Implicit Learning As a Means of Tonal Jazz Pitch-Listening Skills Acquisition

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Load more

University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Doctoral Dissertations Dissertations and Theses March 2019 Implicit Learning As A Means Of Tonal Jazz Pitch-Listening Skills Acquisition David Mosher University of Massachusetts Amherst Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_2 Part of the Music Pedagogy Commons, and the Music Theory Commons Recommended Citation Mosher, David, "Implicit Learning As A Means Of Tonal Jazz Pitch-Listening Skills Acquisition" (2019). Doctoral Dissertations. 1497. https://doi.org/10.7275/13372877 https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_2/1497 This Open Access Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Dissertations and Theses at ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. IMPLICIT LEARNING AS A MEANS OF TONAL JAZZ PITCH-LISTENING SKILLS ACQUISITION A Dissertation Presented by DAVID MOSHER Submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Massachusetts Amherst in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY February 2019 Music Music Theory © Copyright by David Mosher 2019 All Rights Reserved Implicit Learning As A Means Of Tonal Jazz Pitch-Listening Skills Acquisition A Dissertation Presented By DAVID MOSHER Approved as to style and content by: ____________________________________________________ Gary S. Karpinski, chair ____________________________________________________ Stefan Love, member ____________________________________________________ Felipe Salles, member ____________________________________________________ Lisa Sanders, member ____________________________________________________ Roberta M. Marvin, Department Head Department of Music and Dance DEDICATION To Reneé, Zachary, and Jace. You light my world. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The creation of this document did not happen in a vacuum. Throughout the process, I have been fortunate to have as my advisor, Dr. Gary S. Karpinski, who provided much in the way of thought-provoking conversations and insightful feedback coming from a wealth of knowledge regarding aural skills training from a cognitive- perceptual frame of reference. I am also most certainly indebted to Dr. Stefan Love, who graciously extended to me many knowledgeable suggestions and helpful commentary springing both from a deep knowledge of music theory and of jazz theory in particular. I further extend my sincere gratefulness to Dr. Felipe Salles for imparting to me his incredible experience and firm grasp of jazz musical thought and performance. Foundational to the project was the commentary of Dr. Lisa Sanders, providing me incredible expert advice from the fields of cognition and perception. To my entire committee, I offer my deep appreciation for all of your support. In addition to my committee, I would like to express my thankfulness to friends and family who have encouraged and stuck with me through the ups and downs inevitable in any project of this size and depth. Lastly, and with the most heartfelt and undying affection, I thank my wife, Reneé Kathleen Gonzales Mosher, who has been my enabler. She has been an ever-present source of strength and support without which this project would never have come to completion. v ABSTRACT IMPLICIT LEARNING AS A MEANS OF TONAL JAZZ PITCH-LISTENING SKILLS ACQUISITION FEBRUARY 2019 DAVID MOSHER, B.M., HOWARD PAYNE UNIVERSITY M.M., UNIVERSITY OF HOUSTON Ph.D., UNIVERSITY OF MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST Directed by: Professor Gary S. Karpinski In this dissertation, I present a method for developing tonal jazz pitch-listening skills (PLS) which is rooted in scientific experimental findings from the fields of music cognition and perception. Converging experimental evidence supports the notion that humans develop listening skills through implicit learning via immersive, statistically rich exposure to real music from a particular musical idiom, such as tonal jazz. Therefore, I recommend that to acquire tonal jazz pitch-listening skills, learners should (1) immerse themselves in the real music of that idiom, (2) remediate their listening skills, where necessary, by listening to slowed-down versions with exaggerated features, and (3) organize their listening experiences with explicit theoretical labels for particular pitch structures, if they want to communicate about those pitch structures in speech or writing. In order to aid in the practical application of this process, I offer a four-semester learning sequence for the development of tonal jazz pitch-listening skills as well as a variety of formal assessment methods. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS .............................................................................................. v ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................... vi LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................................................... xi CHAPTER 1. THE FRAMING AND SCOPE OF THE TOPIC ................................................. 1 1.1 Introduction ......................................................................................................... 1 1.2 Statement of the Problem .................................................................................... 1 1.3 Research Questions ............................................................................................. 2 1.4 Significance of the Study .................................................................................... 2 1.5 Methodology ....................................................................................................... 3 1.6 Principles Underlying My Recommendations .................................................... 4 1.7 Organization of the Study ................................................................................... 5 1.8 Limitations .......................................................................................................... 5 2. SUMMARY OF MUSIC COGNITION AND PERCEPTION LITERATURE ON PITCH PROCESSING.............................................................................................. 7 2.1 Preliminaries ....................................................................................................... 7 2.1.1 The Relevance of Pitch-Listening Skill Development to Jazz Musicians . 7 2.1.2 Pitch: Definition and Scope of Engagement .............................................. 8 2.1.3 The Rate of PLS Development .................................................................. 9 2.1.4 Tonality .................................................................................................... 10 2.1.5 The Problem of Function: Anatomy and Physiology of Pitch Entities .... 11 2.1.6 Implicit Learning Explained .................................................................... 13 2.2 A List of Some Important PLS ......................................................................... 16 2.3 Expectations ...................................................................................................... 17 2.3.1 Memory and Expectation ......................................................................... 18 2.3.2 Creating a Set of Expectations ................................................................. 20 2.3.3 Melodic Structure Across Stylistic Boundaries ....................................... 21 2.3.4 Melodic Expectations Across Stylistic Boundaries ................................. 23 2.3.5 Dynamically Unfolding Melodic Expectations ....................................... 26 2.3.6 Musical Statistical Regularities................................................................ 28 2.3.7 General Musical Expectations ................................................................. 29 vii 2.3.8 Shared Canon, Shared Expectations, Room for Evolution ...................... 31 2.3.9 Style-Specific Expectations ..................................................................... 32 2.3.10 Style-Specific Melodic Expectations ..................................................... 34 2.4 The Effects of Context on PLS ......................................................................... 35 2.5 The Effects of Musical Training on PLS .......................................................... 37 2.5.1 Musicians and Nonmusicians .................................................................. 38 2.5.2 Playing an Instrument and PLS Development ......................................... 38 2.6 Factors Influencing the Rate and Quality of PLS Development....................... 40 2.6.1 Creating an Attitude of Addiction as a Means to PLS Development ...... 40 2.6.2 Creating an Environment Conducive to Music Learning and PLS Development ............................................................................................ 40 2.6.3 The Critical Period, Brain Plasticity and PLS Development ................... 42 2.6.4 Individuated Instruction, The Zone of Proximal Development, and Targeted Remediation .............................................................................. 43 2.6.5 Explicit Learning Enhances Implicit Learning ........................................ 45 2.6.5.1 The Role of Mental Representations in Pitch Processing ............... 46 2.6.5.2 Gestalt Recognition ......................................................................... 47 2.6.5.3
Recommended publications
  • Harmony Crib Sheets

