<<

The Role of Imagery in Information Processing: Review and Extensions

DEBORAH J. MACINNIS LINDA L. PRICE*

Mental imagery is receiving increased attention in consumer behavior theory and research. This article describes imagery, characterizing it as a processing in which multisensory information is represented in a gestalt form in working and discusses research on the unique effects of imagery at low levels of cognitive elaboration. It specifies researchable propositions for the relationship between high elaboration imagery processing and consumer choice and consumption behaviors Finally, it reviews specific methods for studying imagery.

nformation processing research has traditionally fo- 1978; Cohen 1982; Greenwald and Leavitt 1984; Ros- I cused on discursive or descriptive information pro- siter and Percy 1978, 1983; Smith et al. 1984). cessing (Bettman 1979; Greenwald 1968; Olson, Toy, This article addresses the meaning of imagery pro- and Dover 1982; Swasy and Munch 1985; Wright 1974,' cessing, how it differs from discursive processing—par- 1980).' As such, researchers have examined how sym- ticularly under varying degrees of cognitive elaboration, bols (most commonly words and numbers) are com- and what unique effects it has on processing outcomes! bined in to represent and solve prob- It also reviews research on the unique effects of imagery lems. Discursive (symbolic, language-like) information under conditions of low elaboration and explores the processing encompasses a broad range of strategies. potentially unique effects of imagery at higher levels of Compositional choice strategies, counterarguments, elaboration. The article gives specific attention to the attributions, and formulations of choice rules are illus- role of imagery in problem framing, assessing proba- trations of discursive information processing. Increas- bilities, forming intentions, and generating affect. In ingly, however, attention has been given to the role of addition, it considers the use of imagery in consumer mental imagery in information processing (Childers and settings throughout the phases of consumption and de- Houston 1982, 1984; Childers, Houston, and Heckler velops researchable propositions to guide future re- 1985; Lutz and Lutz 1977, 1978; Rethans and Hastak search on elaborated imagery. Finally, it explores issues 1981; Rossiter 1982; Rossiter and Percy 1983; Smith, related to the conduct of research on imagery. Houston, and Childers 1984). Imagery is a conceptually distinct way of representing information, a way that is "very like picturing and very unlike describing" (Fodor THE MEANING OF IMAGERY 1981, p. 76).^ To date, this emergent research stream Imagery is defined here as (1) a process (not a struc- has focused predominantly on imagery processing at ture) by which (2) sensory information is represented low levels of cognitive elaboration—for example, men- in working memory. Imagery processing, and infor- tally picturing a object. More recently, there mation processing in general, fall on an elaboration have been discussions that refer to imagery processing continuum that ranges from processes limited to the at higher levels of elaboration. These discussions suggest simple retrieval or evocation of a cognitive concept to that elaborated imagery plays a role in influencing (1) affective responses to stimuli, and (2) behavior (Calder As used here, discursive means passing from premises to conclu- sions; proceeding by reasoning or argument (Oxford dictionary). De- scriptive processing is a somewhat broader term suggesting any •Deborah J. Maclnnis is Assistant Professor, Department of Mar- meaningful combination of verbal or other symbolic units. Although keting, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721. Linda L. Price is the two terms have a slightly different connotation, they will be used Assistant Professor, Department of Marketing. University of Pitts- interchangeably here for ease of presentation. burgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260. Authors are listed in alphabetical order •This is not to say that the debate of what imagery is and what it to reflect equal contributions. The authors would like to acknowledge is not has been resolved (cf Block 1981). Processing probably often the many helpful comments of C. Whan Park and three anonymous occurs on a continuum between "pictures" and "symbols." For ex- reviewers on earlier versions of this article. ample, maps and stick figures (as mental images) would have features of both (Fodor 1981). 473

© JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH • Vol. 13 • March 1987 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH 474 processes involving multiple concepts and constructions an individual may draw upon a script of a "romantic (e;g., problem solving, creative thinking, and day- evening" in imagining such an evening. Information dreaming). The following section will describe each of from schemas or scripts can also be processed in a dis- these issues in greater detail, and will serve to (1) dis- cursive mode. Hence, the information structure is viewed as distinct from processing mode. tinguish imagery processing from schemas and scripts, Since imagery processing relies on stored knowledge, (2) distinguish imagery processing from discursive pro- the evocation and vividness of the image is likely to cessing, and (3) describe the elaboration continuum for depend on the level of knowledge development. Con- imagery processing. sistent with this notion. Smith et al. (1984) found that individuals who possessed a script for a given event re- Imagery as a Process, Not a Structure ported evoking imagery experiences when they instan- While the idea of imagery processing is widely ac- tiated that script. Moreover, inclividuals with well-de- cepted, the assumption that knowledge is stored or rep- veloped scripts reported that their imagery experiences resented as images has been hotly debated (Pinker and were significantly more vivid than did individuals with- Kosslyn 1983). Propositional theorists (e.g., Anderson out well-developed scripts. :1978; Anderson and Bower 1973; Lang 1979; Pylyshyn 1973, 1981) argue that knowledge is represented only Imagery as a Sensory Process as a set of verbal propositional networks composed of nodes (representing concepts) and connected by links Imagery processing has several qualities that distin- (representing relationships between concepts; see also guish it from discursive processing. Most fundamen- Kieras 1978). Others have argued that knowledge can tally, imagery processes are evoked as sensory experi- be stored as images. In support of their argument they ences in working memory. Imagery processing includes note that studies on mental rotation of objects, image perceptual or sensory representations in working mem- size, and parallel processing are more consistent with ory that are used in much the same way as an imagery' perspective than a propositional one (Bug- of external stimuli. Thus, imagery involves concrete elski 1983'; Kieras 1978; Kolers 1983; Kosslyn 1975; sensory representations of ideas, , and memo- Kosslyn and Pomerantz 1977; Kosslyn et al. 1981). ries, and it permits a direct recovery of past experiences These researchers contend that images have emergent (Yuille and Catchpole 1977). The evocation of iniagery properties that cannot be explained by propositjonal may be multi-sensory—involving images that incor- network theories. porate, for example, smell, , sight, and tactile sen- sations—or may involve a single sensory dimension, While the debate between propositional theorists and imagery theorists is far from resolved, Yuille and such as sight. In contrast, discursive processing (such Catchpole (1977) have developed a compromise posi- as verbal retrieval, cognitive responding, and verbal en- tion based on the work of Piaget and Inhelder (1973). coding) is more detached from internal sensor>- expe- They propose that the ability to generate images does riences. The absence of sensory dimensions of infor- not necessarily imply that knowledge is stored as images. mation in working memory- makes these discursive They argue that knowledge is stored in an abstract (but processes less concrete (more abstract) than imagery not necessarily verbal) operational code. The well-sub- processes. Moreover, unlike discursive processing, im- stantiated ability to move from words to pictures and agery can be described along several unique sensory- pictures to words suggests that there is a representation related dimensions. For example, vividness refers to the in memory that encompasses both. Once a knowledge clarity of images. And controllability reflects the extent structure has been activated, imagery can be generated to which images can be held in and/or altered in from information contained in that structure. For ex- specific ways at will (Marks 1972). ample, after activating the knowledge structure for That imagery involves internal sensory experiences birds, an individual may imagine a prototypical bird has both intuitive and empirical support. Early research such as a sparrow (see Mervis and Rosch 1981 for a suggested that people can confuse imaging with per- review). ceiving (Perky 1910). Even before research on imagery Consistent with the views of Yuille and Catchpole existed, people talked naturally about the "pictures" in (1977), imagery is conceptualized here as a mode of their heads. Research has since indicated that imagery processing information. In other words, imagery pro- and perceiving (or sensing) share the same physiological cessing is viewed as distinct from information structure machinery, and that imagery produces physiological ef- (knowledge storage). This distinction serves to differ- fects that'mirror perceptual processes (Deckert 1964; entiate imagery from such knowledge structures as Finke 1980; Lang 1979; Perky 1910; Shaw 1940). Im- schemas and scripts. The latter constructs refer to agery content also influences a broad range of physio- knowledge structures regarding an object, person, role, logical responses (e.g., muscular reactions, heart rate, event, or (cf. Abelson 1976; Rosch 1978). It is eye movements, galvanic skin response; Lang 1979). the instantiation of a schema or script that generates Other research stresses the analogue relationship be- imagery, not the schema or script itself. For example. tween imagery and pictures (Kosslyn 1980; Kosslyn et IMAGERY PROCESSING 475

