsefremmende kontekst i psykisk helsearbeid ["In nature, I'm in present in myself": Sensing presence in nature experiences as a health promotion context in mental health work]. (Mastersdegree), University of Agder, Grimstad, Norway.

104 Bengtsson, A., & Grahn, P. (2014). Outdoor environments in healthcare settings: A quality evaluation tool for use in designing healthcare gardens. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 13(4), 878-891. Berget, B., & Braastad, B. O. (2008). Kunnskapsstatus og forskningsbehov for Inn på tunet [Knownledge status and research needs for ‘Into the farmyard’]. Ås, Norway: Institutt for husdyr- og akvakulturvitenskap, Universitetet for miljø- og biovitenskap. Berget, B., Ekeberg, Ø., & Braastad, B. O. (2008). Animal-assisted therapy with farm animals for persons with psychiatric disorders: Effects on self-efficacy, coping ability and quality of life, a randomized controlled trial. Clinical Practice and Epidemiology in Mental Health, 4(9), 1-7. Berget, B., Ekeberg, Ø., Pedersen, I., & Braastad, B. O. (2011). Animal- assisted therapy with farm animals for persons with psychiatric disorders: Effects on anxiety and depression, a randomized controlled trial. Occupational Therapy in Mental Health, 27(1), 50-64. Berget, B., Krøger, E., & Thorød, A. B. (2018). Antrozoologi : Samspill mellom dyr og menneske [: Interaction between animals and humans]. Oslo, Norway: Universitetsforlaget. Berget, B., Skarsaune, I., Ekeberg, Ø., & Braastad, B. O. (2007). Humans with mental disorders working with farm animals: A behavioral study. Occupational Therapy in Mental Health, 23(2), 101-117. Berkman, L. F. (1995). The role of social relations in health promotion. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57(3), 245-254. Berkman, L. F., Glass, T., Brissette, I., & Seeman, T. E. (2000). From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. Social Science & Medicine, 51(6), 843-857. Beyer, K. M., Kaltenbach, A., Szabo, A., Bogar, S., Nieto, F. J., & Malecki, K. M. (2014). Exposure to neighborhood green space and mental health: Evidence from the survey of the health of Wisconsin. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 11(3), 3453-3472. Blum, R. W., McNeely, C., & Nonnemaker, J. (2002). Vulnerability, risk, and protection. Journal of Adolescent Health, 31(1), 28-39. Bokkers, E. (2006). Effects of interactions between humans and domesticated animals. In J. Hassink & M. van Djik (Eds.), Farming for health (pp. 31-41). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer. Bondevik, M. (1996). Loneliness among the oldest old, a comparison between residents living in nursing homes and residents living in the community. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 43(3), 181-198.

105 Bossard, J. H. (1944). The mental hygiene of owning a dog. Mental Hygiene, 28, 408-413. Brage, S., & Bragstad, T. (2011). Unge på arbeids-og helserelaterte ordninger [Young people on occupational and health-related schemes]. Retrieved from https://www.nav.no/no/NAV+og+samfunn/Kunnskap/Analys er+fra+NAV/NAV+rapportserie/NAV+rapporter/unge- p%C3%A5-arbeids-og-helserelaterte-ordninger Bragg, R., & Atkins, G. (2016). A review of nature-based interventions for mental health care. Retrieved from http://www.hphpcentral.com/wp- content/uploads/2016/03/NECR204_edition_1.pdf Breslin, F. C., Hepburn, C. G., Ibrahim, S., & Cole, D. (2006). Understanding stability and change in psychological distress and sense of coherence: A four‐year prospective study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36(1), 1-21. Brissette, I., Cohen, S., & Seeman, T. E. (2000). Measuring social integration and social networks. In S. Cohen, L. G. Underwood, & B. H. Gottlieb (Eds.), Social support measurement and intervention: A guide for health and social scientists (pp. 53- 85). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Burgon, H. (2003). Case studies of adults receiving horse-riding therapy. Anthrozoös, 16(3), 263-276. Børresen, B. (2006). Menneskenes verden sett med dyreøyne : Folk og dyr i 20 000 år [Humans' world through animal eyes: People and animals in 20 000 years]. Oslo, Norway: Tun. Care farming UK. (2017, 16.03.2017). Care farming UK. Retrieved from https://www.carefarminguk.org/home Caron, J. (2012). Predictors of quality of life in economically disadvantaged populations in Montreal. Social Indicators Research, 107(3), 411-427. Caron, J. (2013). Une validation de la forme abrégée de l’Échelle de provisions sociales : l’ÉPS-10 items. [A validation of the Social Provisions Scale: The SPS-10 items]. Santé mentale au Québec, 38(1), 297-318. Caron, J., Tempier, R., Mercier, C., & Leouffre, P. (1998). Components of social support and quality of life in severely mentally ill, low income individuals and a general population group. Community Mental Health Journal, 34(5), 459-475. Chassin, L., Sher, K. J., Hussong, A., & Curran, P. (2013). The developmental psychopathology of alcohol use and alcohol disorders: Research achievements and future directions. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4), 1567-1584. Chu, P. S., Saucier, D. A., & Hafner, E. (2010). Meta-analysis of the relationships between social support and well-being in children

106

and adolescents. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29(6), 624-645. Clatworthy, J., Hinds, J., & M. Camic, P. (2013). Gardening as a mental health intervention: A review. Mental Health Review Journal, 18(4), 214-225. Cobb, S. (1976). Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 38(5), 300-314. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155- 159. Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98(2), 310-357. Cole, K. M., Gawlinski, A., Steers, N., & Kotlerman, J. (2007). Animal- assisted therapy in patients hospitalized with heart failure. American Journal of Critical Care, 16(6), 575-585. Collishaw, S., Maughan, B., Natarajan, L., & Pickles, A. (2010). Trends in adolescent emotional problems in England: A comparison of two national cohorts twenty years apart. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(8), 885-894. Cutcliffe, J. R., & Travale, R. (2016). Unearthing the theoretical underpinnings of "green care" in mental health and substance misuse care: Theoretical underpinnings and contemporary clinical examples. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 37(3), 137- 147. Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. (1987). The provisions of social relationships and adaption to stress. In H. Jones & D. Pearlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships. A research annual 1 (pp. 37-67). Greenwich,CT: Jai Press. Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. W. (1990). Type of social support and specific stress: Toward a theory of optimal matching. In B. R. Sarason, I. G. Sarason, & G. R. Pierce (Eds.), Social support: An interactional view (pp. 319-366). New York, NY: Wiley. Dale, B., Sævareid, H. I., Kirkevold, M., & Söderhamn, O. (2010). Older home nursing patients’ perception of social provisions and received care. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 24(3), 523-532. De Vries, S., Verheij, R. A., Groenewegen, P. P., & Spreeuwenberg, P. (2003). Natural environments—healthy environments? An exploratory analysis of the relationship between greenspace and health. Environment and Planning A, 35(10), 1717-1731. Derogatis, L. R. (2008). Symptom checklist-90-revised (SCL-90-R); Brief symptom inventory (BSI). In A. J. Rush, M. B. First, & D. Blacker (Eds.), Handbook of psychiatric measures (2. ed., pp. 73-76). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. Derogatis, L. R. (2010). Symptom checklist-90-R. Norsk versjon. Manual for administrering og scoring [Symptom checklist-90-