    Harmony Crib Sheets

    Jazz Harmony Primer General stuff There are two main types of harmony found in modern Western music: 1) Modal 2) Functional Modal harmony generally involves a static drone, riff or chord over which you have melodies with notes chosen from various scales. It’s common in rock, modern jazz and electronic dance music. It predates functional harmony, too. In some types of modal music – for example in jazz - you get different modes/chord scale sounds over the course of a piece. Chords and melodies can be drawn from these scales. This kind of harmony is suited to the guitar due to its open strings and retuning possibilities. We see the guitar take over as a songwriting instrument at about the same time as the modes become popular in pop music. Loop based music also encourages this kind of harmony. It has become very common in all areas of music since the late 20th century under the influence of rock and folk music, composers like Steve Reich, modal jazz pioneered by Miles Davis and John Coltrane, and influences from India, the Middle East and pre-classical Western music. Functional harmony is a development of the kind of harmony used by Bach and Mozart. Jazz up to around 1960 was primarily based on this kind of harmony, and jazz improvisation was concerned with the improvising over songs written by the classically trained songwriters and film composers of the era. These composers all played the piano, so in a sense functional harmony is piano harmony. It’s not really guitar shaped. When I talk about functional harmony I’ll mostly be talking about ways we can improvise and compose on pre-existing jazz standards rather than making up new progressions.
  • Riemann's Functional Framework for Extended Jazz Harmony James

    Riemann's Functional Framework for Extended Jazz Harmony James

    Riemann’s Functional Framework for Extended Jazz Harmony James McGowan The I or tonic chord is the only chord which gives the feeling of complete rest or relaxation. Since the I chord acts as the point of rest there is generated in the other chords a feeling of tension or restlessness. The other chords therefore must 1 eventually return to the tonic chord if a feeling of relaxation is desired. Invoking several musical metaphors, Ricigliano’s comment could apply equally well to the tension and release of any tonal music, not only jazz. Indeed, such metaphors serve as essential points of departure for some extended treatises in music theory.2 Andrew Jaffe further associates “tonic,” “stability,” and “consonance,” when he states: “Two terms used to refer to the extremes of harmonic stability and instability within an individual chord or a chord progression are dissonance and consonance.”3 One should acknowledge, however, that to the non-jazz reader, reference to “tonic chord” implicitly means triad. This is not the case for Ricigliano, Jaffe, or numerous other writers of pedagogical jazz theory.4 Rather, in complete indifference to, ignorance of, or reaction against the common-practice principle that only triads or 1 Ricigliano 1967, 21. 2 A prime example, Berry applies the metaphor of “motion” to explore “Formal processes and element-actions of growth and decline” within different musical domains, in diverse stylistic contexts. Berry 1976, 6 (also see 111–2). An important precedent for Berry’s work in the metaphoric dynamism of harmony and other parameters is found in the writings of Kurth – particularly in his conceptions of “sensuous” and “energetic” harmony.
  • On Modulation —

    On Modulation —

    — On Modulation — Dean W. Billmeyer University of Minnesota American Guild of Organists National Convention June 25, 2008 Some Definitions • “…modulating [is] going smoothly from one key to another….”1 • “Modulation is the process by which a change of tonality is made in a smooth and convincing way.”2 • “In tonal music, a firmly established change of key, as opposed to a passing reference to another key, known as a ‘tonicization’. The scale or pitch collection and characteristic harmonic progressions of the new key must be present, and there will usually be at least one cadence to the new tonic.”3 Some Considerations • “Smoothness” is not necessarily a requirement for a successful modulation, as much tonal literature will illustrate. A “convincing way” is a better criterion to consider. • A clear establishment of the new key is important, and usually a duration to the modulation of some length is required for this. • Understanding a modulation depends on the aural perception of the listener; hence, some ambiguity is inherent in distinguishing among a mere tonicization, a “false” modulation, and a modulation. • A modulation to a “foreign” key may be easier to accomplish than one to a diatonically related key: the ear is forced to interpret a new key quickly when there is a large change in the number of accidentals (i.e., the set of pitch classes) in the keys used. 1 Max Miller, “A First Step in Keyboard Modulation”, The American Organist, October 1982. 2 Charles S. Brown, CAGO Study Guide, 1981. 3 Janna Saslaw: “Modulation”, Grove Music Online ed. L. Macy (Accessed 5 May 2008), http://www.grovemusic.com.
  • Day 17 AP Music Handout, Scale Degress.Mus

    Day 17 AP Music Handout, Scale Degress.Mus

    Scale Degrees, Chord Quality, & Roman Numeral Analysis There are a total of seven scale degrees in both major and minor scales. Each of these degrees has a name which you are required to memorize tonight. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 & w w w w w 1. tonicw 2.w supertonic 3.w mediant 4. subdominant 5. dominant 6. submediant 7. leading tone 1. tonic A triad can be built upon each scale degree. w w w w & w w w w w w w w 1. tonicw 2.w supertonic 3.w mediant 4. subdominant 5. dominant 6. submediant 7. leading tone 1. tonic The quality and scale degree of the triads is shown by Roman numerals. Captial numerals are used to indicate major triads with lowercase numerals used to show minor triads. Diminished triads are lowercase with a "degree" ( °) symbol following and augmented triads are capital followed by a "plus" ( +) symbol. Roman numerals written for a major key look as follows: w w w w & w w w w w w w w CM: wI (M) iiw (m) wiii (m) IV (M) V (M) vi (m) vii° (dim) I (M) EVERY MAJOR KEY FOLLOWS THE PATTERN ABOVE FOR ITS ROMAN NUMERALS! Because the seventh scale degree in a natural minor scale is a whole step below tonic instead of a half step, the name is changed to subtonic, rather than leading tone. Leading tone ALWAYS indicates a half step below tonic. Notice the change in the qualities and therefore Roman numerals when in the natural minor scale.
  • Robert GADEN: Slim GAILLARD