107 R. Norwegian version. Manual for administration and scoring]. Stockholm, Sweden: NCS Perason. Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., Richels, K., Uhlenhuth, E. H., & Covi, L. (1974). The hopkins symptoms checklist (HSCL) -A self-report symptom inventory. Behavioral Science, 19, 1-15. Drageset, S., & Lindstrøm, T. C. (2003). The mental health of women with suspected breast cancer: The relationship between social support, anxiety, coping and defence in maintaining mental health. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 10(4), 401-409. Eckersley, R. (2011). Troubled youth: An island of misery in an ocean of happiness, or the tip of an iceberg of suffering? Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 5(1), 6-11. Elings, M., & Hassink, J. (2008). Green care farms, a safe community between illness or addiction and the wider society. International Journal of Therapeutic Communities, 29(3), 310-322. Ellingsen-Dalskau, L. H. (2017). Prevocational training on care-farms for people outside the work-force - the self-determination theory perspective. (Philosophiae Doctor (PhD) Thesis), Norwegian University of Life sciences, Ås, Norway. Ellingsen-Dalskau, L. H., Berget, B., Pedersen, I., Tellnes, G., & Ihlebæk, C. (2016). Understanding how prevocational training on care farms can lead to functioning, motivation and well-being. Disability and Rehabilitation, 38(25), 2504-2513. Ellingsen-Dalskau, L. H., Berget, B., Tellnes, G., & Ihlebæk, C. (2017). Satisfaction of basic psychological needs as a mediator in the relationship between subjective health complaints and satisfaction with life for people out of work. Unpublished manuscript Norwegian University of Life sciences. Ås, Norway. Ellingsen-Dalskau, L. H., Morken, M., Berget, B., & Pedersen, I. (2016). Autonomy support and need satisfaction in prevocational programs on care farms: The self-determination theory perspective. Work, 53(1), 73-85. Eriksson, H. G. (2003). Omsorg og sosialt arbeid [Care and social work]. In M.-A. Zahl (Ed.), Sosialt arbeid. Refleksjon og handling (pp. 99-118). Bergen, Norway: Fagbokforlaget. Eriksson, M., & Lindström, B. (2005). Validity of Antonovsky's Sense of Coherence Scale: A systematic review. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 59, 460-466. Eriksson, M., & Lindström, B. (2006). Antonovsky's Sense of Coherence Scale and the relation with health: A systematic review. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 60(5), 376-381. Eriksson, M., & Lindström, B. (2007). Antonovsky's Sense of Coherence Scale and its relation with quality of life: A systematic

108

review. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 61(11), 938-944. Eriksson, M., & Mittelmark, M. B. (2017). The sense of coherence and its measurement. In M. B. Mittelmark, S. Sagy, M. Eriksson, G. F. Bauer, J. M. Pelikan, B. Lindström, & G. A. Espnes (Eds.), The handbook of salutogenesis (pp. 97-106). Cham, Germany: Springer. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007%2F978-3-319- 04600-6.pdf. Eskin, M. (1995). Suicidal behavior as related to social support and assertiveness among Swedish and Turkish high school students: A cross‐cultural investigation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(2), 158-172. Fauske, H. (1996). Changing youth: Transition to adulthood in Norway. Young, 4(1), 47-62. Fine, A. H. (2015). Handbook on animal-assisted therapy : Foundations and guidelines for animal-assisted interventions (4 ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press Fjeldavli, E. (2006). The lay beliefs about farming for health. In J. Hassink & M. VanDijk (Eds.), Farming for health (pp. 73-90). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer. Foster, C. E., Horwitz, A., Thomas, A., Opperman, K., Gipson, P., Burnside, A., . . . King, C. A. (2017). Connectedness to family, school, peers, and community in socially vulnerable adolescents. Children and Youth Services Review, 81, 321-331. Frønes, I., & Strømme, H. (2014). Risiko og marginalisering : Norske barns levekår i kunnskapssamfunnet [Risk and marginalisation: The living conditions of Norwegian children in the knowledge society] (2. ed.). Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal akademisk. Fuglsang, J. K. (2011). Grøn omsorg - og udvikling af multifunktionelle landbrug som pædagogisk og beskæftigelsesmæssig ressource [Green care - and development of multifunctional agriculture as an educational and employment resource]. Århus, Denmark: Videncentret for Landbrug. Gadamer, H.-G. (2006). The enigma of health: The art of healing in a scientific age. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Gallis, C. (2013). What is green care? Introduction, history, and origins. In C. Gallis (Ed.), Green Care. For human thearpy, social innovation, rural economy, and education. (pp. 3-9). New York, NY: Nova Biomedical. Garrity, T. F., & Stallones, L. (1998). Effects of pet contact on human well-being: Review of recent research. In C. C. Wilson & D. C. Turner (Eds.), Companion animals in human health (pp. 3-22). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

109

Geyer, S. (1997). Some conceptual considerations on the sense of coherence. Social Science & Medicine, 44(12), 1771-1779. Gonzalez, M. T., Hartig, T., Patil, G. G., Martinsen, E. W., & Kirkevold, M. (2009). Therapeutic horticulture in clinical depression: A prospective study. Research and Theory for Nursing Practice: An International Journal, 23(4), 312-328. Gonzalez, M. T., Hartig, T., Patil, G. G., Martinsen, E. W., & Kirkevold, M. (2011a). A prospective study of existential issues in therapeutic horticulture for clinical depression. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 32(1), 73-81. Gonzalez, M. T., Hartig, T., Patil, G. G., Martinsen, E. W., & Kirkevold, M. (2011b). A prospective study of group cohesiveness in therapeutic horticulture for clinical depression. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 20(2), 119-129. Gorman, R., & Cacciatore, J. (2017). Cultivating our humanity: A systematic review of care farming & traumatic grief. Health & Place, 47, 12-21. Grahn, P. (2011). Om stödjande miljöer och rofyllda ljud [On supportive environments and restful sounds]. In F. Mossberg (Ed.), Ljudmiljö, hälsa och stadsbygnad (pp. 42-55). Lund, Sweden: Ljudmiljöcentrum, Lunds universitet. Grahn, P., Ivarsson, C. T., Stigsdotter, U. K., & Bengtsson, I.-L. (2010). Using affordances as a health-promoting tool in a therapeutic garden. Innovative Approaches to Researching Landscape and Health, 1(5), 116-154. Grahn, P., Pálsdóttir, A. M., Ottosson, J., & Jonsdottir, I. H. (2017). Longer nature-based rehabilitation may contribute to a faster return to work in patients with reactions to severe stress and/or depression. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14(11), 1-17. Graneheim, U. H., & Lundman, B. (2004). Qualitative content analysis in nursing research: Concepts, procedures and measures to achieve trustworthiness. Nurse Education Today, 24(2), 105- 112. Granerud, A., & Eriksson, B. G. (2014). Mental health problems, recovery, and the impact of green care services: A qualitative, participant-focused approach. Occupational Therapy in Mental Health, 30(4), 317-336. Grevenstein, D., Bluemke, M., & Kroeninger-Jungaberle, H. (2016). Incremental validity of sense of coherence, neuroticism, extraversion, and general self-efficacy: Longitudinal prediction of substance use frequency and mental health. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 14(1), 1-14. Griffiths, C. A. (2010). A critical analysis of Antonovsky's sense of coherence theory in relation to mental health and mental health

110

disorder and the effect of a lifelong learning intervention on the sense of coherence of mental health users. (Doctor of Philosophy), Middelsex University, Middelsex, UK. Guldvåg, B. (2017, 23.02.2017). Helsedirektørens årlige tale 2017 [Director of Health's Annual Report 2017]. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=1BakLLgNQGQ Görlich, A., Pless, M., Katznelson, N., & Olsen, P. (2011). Hvem er de unge ledige? Unge uden uddannelse og arbejde i Faxe kommune. [Who are the young unemployed? Young people without education and work in Faxe municipality]. Retrieved from http://vbn.aau.dk/files/232187234/hvem_er_de_unge_ledige _web.pdf Hagquist, C., & Andrich, D. (2004). Is the Sense of Coherence- instrument applicable on adolescents? A latent trait analysis using Rasch-modelling. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(4), 955-968. Hakanen, J. J., Feldt, T., & Leskinen, E. (2007). Change and stability of sense of coherence in adulthood: Longitudinal evidence from the healthy child study. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(3), 602-617. Hammer, T., & Hyggen, C. (2013). Ung voksen-risiko for marginalisering [Young adult - risk of marginalisation]. In T. Hammer & C. Hyggen (Eds.), Ung voksen og utenfor. Mestring og marginalisering på vei til voksenliv (pp. 13-27). Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal. Handlin, L., Hydbring-Sandberg, E., Nilsson, A., Ejdebäck, M., Jansson, A., & Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (2011). Short-term interaction between dogs and their owners: Effects on oxytocin, cortisol, insulin and heart rate—an exploratory study. Anthrozoös, 24(3), 301-315. Hansen, R., & Solem, M.-B. (2017). Sosialt arbeid : En situert praksis [Social work: A situated practice]. Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal akademisk. Hassink, J., De Bruin, S. R., Berget, B., & Elings, M. (2017). Exploring the role of farm animals in providing care at care farms. Animals, 7(45), 1-20. Hassink, J., Elings, M., Zweekhorst, M., Van den Nieuwenhuizen, N., & Smit, A. (2010). Care farms in the Netherlands: Attractive empowerment-oriented and strengths-based practices in the community. Health Place, 16(3), 423-430. Hassink, J., & Van Dijk, M. (2006). Farming for health. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.