    Robert GADEN: Slim GAILLARD

    This discography is automatically generated by The JazzOmat Database System written by Thomas Wagner For private use only! ------------------------------------------ Robert GADEN: Robert Gaden -v,ldr; H.O. McFarlane, Karl Emmerling, Karl Nierenz -tp; Eduard Krause, Paul Hartmann -tb; Kurt Arlt, Joe Alex, Wolf Gradies -ts,as,bs; Hans Becker, Alex Beregowsky, Adalbert Luczkowski -v; Horst Kudritzki -p; Harold M. Kirchstein -g; Karl Grassnick -tu,b; Waldi Luczkowski - d; recorded September 1933 in Berlin 65485 ORIENT EXPRESS 2.47 EOD1717-2 Elec EG2859 Robert Gaden und sein Orchester; recorded September 16, 1933 in Berlin 108044 ORIENTEXPRESS 2.45 OD1717-2 --- Robert Gaden mit seinem Orchester; recorded December 1936 in Berlin 105298 MEIN ENTZÜCKENDES FRÄULEIN 2.21 ORA 1653-1 HMV EG3821 Robert Gaden mit seinem Orchester; recorded October 1938 in Berlin 106900 ICH HAB DAS GLÜCK GESEHEN 2.12 ORA3296-2 Elec EG6519 Robert Gaden mit seinem Orchester; recorded November 1938 in Berlin 106902 SIGNORINA 2.40 ORA3571-2 Elec EG6567 106962 SPANISCHER ZIGEUNERTANZ 2.45 ORA 3370-1 --- Robert Gaden mit seinem Orchester; Refraingesang: Rudi Schuricke; recorded September 1939 in Berlin 106907 TAUSEND SCHÖNE MÄRCHEN 2.56 ORA4169-1 Elec EG7098 ------------------------------------------ Slim GAILLARD: "Swing Street" Slim Gaillard -g,vib,vo; Slam Stewart -b; Sam Allen -p; Pompey 'Guts' Dobson -d; recorded February 17, 1938 in New York 9079 FLAT FOOT FLOOGIE 2.51 22318-4 Voc 4021 Some sources say that Lionel Hampton plays vibraphone. 98874 CHINATOWN MY CHINATOWN
  • Chapter 21 on Modal Mixture

    Chapter 21 on Modal Mixture

    CHAPTER21 Modal Mixture Much music of the late eighteenth and early nineteenth centuries commonly employs a type of chromaticism that cannot be said to derive from applied functions and tonicization. In fact, the chromaticism often appears to be nonfunctional-a mere coloring of the melodic and harmonic surface of the music. Listen to the two phrases of Example 21.1. The first phrase (mm. 1-8) contains exclusively diatonic harmonies, but the second phrase (mm. 11-21) is full of chromaticism. This creates sonorities, such as the A~-major chord in m. 15, which appear to be distant from the underlying F-major tonic. The chromatic harmonies in mm. 11-20 have been labeled with letter names that indicate the root and quality of the chord. EXAMPLE 21.1 Mascagni, "A casa amici" ("Homeward, friends"), Cavalleria Rusticana, scene 9 418 CHAPTER 21 MODAL MIXTURE 419 These sonorities share an important feature: All but the cadential V-I are members of the parallel mode, F minor. Viewing this progression through the lens of F minor reveals a simple bass arpeggiation that briefly tonicizes 420 THE COMPLETE MUSICIAN III (A~), then moves to PD and D, followed by a final resolution to major tonic in the major mode. The Mascagni excerpt is quite remarkable in its tonal plan. Although it begins and ends in F major, a significant portion of the music behaves as though written in F minor. This technique of borrowing harmonies from the parallel mode is called modal mixture (sometimes known simply as mixture). We have already encountered two instances of modal mixture.
  • Music in Theory and Practice

    Music in Theory and Practice

    CHAPTER 4 Chords Harmony Primary Triads Roman Numerals TOPICS Chord Triad Position Simple Position Triad Root Position Third Inversion Tertian First Inversion Realization Root Second Inversion Macro Analysis Major Triad Seventh Chords Circle Progression Minor Triad Organum Leading-Tone Progression Diminished Triad Figured Bass Lead Sheet or Fake Sheet Augmented Triad IMPORTANT In the previous chapter, pairs of pitches were assigned specifi c names for identifi cation CONCEPTS purposes. The phenomenon of tones sounding simultaneously frequently includes group- ings of three, four, or more pitches. As with intervals, identifi cation names are assigned to larger tone groupings with specifi c symbols. Harmony is the musical result of tones sounding together. Whereas melody implies the Harmony linear or horizontal aspect of music, harmony refers to the vertical dimension of music. A chord is a harmonic unit with at least three different tones sounding simultaneously. Chord The term includes all possible such sonorities. Figure 4.1 #w w w w w bw & w w w bww w ww w w w w w w w‹ Strictly speaking, a triad is any three-tone chord. However, since western European music Triad of the seventeenth through the nineteenth centuries is tertian (chords containing a super- position of harmonic thirds), the term has come to be limited to a three-note chord built in superposed thirds. The term root refers to the note on which a triad is built. “C major triad” refers to a major Triad Root triad whose root is C. The root is the pitch from which a triad is generated. 73 3711_ben01877_Ch04pp73-94.indd 73 4/10/08 3:58:19 PM Four types of triads are in common use.
  • Music 160A Class Notes Dr. Rothfarb October 8, 2007 Key