111

Haubenhofer, D. K., Elings, M., Hassink, J., & Hine, R. (2010). The development of green care in western European countries. Explore, 6(2), 106-111. Haugan, L., Nyland, R., Fjeldavli, E., Meistad, T., & Braastad, B. O. (2006). Green care in Norway: Farms as a resource for the educational, health and social sector. In J. Hassink & M. Van Dijk (Eds.), Farming for health (pp. 109-126). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer. Hauge, H., Kvalem, I. L., Berget, B., Enders-Slegers, M.-J., & Braastad, B. O. (2014). Equine-assisted activities and the impact on perceived social support, self-esteem and self-efficacy among adolescents–an intervention study. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 19(1), 1-21. Heidegger, M. (2007). Væren og tid [Being and time]. Århus, Denmark: Klim. Heitzmann, C. A., & Kaplan, R. M. (1988). Assessment of methods for measuring social support. Health Psychology, 7(1), 75-109. Hine, R., Peacock, J., & Pretty, J. N. (2008). Care farming in the UK: Contexts, benefits and links with therapeutic communities. Therapeutic Communities, 29(3), 245-260. Holmberg, S., Thelin, A., & Stiernström, E.-L. (2004). Relationship of sense of coherence to other psychosocial indices. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 20(4), 227-236. Holmefur, M., Sundberg, K., Wettergren, L., & Langius-Eklöf, A. (2015). Measurement properties of the 13-item Sense of Coherence Scale using Rasch analysis. Quality of Life Research, 24(6), 1455-1463. Howell, A. J., Dopko, R. L., Passmore, H.-A., & Buro, K. (2011). : Associations with well-being and mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(2), 166-171. Howell, A. J., Passmore, H.-A., & Buro, K. (2013). Meaning in nature: Meaning in life as a mediator of the relationship between nature connectedness and well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14(6), 1681-1696. Hummelvoll, J. K., Hopfenbeck, M., & Kogstad, R. (2012). Grønn omsorg i tekst og tale : En diskursorientert tilnærming [Green care in text and speech: A discourse-oriented approach]. Elverum, Norway: Høgskolen i Hedmark. Hyggen, C., & Hammer, T. (2013). Unge voksne-mestring og marginalisering i den norske velferdsstaten [Young adults - mastering and marginalisation in the Norwegian welfare state]. In T. Hammer & C. Hyggen (Eds.), Ung voksen og utenfor.Mestring og marginalisering på vei til voksenliv (pp. 226-233). Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal Akademisk.

112

Iancu, S. C., Hoogendoorn, A. W., Zweekhorst, M. B., Veltman, D. J., Bunders, J. F., & van Balkom, A. J. (2015). Farm-based interventions for people with mental disorders: A systematic review of literature. Disability and rehabilitation, 37(5), 379- 388. Iancu, S. C., Zweekhorst, M. B. M., Veltman, D. J., van Balkom, A. J. L. M., & Bunders, J. F. G. (2014). Mental health recovery on care farms and day centres: A qualitative comparative study of users’ perspectives. Disability and Rehabilitation, 36(7), 573-583. Institute for Health Metrics and Evaluation (IHME). (2016). Norway: State of the nation’s health: Findings from the global burden of disease. Retrieved from http://www.healthdata.org/sites/default/files/files/policy_rep ort/2016/PolicyReport_IHME_GBD-Norway_2016.pdf Joachim, B., Lyon, D. D., & Farrell, S. (2003). Augmenting treatment of obsessive‐compulsive disorder with Antonovsky's sense of coherence theory. Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, 39(4), 163- 168. Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. Journal of , 15(3), 169-182. Katcher, A. H., & Beck, A. M. (1987). Health and caring for living things. Anthrozoös, 1(3), 175-183. Kellert, S. R., & Wilson, E. O. (1993). The biophilia hypothesis. Washington, D.C: Island Press. Knudsen, A., Tollånes, M., Haaland, Ø., Kinge, J., Skirbekk, V., & Vollset, S. (2017). Sykdomsbyrde i Norge 2015. Resultater fra global burden of diseases, injuries, and risk factors study 2015 (GBD 2015)[Disease burden in Norway 2015. Results from the global burden of diseases, injuries, and risk factors study 2015 (GBD 2015)]. Retrieved from https://www.fhi.no/globalassets/dokumenterfiler/rapporter/2 015/sykdomsbyrde_i_norge_2015.pdf Kogstad, R. E., Agdal, R., & Hopfenbeck, M. S. (2014). Narratives of natural recovery: Youth experience of social inclusion through green care. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 11(6), 6052-6068. Kogstad, R. E., Eriksson, B. G., Granerud, A., Hummervoll, J. K., Lauritzen, T., & Batt-Rawden, K. (2012). Håndbok for kjøpere av Inn på tunet-tjenester [Handbook for buyers of Into the farmyard services]. Retrieved from https://www.matmerk.no/cms/files/768/inn-pa-tunet- handbok-for-kjop-av-tjenester

113

Kogstad, R. E., Mønness, E., & Sørensen, T. (2013). Social networks for mental health clients: Resources and solution. Community Mental Health Journal, 49(1), 95-100. Koren, E., & Træen, B. (2003). Jenter og hest [Girls and horses]. Tidsskrift for Ungdomsforskning, 3(2), 3-26. Kvale, S. (1997). Det kvalitative forskningsintervju [The qualitative research interview]. Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal Akademisk. Landsverk, S. S., & Kane, C. F. (1998). Antonovsky's sense of coherence: Theoretical basis of psychoeducation in schizophrenia. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 19(5), 419-431. Langeland, E., Riise, T., Hanestad, B. R., Nortvedt, M. W., Kristoffersen, K., & Wahl, A. K. (2006). The effect of salutogenic treatment principles on coping with mental health problems: A randomised controlled trial. Patient Education and Counseling, 62(2), 212-219. Langeland, E., & Vinje, H. F. (2017). The application of salutogenesis in mental healthcare settings. In M. B. Mittelmark, S. Sagy, M. Eriksson, G. F. Bauer, J. M. Pelikan, B. Lindström, & G. A. Espnes (Eds.), The handbook of salutogenesis (pp. 299-305). Cham, Germany: Springer. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007%2F978-3-319- 04600-6.pdf. Langeland, E., & Wahl, A. K. (2009). The impact of social support on mental health service users’ sense of coherence: A longitudinal panel survey. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 46(6), 830-837. Langeland, E., Wahl, A. K., Kristoffersen, K., & Hanestad, B. R. (2007). Promoting coping: Salutogenesis among people with mental health problems. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 28(3), 275- 295. Leamy, M., Bird, V., Le Boutillier, C., Williams, J., & Slade, M. (2011). Conceptual framework for personal recovery in mental health: Systematic review and narrative synthesis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 199(6), 445-452. Leck, C., Upton, D., & Evans, N. (2013). Social aspects of green care. In C. Gallis (Ed.), Green Care. For human therapy, social innovation, rural economy, and education (pp. 155-188). New York, NY: Nova Biomedical. Leck, C., Upton, D., & Evans, N. (2015). Growing well-beings: The positive experience of care farms. British Journal of Health Psychology, 20(4), 745-762. Lerdal, A., Fagermoen, M. S., Bonsaksen, T., Gay, C. L., & Kottorp, A. (2014). Rasch analysis of the Sense of Coherence Scale in a sample of people with morbid obesity – a cross-sectional study. BMC Psychology, 2(1), 1-10.

114

Lerdal, A., Opheim, R., Gay, C. L., Moum, B., Fagermoen, M. S., & Kottorp, A. (2017). Psychometric limitations of the 13-item Sense of Coherence Scale assessed by Rasch analysis. BMC Psychology, 5(18), 1-8. Levin, I., & Ellingsen, I. T. (2015). Relasjoner i sosialt arbeid [Relations in social work]. In I. T. Ellingesen, I. Levin, B. Berg, & L. C. Kleppe (Eds.), Sosialt arbeid. En grunnbok (pp. 112-124). Oslo, Norway: Universitetsforlaget. Levinson, B. M. (1962). The dog as a" co-therapist". Mental Hygiene, 46, 59-65. Lian, O. S. (2004). Sosiologiske forståelser av helse, sykdom og medisinsk praksis [Sociological understandings of health, illness and medical practice]. In J. G. Mæland, P. Fuggeli, G. Høyer, & S. Westin (Eds.), Sosialmedisin - i teori og praksis (pp. 101-117). Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal Akademisk. Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Lindström, B., & Eriksson, M. (2015). Haikerens guide til salutogenese: Helsefremmende arbeid i et salutogent perspektiv [The hitchiker's guide to salutogenesis. Salutogenic pathways to health promotion]. Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal Akademisk. Lingås, L. G. (2003). Tanker om verdigrunnlaget i sosialt arbeid [Thoughts on the value base in social work]. In M.-A. Zahl (Ed.), Sosialt arbeid. Refleksjon og handling (pp. 77-97). Bergen, Norway: Fagbokforlaget. Long, T., & Johnson, M. (2000). Rigour, reliability and validity in qualitative research. Clinical Effectiveness in Nursing, 4(1), 30- 37. Lund, I. E., Granerud, A., & Eriksson, B. G. (2015). Green care from the provider’s perspective: An insecure position facing different social worlds. SAGE Open, 5(1), 1-10. Länsimies, H., Pietilä, A.-M., Hietasola-Husu, S., & Kangasniemi, M. (2017). A systematic review of adolescents’ sense of coherence and health. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 31(4), 651-661. Maas, J., Verheij, R. A., Groenewegen, P. P., De Vries, S., & Spreeuwenberg, P. (2006). Green space, urbanity, and health: How strong is the relation? Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 60(7), 587-592. Mallon, G. P. (1994). Cow as a co-therapist: Utilization of farm animals as therapeutic aides with children in residential treatment. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 11(6), 455-474.

115

Mancini, J. A., & Blieszner, R. (1992). Social provisions in adulthood: Concept and measurement in close relationships. Journal of Gerontology, 47(1), 14-20. Marsh, S. C., Clinkinbeard, S. S., Thomas, R. M., & Evans, W. P. (2007). Risk and protective factors predictive of sense of coherence during adolescence. Journal of Health Psychology, 12(2), 281- 284. Matmerk. (2018). Finn Inn på tunet gård [Find' Into the farmyard' services]. Retrieved from http://www.matmerk.no/no/inn-pa- tunet/new-finn-gaard Mayer, F. S., & Frantz, C. M. (2004). The connectedness to nature scale: A measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(4), 503-515. Mayeroff, M. (1990). On caring. New York, NY: HarperPerennial. McKee-Ryan, F., Song, Z., Wanberg, C. R., & Kinicki, A. J. (2005). Psychological and physical well-being during unemployment: A meta-analytic study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(1), 53- 76. McNicholas, J., & Collis, G. M. (2000). Dogs as catalysts for social interactions: Robustness of the effect. British Journal of Psychology, 91(1), 61-70. McNicholas, J., & Collis, G. M. (2006). Animals as social supports: Insights for understanding animal-assisted therapy. In A. Fine (Ed.), Animal-assisted therapy. Theoretical foundations and guidelines for practice. (pp. 49-71). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Medin, J., & Alexanderson, K. (2000). Begreppen hälsa och hälsofrämjande : En litteraturstudie [The concepts of health and health promotion: A literature study]. Lund, Sweden: Studentlitteratur. Meehan, M., Massavelli, B., & Pachana, N. (2017). Using attachment theory and social support theory to examine and measure pets as sources of social support and attachment figures. Anthrozoös, 30(2), 273-289. Milner, A., Krnjacki, L., Butterworth, P., & LaMontagne, A. D. (2016). The role of social support in protecting mental health when employed and unemployed: A longitudinal fixed-effects analysis using 12 annual waves of the HILDA cohort. Social Science & Medicine, 153, 20-26. Mittelmark, M. B., & Bauer, G. F. (2017). The meanings of salutogenesis. In M. B. Mittelmark, S. Sagy, M. Eriksson, G. F. Bauer, J. M. Pelikan, B. Lindström, & G. A. Espnes (Eds.), The handbook of salutogenesis (pp. 7-13). Cham, Germany: Springer. Retrieved from

116

https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007%2F978-3-319- 04600-6.pdf. Moksnes, U. K., Espnes, G. A., & Haugan, G. (2014). Stress, sense of coherence and emotional symptoms in adolescents. Psychology & Health, 29(1), 32-49. NAV. (2011). Veilder for NAV-kontoret. Grønt arbeid en god start på veien mot arbeidslivet [Guideline for NAV offices. Green work a good start on the way to working life]. Retrieved from https://www.nav.no/no/Person/Arbeid/Oppfolging+og+tiltak +for+a+komme+i+jobb/Relatert+informasjon/_attachment/8 05337700?_ts=137c19305b8&download=true NAV. (2014). Mottakere av uføretrygd etter hoveddiagnose (primærdiagnose), alder og kjønn. Pr. 31.12.2014. [Recievers of disability pension after main diagnosis, age and gender. 31.12.2014]. Retrieved from https://www.nav.no/no/NAV+og+samfunn/Statistikk/AAP+n edsatt+arbeidsevne+og+uforetrygd+- +statistikk/Tabeller/mottakere-av-uf%C3%B8retrygd-etter- hoveddiagnose-prim%C3%A6rdiagnose-alder-og- kj%C3%B8nn.pr.31.12.2014.kvinne NAV. (2017). What is NAV? Retrieved from https://www.nav.no/en/Home/About+NAV/What+is+NAV NAV. (2018a). Mottakere av arbeidsavklaringspenger. Diagnose og alder. Januar-august 2018 [Recipients of work assesment allowances. Diagnosis and age. January-August 2018]. Retrieved from https://www.nav.no/no/NAV+og+samfunn/Statistikk/AAP+n edsatt+arbeidsevne+og+uforetrygd+- +statistikk/Arbeidsavklaringspenger NAV. (2018b). Mottakere av arbeidsavklaringspenger. Diagnose og alder. Juni 2014-2018 [Recipients of work assesment allowances. Diagnosis and age. June 2014-2018]. Retrieved from https://www.nav.no/no/NAV+og+samfunn/Statistikk/AAP+n edsatt+arbeidsevne+og+uforetrygd+- +statistikk/Arbeidsavklaringspenger NAV. (2018c). Mottakere av uføretrygd, etter kjønn og alder. Pr. 30.06.2009 - 2018 [Recipients of disability pensions, gender and age. 30.06.2009 - 2018]. Retrieved from https://www.nav.no/541791/mottakere-av-uf%C3%B8retrygd- etter-kj%C3%B8nn-og-alder.pr.30.06.2009-2018.antall Nes, R. B., & Clench-Aas, J. (2011). Psykisk helse i Norge. Tilstandsrapport med internasjonale sammenligninger [Mental health in Norway. State of the art with international comparisons]. Retrieved from https://www.fhi.no/globalassets/dokumenterfiler/rapporter/2