    Music 160A Class Notes Dr. Rothfarb October 8, 2007 Key

    Music 160A class notes Dr. Rothfarb October 8, 2007 Key determination: • Large-scale form in tonal music goes hand in hand with key organization. Your ability to recognize and describe formal structure will be largely dependent on your ability to recognize key areas and your knowledge of what to expect. Knowing what to expect: • Knowing what keys to expect in certain situations will be an invaluable tool in formal analysis. Certain keys are common and should be expected as typical destinations for modulation (we will use Roman numerals to indicate the relation to the home key). Here are the common structural key areas: o V o vi • Other keys are uncommon for modulation and are not structural: o ii o iii o IV Tonicization or modulation?: • The phenomena of tonicization and modulation provide a challenge to the music analyst. In both cases, one encounters pitches and harmonies that are foreign to the home key. How then, does one determine whether a particular passage is a tonicization or a modulation? • To begin with, this is a question of scale. Tonicizations are much shorter than modulations. A tonicization may last only one or two beats, while a modulation will last much longer—sometimes for entire sections. • A modulation will have cadences. These cadences confirm for the listener that the music has moved to a new key area. A tonicization—though it might have more than two chords—will not have cadences. • A modulation will also typically contain a new theme. Tonicizations, on the other hand, are finished too quickly to be given any significant thematic material.
  • Max Neuhaus, R. Murray Schafer, and the Challenges of Noise

    Max Neuhaus, R. Murray Schafer, and the Challenges of Noise

    University of Kentucky UKnowledge Theses and Dissertations--Music Music 2018 MAX NEUHAUS, R. MURRAY SCHAFER, AND THE CHALLENGES OF NOISE Megan Elizabeth Murph University of Kentucky, [email protected] Digital Object Identifier: https://doi.org/10.13023/etd.2018.233 Right click to open a feedback form in a new tab to let us know how this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Murph, Megan Elizabeth, "MAX NEUHAUS, R. MURRAY SCHAFER, AND THE CHALLENGES OF NOISE" (2018). Theses and Dissertations--Music. 118. https://uknowledge.uky.edu/music_etds/118 This Doctoral Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Music at UKnowledge. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations--Music by an authorized administrator of UKnowledge. For more information, please contact [email protected]. STUDENT AGREEMENT: I represent that my thesis or dissertation and abstract are my original work. Proper attribution has been given to all outside sources. I understand that I am solely responsible for obtaining any needed copyright permissions. I have obtained needed written permission statement(s) from the owner(s) of each third-party copyrighted matter to be included in my work, allowing electronic distribution (if such use is not permitted by the fair use doctrine) which will be submitted to UKnowledge as Additional File. I hereby grant to The University of Kentucky and its agents the irrevocable, non-exclusive, and royalty-free license to archive and make accessible my work in whole or in part in all forms of media, now or hereafter known. I agree that the document mentioned above may be made available immediately for worldwide access unless an embargo applies.
  • Bebopnet: Deep Neural Models for Personalized Jazz Improvisations - Supplementary Material

    Bebopnet: Deep Neural Models for Personalized Jazz Improvisations - Supplementary Material