117

011/rapport-20112-psykisk-helse-i-norge.-tilstandsrapport- med-internasjonale-sammenligninger..pdf Nielsen, A. M., & Hansson, K. (2007). Associations between adolescents' health, stress and sense of coherence. Stress and Health, 23(5), 331-341. Nightingale, F. (1859). Notes on nursing. What is it and what is it not. (4 ed.). London, UK: Duckworth. Nilsson, B., Holmgren, L., Stegmayr, B., & Westman, G. (2003). Sense of coherence-stability over time and relation to health, disease, and psychosocial changes in a general population: A longitudinal study. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 31(4), 297-304. Nilsson, B., Holmgren, L., & Westman, G. (2000). Sense of coherence in different stages of health and disease in northern Sweden: Gender and psychosocial differences. Scandinavian Journal of Primary Health Care, 18(1), 14-20. Nilsson, K. W., Leppert, J., Simonsson, B., & Starrin, B. (2010). Sense of coherence and psychological well-being: Improvement with age. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 64(4), 347-352. Nissen, M., & Skærbæk, E. (2014). Hva er psykososialt arbeid? [What is psychosocial work?]. In E. Skærbæk & M. Nissen (Eds.), Psykososialt arbeid. Fortellinger, medvirkning og fellesskap. (pp. 11-28). Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal akademisk. Noble-Carr, D., Barker, J., McArthur, M., & Woodman, E. (2014). Improving practice: The importance of connections in establishing positive identity and meaning in the lives of vulnerable young people. Children and Youth Services Review, 47, 389-396. Nordenfelt, L. (2018). Functions and health: Towards a praxis-oriented concept of health. Biological Theory, 13(1), 10-16. Norwegian ministry of agriculture and food, & Norwegian ministry of local government and regional development. (2012). Green Care. National strategy. Retrieved from https://www.regjeringen.no/globalassets/upload/lmd/vedlegg /brosjyrer_veiledere_rapporter/m- 0734_green_care_national_strategy.pdf NOVA. (2014). Ungdata. Nasjonale resultater 2014. [Young data. National results 2014]. Oslo, Norway: NOVA Retrieved from http://www.hioa.no/Om-HiOA/Senter-for-velferds-og- arbeidslivsforskning/NOVA/Publikasjonar/Rapporter/2015/U ngdata.-Nasjonale-resultater-2014. NOVA. (2016). Ungdata [Young data]. Retrieved from http://www.ungdata.no/English Oades, L., Deane, F. P., & Anderson, J. (2013). Peer support in a mental health service context. In R. King, C. Lloyd, T. Meehan, F. P.

118

Deane, & D. Kavanagh (Eds.), Manual of psychosocial rehabilitation (pp. 185-193). West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons. Odendaal, J. S., & Meintjes, R. A. (2003). Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative behaviour between humans and dogs. The Veterinary Journal, 165(3), 296-301. Ohly, H., White, M. P., Wheeler, B. W., Bethel, A., Ukoumunne, O. C., Nikolaou, V., & Garside, R. (2016). Attention restoration theory: A systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health, Part B, 19(7), 305-343. Pallant, J. F., & Tennant, A. (2007). An introduction to the Rasch measurement model: An example using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS). British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46(1), 1-18. Pàlsdòttir, A. M., Persson, D., Persson, B., & Grahn, P. (2014). The journey of recovery and empowerment embraced by nature — Clients’ perspectives on nature-based rehabilitation in relation to the role of the natural environment. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 11(7), 7094-7115. Pálsdóttir, A. M., Stigsdotter, U. K., Persson, D., Thorpert, P., & Grahn, P. (2018). The qualities of natural environments that support the rehabilitation process of individuals with stress-related mental disorder in nature-based rehabilitation. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 29, 312-321. Pedersen, I., Ihlebæk, C., & Kirkevold, M. (2012). Important elements in farm animal-assisted interventions for persons with clinical depression: A qualitative interview study. Disability and Rehabilitation, 34(18), 1526-1534. Pedersen, I., Martinsen, E. W., Berget, B., & Braastad, B. O. (2012). Farm animal-assisted intervention for people with clinical depression: A randomized controlled trial. Anthrozoös, 25(2), 149-160. Pedersen, I., Nordaunet, T., Martinsen, E. W., Berget, B., & Braastad, B. O. (2011). Farm animal-assisted intervention: Relationship between work and contact with farm animals and change in depression, anxiety, and self-efficacy among persons with clinical depression. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 32(8), 493-500. Polit, D. F., & Hungler, B. P. (1999). Nursing research : Principles and methods (6 ed.). Philadelphia, PE: Lippincott. Posadzki, P., Stockl, A., Musonda, P., & Tsouroufli, M. (2010). A mixed‐ method approach to sense of coherence, health behaviors, self‐ efficacy and optimism: Towards the operationalization of

119 positive health attitudes. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51(3), 246-252. Potrebny, T., Wiium, N., & Lundegård, M. M.-I. (2017). Temporal trends in adolescents’ self-reported psychosomatic health complaints from 1980-2016: A systematic review and meta- analysis. PloS One, 12(11), 1-24. Pretty, J. (2004). How nature contributes to mental and physical health. Spirituality and Health International, 5(2), 68-78. Price-Robertson, R., Obradovic, A., & Morgan, B. (2017). Relational recovery: Beyond individualism in the recovery approach. Advances in Mental Health, 15(2), 108-120. Rajesh, G., Eriksson, M., Pai, K., Seemanthini, S., Naik, D. G., & Rao, A. (2016). The validity and reliability of the Sense of Coherence Scale among Indian university students. Global Health Promotion, 23(4), 16-26. Ramsdal, G. H., Bergvik, S., & Wynn, R. (2018). Long-term dropout from school and work and mental health in young adults in Norway: A qualitative interview-based study. Cogent Psychology, 5(1), 1-16. Rappe, E., Koivunen, T., & Korpela, E. (2008). Group gardening in mental outpatient care. Therapeutic Communities: the International Journal for Therapeutic and Supportive Organizations, 29(3), 273-284. Rasch, G. (1980). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests (Expanded ed.). Chicago, Ill: University of Chicago Press. Reneflot, A., Aarø, L. E., Aase, H., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Tambs, K., & Øverland, S. (2018). Psykisk helse i Norge [Mental health in Norway]. Retrieved from https://www.fhi.no/globalassets/dokumenterfiler/rapporter/2 018/psykisk_helse_i_norge2018.pdf Restall, B., & Conrad, E. (2015). A literature review of connectedness to nature and its potential for environmental management. Journal of Environmental Management, 159, 264-278. Ringdal, K. (2018). Enhet og mangfold : Samfunnsvitenskapelig forskning og kvantitativ metode [Unity and diversity: Social science research and quantitative method] (4 ed.). Bergen, Norway: Fagbokforlaget. Roszak, T., Gomes, M. E., & Kanner, A. D. (1995). Ecopsychology : Restoring the earth, healing the mind. San Francisco, CA: Sierra Club Books. Rothe, E. Q., Vega, B. J., Torres, R. M., Soler, S. M. C., & Pazos, R. M. M. (2005). From kids and horses: Equine facilitated psychotherapy for children. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 5(2), 373-383.

120

Rousing, T., & Wemelsfelder, F. (2006). Qualitative assessment of social behaviour of dairy cows housed in loose housing systems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 101(1), 40-53. Rueger, S. Y., Malecki, C. K., Pyun, Y., Aycock, C., & Coyle, S. (2016). A meta-analytic review of the association between perceived social support and depression in childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 142(10), 1017-1067. Sahlin, E., Matuszczyk, J. V., Ahlborg, J., Gunnar, & Grahn, P. (2012). How do participants in nature-based therapy experience and evaluate their rehabilitation? Journal of Therapeutic Horticulture, 22(1), 8-22. Salomon, R. E., Salomon, A. D., & Beeber, L. S. (2017). Green care as psychosocial intervention for depressive symptoms: What might be the key ingredients? Journal of the American Psychiatric Nurses Association, 24(3), 199-208. Schreuder, E., Rijnders, M., Vaandrager, L., Hassink, J., Enders- Slegers, M.-J., & Kennedy, L. (2014). Exploring salutogenic mechanisms of an outdoor experiential learning programme on youth care farms in the Netherlands: Untapped potential? International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 19(2), 139-152. Schulenberg, J. E., & Maggs, J. L. (2002). A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Supplement(14), 54-70. Sempik, J., & Bragg, R. (2013). Green care: Origins and activities. In C. Gallis (Ed.), Green Care. For human therapy, social innovation, rural economy and education (pp. 11-31). New York, NY: Nova Biomedical. Sempik, J., Hine, R., & Wilcox, D. (2010). Green care: A conceptual framework. A report of the working group on health benefits of green care. Retrieved from http://www.agrarumweltpaedagogik.ac.at/cms/upload/bilder/ green_care_a_conceptual_framework.pdf Sempik, J., Rickhuss, C., & Beeston, A. (2014). The effects of social and therapeutic horticulture on aspects of social behaviour. The British Journal of Occupational Therapy, 77(6), 313-319. Serpell, J., McCune, S., Gee, N., & Griffin, J. A. (2017). Current challenges to research on animal-assisted interventions. Applied Developmental Science, 21(3), 223-233. Settersten, R. A., Furstenberg, F. F., & Rumbaut, R. G. (2005). On the frontier of adulthood : Theory, research, and public policy. Chicago, Ill.: University of Chicago Press. Siqveland, J., Moum, T., & Leiknes, K. A. (2016). Måleegenskaper ved den norske versjonen av symptom checklist 90 revidert (SCL- 90-R). [Psychometric properties of Norwegian version of

121

symptoms checklist 90 revised (SCL-90-R)]. Retrieved from https://www.fhi.no/globalassets/dokumenterfiler/rapporter/m aleegenskaper-ved-den-norske-versjonen-av-symptom- checklist-90-rapport-2016.pdf Skärsäter, J., Langius, A., & Ågren, H. (2005). Sense of coherence and social support in relation to recovery in first-episode patients with major depression: A one-year prospective study. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 14(4), 258- 264. Social. (2018). Social. In English Oxford Living Dictionaries. Solem, M.-B. (2017). Styrkebaserte perspektiver [Strenghtbased perspectives]. In R. Hansen & M.-B. Solem (Eds.), Sosialt arbeid. En situert praksis (pp. 94-114). Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal akademisk. Solstad, A. (2017). Sosialt arbeid i en velferdspolitisk kontekst [Social work in a welfare political context]. In R. Hansen & M.-B. Solem (Eds.), Sosialt arbeid. En situert praksis (pp. 331-355). Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal Akademisk. Statistics Norway. (2017). Idrett og friluftsliv, levekårsundersøkelsen [Sports and outdoor activities, survey on living conditions]. Retrieved from https://www.ssb.no/fritid/ Statistics Norway. (2018a). Ekteskap og skilsmisser [Marriages and divorces]. Retrieved from https://www.ssb.no/statbank/table/05866 Statistics Norway. (2018b). Foreldrenes gjennomsnittlige fødealder ved første barns fødsel 1961-2017. [Avergae age for parents when having their first child 1961-2017]. Retrieved from https://www.ssb.no/statbank/table/07872 Strand, B. H., Dalgard, O. S., Tambs, K., & Rognerud, M. (2003). Measuring the mental health status of the Norwegian population: A comparison of the instruments SCL-25, SCL-10, SCL-5 and MHI-5 (SF-36). Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 57(2), 113-118. Thoits, P. A. (2011). Mechanisms linking social ties and support to physical and mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 52(2), 145-161. Toft, H., Neupane, S. P., Bramness, J. G., Tilden, T., Wampold, B. E., & Lien, L. (2018). The effect of trauma and alcohol on the relationship between level of cytokines and depression among patients entering psychiatric treatment. BMC Psychiatry, 18(95), 1-10. Tordsson, B. (2003). Å svare på naturens åpne tiltale : en undersøkelse av meningsdimensjoner i norsk friluftsliv på 1900-tallet og en drøftelse av friluftsliv som sosiokulturelt fenomen [To respond to nature's open request: a survey of

122

opinion dimensions in Norwegian outdoor life in the 20th century and a discussion of outdoor life as a socio-cultural phenomenon]. (Philosophiae Doctor (PhD) Thesis), Norges idrettshøgskole, Institutt for samfunnsfag, Oslo, Norway. Toyoda, M. (2013). Green care in Japan. In C. Gallis (Ed.), Green Care. For human therapy, social innovation, rural economy and education (pp. 289-304). New York, NY: Nova Biomedical. Tuke, D. H. (1882). Chapters in the history of the insane in the British Isles. London, UK.: Kegan Paul Trench. Turner, R. J., & Brown, R. L. (2010). Social support and mental health. In T. L. Scheid & T. N. Brown (Eds.), A handbook for the study of mental health: Social contexts, theories, and systems (2. ed., pp. 200-212). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Uchino, B. N. (2004). Social support and physical health : Understanding the health consequences of relationships. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press. Ulrich, R. S. (1993). Biophilia, biophobia, and natural landscapes. In S. R. Kellert & E. O. Wilson (Eds.), The biophilia hypothesis (pp. 73-137). Washington, DC: Island Press. Van den Berg, A. E., Maas, J., Verheij, R. A., & Groenewegen, P. P. (2010). Green space as a buffer between stressful life events and health. Social Science & Medicine, 70(8), 1203-1210. Van Droogenbroeck, F., Spruyt, B., & Keppens, G. (2018). Gender differences in mental health problems among adolescents and the role of social support: Results from the Belgian health interview surveys 2008 and 2013. BMC Psychiatry, 18(6), 1-9. Vik, J., & Farstad, M. (2009). Green care governance: Between market, policy and intersecting social worlds. Journal of Health, Organisation and Management, 23(5), 539-553. Volanen, S. M., Lahelma, E., Silventoinen, K., & Suominen, S. (2004). Factors contributing to sense of coherence among men and women. The European Journal of Public Health, 14(3), 322-330. Volanen, S. M., Suominen, S., Lahelma, E., Koskenvuo, M., & Silventoinen, K. (2007). Negative life events and stability of sense of coherence: A five‐year follow‐up study of Finnish women and men. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 48(5), 433-441. von Lindern, E., Lymeus, F., & Hartig, T. (2017). The restorative environment: A complementary concept for salutogenesis studies. In M. B. Mittelmark, S. Sagy, M. Eriksson, G. F. Bauer, J. M. Pelikan, B. Lindström, & G. A. Espnes (Eds.), The handbook of salutogenesis (pp. 181-195). Cham, Germany: Springer. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007%2F978-3-319- 04600-6.pdf.

123

von Soest, T., & Hyggen, C. (2013). Psykiske plager blant ungdom og unge voksne–hva vet vi om utviklingen i de siste årtiene? [Mental illnesses in youth and young adults - What do we know about developments in recent decades?]. In T. Hammer & C. Hyggen (Eds.), Ung voksen og utenfor. Mestring og marginalitet på vei til voksenlivet (pp. 88-109). Oslo, Norway: Gyldendal Akademisk. Walker, I., & Read, J. (2002). The differential effectiveness of psychosocial and biogenetic causal explanations in reducing negative attitudes toward “mental illness”. Psychiatry, 65(4), 313-325. Wang, R. A. H., Davis, O. S., Wootton, R. E., Mottershaw, A., & Haworth, C. M. (2017). Social support and mental health in late adolescence are correlated for genetic, as well as environmental, reasons. Scientific Reports, 7, 1-14. Weiss, R. S. (1974). The provisions of social relationships. In Z. Rubin (Ed.), Doing unto others: Joining, molding, conforming, helping, loving (pp. 17-26). Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice- Hall. Weissbecker, I., Salmon, P., Studts, J. L., Floyd, A. R., Dedert, E. A., & Sephton, S. E. (2002). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and sense of coherence among women with fibromyalgia. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 9(4), 297-307. Whittemore, R., & Knafl, K. (2005). The integrative review: Updated methodology. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 52(5), 546-553. WHO. (1948). Constitution. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/about/mission/en/ WHO. (2015). The European mental health action plan 2013-2020. Retrieved from http://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0020/2806 04/WHO-Europe-Mental-Health-Acion-Plan-2013-2020.pdf WHO. (2018). Adolescents: Health risks and solutions. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/en/news-room/fact- sheets/detail/adolescents-health-risks-and-solutions Wills, T. A. (1991). Support and interpersonal relationships. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Prosocial behavior (pp. 265-289). London, UK: Sage Publications. Wills, T. A., & Shinar, O. (2000). Measuring percieved and recieved social support. In L. G. Underwood, S. Cohen, & B. H. Gottlieb (Eds.), Social support measurement and intervention (pp. 86- 135). Cary, NC: Oxford University Press. Wilson, E. O. (1984). Biophilia. Cambridge, Mass. : Harvard University Press. Wolfe, E. W., & Smith, E. V. (2007a). Instrument development tools and activities for measure validation using Rasch models: Part I

124

- instrument development tools. Journal of Applied Measurement, 8(1), 97-123. Wolfe, E. W., & Smith, E. V. (2007b). Instrument development tools and activities for measure validation using Rasch models: Part II - validation activities. Journal of Applied Measurement, 8(2), 204-234. Wolff, A. C., & Ratner, P. A. (1999). Stress, social support, and sense of coherence. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 21(2), 182- 197. Ziehe, T. (2012). Tidstypiska menings- och handlingskriser hos ungdomar [Meaning- and actioncrises in adolecents typical for the period]. Retrieved from https://old.liu.se/uv/lararrummet/venue/tidstypiska-menings- och-handlingskriser-hos-ungdomar?l=sv

125

Appendices

Appendix 1

Spørreskjema om erfaringer med ulike Grønn omsorg tiltak blant unge mellom 16-30 år

Dato når spørreskjemaet er fylt ut:______Appendix 1

Om undersøkelsen:

Denne studien handler om ulike Grønn omsorg tiltak og er rettet mot personer som er aktive deltagere i slike tiltak.