    BEBOPNET: DEEP NEURAL MODELS FOR PERSONALIZED JAZZ IMPROVISATIONS - SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL 1. SUPPLEMENTARY MUSIC SAMPLES We provide a variety of MP3 files of generated solos in: https://shunithaviv.github.io/bebopnet Each sample starts with the melody of the jazz stan- dard, followed by an improvisation whose duration is one chorus. Sections BebopNet in sample and BebopNet out of sample contain solos of Bebop- Net without beam search over chord progressions in and out of the imitation training set, respectively. Section Diversity contains multiple solos over the same stan- Figure 1. Digital CRDI controlled by a user to provide dard to demonstrate the diversity of the model for user- continuous preference feedback. 4. Section Personalized Improvisations con- tain solos following the entire personalization pipeline for the four different users. Section Harmony Guided Improvisations contain solos generated with a har- monic coherence score instead of the user preference score, as described in Section 3.2. Section Pop songs contains solos over the popular non-jazz song. Some of our favorite improvisations by BebopNet are presented in the first sec- Algorithm 1: Score-based beam search tion, Our Favorite Jazz Improvisations. Input: jazz model fθ; score model gφ; batch size b; beam size k; update interval δ; input in τ sequence Xτ = x1··· xτ 2 X 2. METHODS τ+T Output: sequence Xτ+T = x1··· xτ+T 2 X in in in τ×b 2.1 Dataset Details Vb = [Xτ ;Xτ ; :::; Xτ ] 2 X ; | {z } A list of the solos included in our dataset is included in b times scores = [−1; −1; :::; −1] 2 Rb section 4.
  • Abstracts Euromac2014 Eighth European Music Analysis Conference Leuven, 17-20 September 2014

    Abstracts Euromac2014 Eighth European Music Analysis Conference Leuven, 17-20 September 2014

    Edited by Pieter Bergé, Klaas Coulembier Kristof Boucquet, Jan Christiaens 2014 Euro Leuven MAC Eighth European Music Analysis Conference 17-20 September 2014 Abstracts EuroMAC2014 Eighth European Music Analysis Conference Leuven, 17-20 September 2014 www.euromac2014.eu EuroMAC2014 Abstracts Edited by Pieter Bergé, Klaas Coulembier, Kristof Boucquet, Jan Christiaens Graphic Design & Layout: Klaas Coulembier Photo front cover: © KU Leuven - Rob Stevens ISBN 978-90-822-61501-6 A Stefanie Acevedo Session 2A A Yale University [email protected] Stefanie Acevedo is PhD student in music theory at Yale University. She received a BM in music composition from the University of Florida, an MM in music theory from Bowling Green State University, and an MA in psychology from the University at Buffalo. Her music theory thesis focused on atonal segmentation. At Buffalo, she worked in the Auditory Perception and Action Lab under Peter Pfordresher, and completed a thesis investigating metrical and motivic interaction in the perception of tonal patterns. Her research interests include musical segmentation and categorization, form, schema theory, and pedagogical applications of cognitive models. A Romantic Turn of Phrase: Listening Beyond 18th-Century Schemata (with Andrew Aziz) The analytical application of schemata to 18th-century music has been widely codified (Meyer, Gjerdingen, Byros), and it has recently been argued by Byros (2009) that a schema-based listening approach is actually a top-down one, as the listener is armed with a script-based toolbox of listening strategies prior to experiencing a composition (gained through previous style exposure). This is in contrast to a plan-based strategy, a bottom-up approach which assumes no a priori schemata toolbox.
  • CMS-Southern Chapter/SCSMT Joint Conference

    CMS-Southern Chapter/SCSMT Joint Conference

    CMS-Southern Chapter/SCSMT Joint Conference February 28 – March 1, 2020 Blair School of Music Vanderbilt University Nashville, Tennessee A Message from the CMS Southern Chapter President Welcome to the 41st Annual Conference of the CMS Southern Chapter! We are back together after a very successful 40th anniversary meeting last year at the University of Central Florida in Orlando. It was a fantastic event and moment of reflection for many who have been involved in the Southern Chapter over the years. This year we are excited to be in Nashville, Tennessee. All conference activities will take place at the Blair School of Music, Vanderbilt University. We extend our gratitude to the Blair School of Music for hosting our many performances, demonstrations, papers, posters, and concerts. We are also incredibly thankful to our hosts from Belmont University, Kristian Klefstad and Mark Volker. We appreciate your dedication to the chapter and work to organize a conference with so many moving parts and scheduling challenges. Thank you! This year’s conference is held jointly with the annual meeting of the South Central Society of Music Theory. We hope that attendees from CMS and SCSMT learn from each other and connect to form new friendships, opportunities, and dialogues. We welcome our friends from SCSMT! This year’s conference theme is “Improvisation: Science, Practice, and Pedagogy.” Our program features many excellent sessions and performances that center on this topic. We are excited to welcome four guest speakers to our conference: Martin Norgaard, Dennis Thurmond, Dariusz Terefenko, and Andrew Goldman. We are certain you will enjoy all of these sessions and workshops with our esteemed guests.