I dette spørreskjemaet vil det bli stilt spørsmål om deg, tiltaket du er på og hvordan du opplever å være der. Hva som fører til opplevelse av sammenheng i tilværelsen, nettverksstøtte og selvopplevd helse. Spørreskjemaet tar ca. 30 minutter å fylle ut.

Om noe skulle være uklart i spørreskjemaet er det bare å ta kontakt på telefon eller mail.

Deltagelse i denne undersøkelsen er frivillig. Appendix 1

Bakgrunn

Først stiller vi noen spørsmål om deg og tiltaket du er på. Tiltaket brukes i spørsmålene som betegnelse på det tilbudet du har innen Grønn omsorg/Grønt arbeid/Inn på tunet, altså den gården eller det stedet du er på.

Sett kryss i ruten foran det svaralternativet som passer best for deg.

1. Hvilket kjønn er du?  Kvinne  Mann

2. Hvor gammel er du?:______(antall år)

3. Hvilket fylke bor du i?  Østfold  Akershus  Oslo  Hedmark  Oppland  Sør-Trøndelag  Nord-Trøndelag  Nordland  Troms  Finnmark  Møre og Romsdal  Sogn og Fjordane  Hordaland  Rogaland  Vest-Agder  Aust- Agder Appendix 1

 Telemark  Buskerud  Vestfold

4. Hva er din høyeste fullførte utdanning?  Ungdomsskole  Videregående skole, avsluttet med studiekompetanse  Videregående skole, yrkesfag  Videregående skole, fagbrev (gjennomført lærling tiden og fått fagbrev)  Universitet og høgskoleutdanning

5. Hva gjorde du rett før du fikk tilbudet på tiltaket du nå er på? (her er flere svar mulige)  Var yrkesaktiv, sykemeldt eller i medisinsk rehabilitering  Var arbeidssøker eller var sysselsatt (hadde jobb) via NAV  Var student/skoleelev  Var hjemmeværende  Hadde stønad fra NAV (sosialhjelp, uføretrygd og lignende)

6. Hvilke hjelpetilbud har du erfaring fra: (Her er flere svaralternativer mulig)  Skolehelsetjeneste/helsesøster  Barnevernet  PP- tjenesten (Pedagogisk psykologisk tjeneste)  NAV (arbeid, trygd og sosialtjeneste)  DPS (distriktspsykiatrisk senter)  Behandlingsinstitusjon (døgnopphold på mer enn 1 uke)  Kommunalt tilbud innen psykisk helse/rus  BUP (Barne- og ungdomspsykiatrisk poliklinikk)  Vet ikke (har fått hjelp, men husker ikke/vet ikke fra hvem)  Ingen

Appendix 1

7. Vil du si at din anvendelse av rusmidler er en medvirkende årsak til at du er på Grønn omsorg tiltaket?  Ja  Nei  Vet ikke

8. Vil du si at din psykiske helse er en medvirkende årsak til at du er på Grønn omsorg tiltaket?  Ja  Nei  Vet ikke

9. Hvilken instans har skaffet deg det tilbudet i Grønn omsorg som du er på nå?  NAV  Kommunen  BUP/DPS  Skolen  Vet ikke  Andre. Evt. hvem…?

Appendix 1

Spørsmål om deg, og tiltaket du er på

10. Hadde du hørt om slike steder som tilbudet du er på (Grønn omsorg/grønt arbeid), hvor man kan arbeide med dyr og natur før du begynte på tiltaket?  Ja  Nei  Vet ikke

11. Hadde du spesiell interesse for det du nå jobber med (grønn omsorg/grønt arbeid), før du fikk tilbud om en plass her?  Nei, jeg hadde ingen spesiell interesse  Ja, jeg ønsket selv å få en plass innen grønn omsorg/grønt arbeid  Annet. Evt. hva?......

12. Hvilke forventninger hadde du til å begynne på gården/tiltaket? (gledet eller gruet du deg?)  Jeg hadde positive forventninger (gledet meg)  Jeg hadde ingen forventninger (verken gruet eller gledet meg)  Jeg hadde negative forventninger (gruet meg)  Både og (både gledet og gruet meg)

13. Hva gjør du når du er på gården/tiltaket? (Her er flere svar mulig)  Steller/arbeider med dyr  Jobber inne/ i verksted, med maskiner, tre eller lignende  Jobber ute, med vedlikehold, snekring, ved eller lignende  Er ute på tur i naturen  Lager mat, holder på med håndarbeid, eller lignende  En kombinasjon av ulike aktiviteter  Annet. Eventuelt hva?

Appendix 1

14. Hvilke av disse aktivitetene gjør du oftest når du er på tiltaket? (kun et svar er mulig).  Steller/arbeider med dyr  Jobber inne/ i verksted, med maskiner, tre eller lignende  Jobber ute, med vedlikehold, snekring, ved eller lignende  Er ute på tur i naturen  Lager mat, holder på med håndarbeid, eller lignende  En kombinasjon av ulike aktiviteter  Annet. Eventuelt hva?

15. Hvilke dyr er det på gården/tiltaket? (Her er flere svar mulig)  Det er ikke dyr på gården/tiltaket  Katt  Hund  Smådyr (hamster, kanin, marsvin, mus og lignende)  Hest  Ku  Sau/gris/geit  Annet. Eventuelt hva?

16. Har du eller har du tidligere hatt dyr, eller arbeidet med dyr? (her er flere svaralternativer mulig)  Nei, har verken hatt eller arbeidet med dyr tidligere  Ja, Katt  Ja, Hund  Ja, Smådyr (hamster, kanin, marsvin, mus og lignende)  Ja, Hest  Ja, Ku  Ja, Sau/gris/geit  Annet. Eventuelt hva?

Appendix 1

17. Trives du med det du gjør på gården/tiltaket?  Trives veldig godt med det jeg gjør  Trives ganske godt med det jeg gjør  Trives med noe av det jeg gjør  Trives ikke med det jeg gjør  Vet ikke/er usikker

18. Er det noen av aktivitetene du trives spesielt godt med? (et svar mulig)  Stelle/arbeide med dyr  Jobbe inne/ i verksted, med maskiner, tre eller lignende  Jobbe ute, med vedlikehold, snekring, ved eller lignende  Være ute på tur i naturen  Lage mat, holde på med håndarbeid, eller lignende  Trives ikke med noen av aktivitetene  Jeg liker det å kunne kombinere flere aktiviteter

19. Cirka hvor lenge har du vært på tiltaket?  Jeg skal snart begynne/jeg har akkurat begynt  1 måned  2 måneder  3 måneder  4 måneder  5 måneder  6 måneder  7 måneder  8 måneder  9 måneder  10 måneder  11 måneder  12 måneder  Over 12 måneder

Appendix 1

20. Hvor ofte er du vanligvis på tiltaket?  7 dager i uka  6 dager i uka  5 dager i uka  4 dager i uka  3 dager i uka  2 dager i uka  1 dag i uka  Mindre enn 1 gang i uka

21. Hvor lenge er du vanligvis på tiltaket hver gang?  Hele dagen (5 timer eller mer)  4 timer  3 timer  2 timer  1 time  under 1 time  Det varierer

22. I hvilken grad får du den hjelp og veiledning du har behov for i forhold til oppgaver/oppgavene på tiltaket?  I svært stor grad  I stor grad  I noen grad  I liten grad  I svært liten grad

Appendix 1

23. Er du sammen med andre deltagere på gården/tiltaket?  Nei, jeg er alene som deltager på tiltaket  Ja, en liten del av tiden er jeg sammen med andre deltagere  Ja, ca. halvparten av tiden er jeg sammen med andre deltagere  Ja, det meste av tiden er jeg sammen med andre deltagere  Jeg er alltid sammen med andre deltagere

24. Hvor mange deltagere er dere eventuelt på gården/tiltaket samtidig?  Kun meg  2  3-5  6-10  11-15  16-20  Over 20

25. Hvor fornøyd er du samlet sett med tiltaket?  Svært fornøyd  Ganske fornøyd  Blandet  Ganske misfornøyd  Svært misfornøyd

Under kommer noen spørsmål formulert som påstander, hvor du får svaralternativ om hvor godt de stemmer for deg.

26. Det sosiale på tiltaket er viktig for at jeg skal trives der  Stemmer helt  Stemmer delvis  Stemmer knapt  Stemmer ikke

Appendix 1

27. Samværet med de andre deltagerne på gården er viktig for at jeg skal ha det bra der  Stemmer helt  Stemmer delvis  Stemmer knapt  Stemmer ikke

28. Tiltakslederen/tiltakslederne er viktig for at jeg skal ha det bra på tiltaket  Stemmer helt  Stemmer delvis  Stemmer knapt  Stemmer ikke

29. Hvor viktig er dyra på gården/tiltaket for deg?  Er ikke dyr der  Ikke viktig  Litt viktig  Viktig  Veldig viktig

30. Hvor viktig er det for deg å være ute i naturen når du er på gården/tiltaket?  Er ikke ute  Ikke viktig  Litt viktig  Viktig  Veldig viktig

Appendix 1

31. Hva er viktigst for deg på gården/tiltaket? (Ranger fra 1-6, hvor 1 er det som er mest viktig for deg, 2 er det som er nest viktigst osv., og 6 er det som er minst viktig. Hvis det er noen av disse tingene du ikke gjør, lar du være å rangere det. Plasser tallene i boksene foran hvert svaralternativ.

Dyrene

De andre personene på tiltaket

Å få være ute i naturen

Å få jobbe med planter og skog

Arbeidsoppgavene

Bonden/lederen på tiltaket

Appendix 1

Sosiale relasjoner på tiltaket:

Her følger fem påstander om sosiale relasjoner på tiltaket. Det finnes ikke noe «rett» eller «galt» svar. Angi det svaralternativet som passer best for deg.

Stemmer Stemmer Stemmer Stemmer helt delvis knapt ikke Jeg får støtte fra bonden/lederne på tiltaket for å løse problemer

Jeg får støtte fra de andre deltagerne for å løse problemer Bonden/lederne på tiltaket viser at de setter pris på det arbeidet jeg gjør Det er godt samhold blant deltagerne Jeg føler at dyrene på tiltaket kan gi meg sosial støtte på lik linje med andre mennesker Appendix 1

Nå kommer noen spørsmål som ikke er direkte knyttet til tiltaket men som handler om livet ditt generelt.

SOSIAL STØTTE.

Social Provisions Scale er ikke gjengitt her, på grunn av opphavsrettslige grunner. Hopkins Symptoms Checklist

I blant opplever vi at vi ikke har det helt bra. Det kan vi uttrykke som ulike former for plager. Nedenfor er en liste over slike ulike plager. Du skal svare på i hvor stor grad du har vært plaget av noe av dette den siste uken (til og med i dag)?

Sett et kryss for hver plage Ikke Litt Ganske mye Veldig mye plaget plaget plaget plaget

Plutselig frykt uten grunn

Føler deg redd eller engstelig

Matthet eller svimmelhet

Føler deg anspent eller oppjaget

Lett for å klandre deg selv

Søvnproblemer

Nedtrykt, tungsindig (trist)

Følelse av å være unyttig, lite verd

Følelse av at alt er et slit

Følelse av håpløshet mht. framtiden

MESTRING

Sense of Coherence - 13 er ikke gjengitt her på grunn av opphavsrettslige grunner. Appendix 1

Avsluttende spørsmål

1. Har du noen andre kommentarer, eller noe du føler du ikke har fått formidlet?

Tusen takk for at du har ønsket å være med å svare på denne undersøkelsen. Det er vi veldig glade for, og ditt bidrag er veldig viktig for oss i det videre arbeidet.

Hvis du ønsker å delta videre i studien og fylle ut dette spørreskjemaet (en kortere versjon, med noen færre spørsmål enn nå) en gang til om ca. 2 måneder oppgir du kontaktinformasjon under.

Ja, jeg ønsker å delta videre i undersøkelsen og samtykker til å motta et nytt spørreskjema om ca. 2. måneder

Navn:______

Adresse vi kan sende spørreskjemaet til (her kan du velge å oppgi adressen til tiltaket om du ønsker det):______

______

______

Evt. telefonnummer:______

Appendix 2

Intervjuguide: Prosesstudie – ungdoms erfaringer, delstudie 3:

Intervjuguiden har følgende hovedtema, med underspørsmål (Alder, kjønn og bosted registreres): Før – ved oppstart – underveis – ved avslutning – tanker om veien videre

Forventninger til tiltaket og beskrivelser av det - Hvordan trodde du det ville være, ville gå...?

- Hvordan er dagene/programmet – i forhold til hva du forventet (gis opplæring/kvalifisering?)? (se spm. 3 i studie 2)

Individuell tilpasning av tiltaket – Oppleves tiltaket som fleksibelt?

– Har deltakerne kommet med ønsker/blitt spurt om sine ønsker? Ble det tatt hensyn til?

– Hvordan ble det begrunnet hvis ønskene ikke ble tatt hensyn til?

Hva tror deltakerne kunne/ville vært alternativ til tiltaket? - Hva hvis det ikke fantes...?

Hvilke andre tiltak/tjenester mottar brukeren/ er han/hun med på? - Hva består de ulike tiltakene av?

- Hva er forskjellen på dem/ hva er positivt og negativt ved ulike tiltak/tilbud?

Forholdet til de som arbeider ved tiltaket - Hvem er de viktige personene i det daglige?

- Hva er det beste med dem/det de står for?

- Får dine venner og din familie også mulighet til å møte og snakke med de som er på gården?

Forholdet til andre som deltar? - Jobber dere mye sammen?

- Har dere mye felles og er det rom for og muligheter til gode samtaler?

- Danner det seg lett grupper som ikke har så mye kontakt med hverandre?

Forutsigbarhet og koordinering med andre instanser ungdommene må forholde seg til - Fikk du mulighet til å forberede deg på å komme hit – visste du hva det var?

- Planlegger dere hvordan du skal gå videre etter at tiltaksperioden er ferdig

- Får du hjelp til å holde kontakt med skole, NAV, Helsetjeneste osv - går dette greit?

- Hva ser du for deg at du vil gjøre etter at tiltaksperioden er over (og er den lang nok?)?

Kontakten med dyr og natur som en verdi i seg selv - Kan du si noe om hvordan du synes det er å stelle med dyr – og hvilke dyr du liker best?

- Hva er det spesielle i å være i naturen og på gården i forhold til hvordan du lever ellers? Anne Mari Steigen | Social support in nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems

Social support in nature-based services for Social support in nature-based young adults with mental health problems services for young adults with There is increasing interest in the potential for using nature and animals in services and interventions for people with mental health problems. However, the knowledge about nature-based services and participants is limited. The mental health problems overall aim of this thesis is to examine nature-based services for young adults with mental health problems in Norway, focusing on the meaning of different dimensions of social support.

The results indicate that social aspects are important in nature-based services. The young adults participating in the services exhibit more symptoms of mental health problems than a sample of young adults from the general population, Anne Mari Steigen but fewer than a sample of clinical mental health care in-patients. Furthermore, the results show that a variety of dimensions of social support is found in the services and that nature-based services add specific qualities to these dimensions, for instance, through support from animals. For participants struggling with mental health problems and social insecurity, emotional and esteem support, and the opportunity to provide care and support to others may be of particular importance in strengthening their self-esteem and social confidence. 2018:43

ISBN 978-91-7063-879-4 (Print)

ISBN 978-91-7063-974-6 (pdf) Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences

ISSN 1403-8099 Social Work

DOCTORAL THESIS | Karlstad University Studies | 2018:43 DOCTORAL THESIS | Karlstad University Studies | 2018:43