al. 1983; Paivio 1975; Shepard 1978; Shepard and et al. 1984; Yalch and Yalch 1984) can elicit elaborated Meltzer 1971). Specifically, imagery is presumed to have imagery processing, elaborated discursive processing, properties that preserve the spatial and size dimensions or both. in actual stimuli. Thus, just as an ant looks small com- It is important to remember that imagery and dis- pared to a butterfly, imagery would reproduce their rel- cursive processing are not mutually exclusive processes ative relationship. Compared with symbolic or lan- (Arieti 1976; Bugelski 1983; Holbrook and Moore guage-like processing, imagery processing bears a non- 1981 a). For example, an individual may evoke an image arbitrary correspondence to the thing being represented. and give it a verbal label or generate cognitive responses While it is recognized that imagery is a sensory and based on imagined (visualized) scenarios. When con- sometimes a multisensory process, the remainder of this sumers in "real" contexts engage in imagery, it is quite article focuses predominantly on visual imagery. That likely that discursive and imagery processing and the is, the examples and discussion tend to emphasize pic- contents of each become elaborately intertwined.' Based turing and visualizing rather than other sensory prop- on current research, it is difficult to disentangle imagery erties ofthe informatioti in working memory. and discursive processing and ensure that the unique effects of each processing mode are independent of con- The Elaboration Continuum Of Imagery tent effects. Nevertheless, it is important to understand the situations that influence the dominance of one pro- Several streams of research have proposed that in- cessing mode over the other. Understanding the unique formation can be processed at different levels of cog- effects of each processing mode on processing outcomes nitive elaboration (Cacioppo and Petty 1984; Chaiken is an equally important issue. Research that addresses 1980; Craik and Lockhart 1972; Craik and Tulving these issues is summarized in the Figure, which serves 1975; Greenwald and Leavitt 1984; Mitchell 1981; Petty as a framework for the remainder of this article. and Cacioppo 1983). Elaboration reflects the extent to Our attention will now turn to the unique effects of which information in working memor>' is integrated low elaboration imagery (vs. low elaboration symbolic/ with prior knowledge structures. Information processed language-like processing) on learning and memor>s the at a low level of elaboration may elicit only a recognition response. Information processed at higher levels of potential effects of elaborated imagery on information elaboration, however, establishes connections between processing and the phases of consumption, and finally, encoded information and prior knowledge, and thus issues involved in the conduct of research on imagery involves the activation and integration of data from (imagery-eliciting strategies and measures of imagery multiple knowledge structures. Thinking, problem processing). solving, cognitive responding, and daydreaming are all processes involving high degrees of cognitive elabora- REVIEW OF LOW-ELABORATION tion (Arieti 1976; Bugelski 1983; Greenwald and Leavitt IMAGERY PROCESSING 1984; Simon 1978). The bulk of research on imagery has examined im- There has been an implicit confound in some research agery experiences that fall at the low end ofthe elabo- between processing mode (imagery vs. discursive) and ration continuum. Two broad imagery research streams processing level (high vs. low elaboration; Craik and are: (1) studies ofthe effect of imagery as a mnemonic Lockhart 1972; Craik and Tulving 1975; Petty and Ca- device, and (2) studies of the effect of imagery in en- cioppo 1983). Specifically, low elaboration has been hancing incidental learning. Excellent reviews of these implicitly linked to imagery and high elaboration to studies exist elsewhere (cf. Ernest 1977; Lutz and Lutz discursive (symbolic, language-like) processing. How- 1978; Paivio 1971; Richardson 1983); hence, the review ever, both imagery- and discursive processes can be de- provided here is brief and selective. scribed on an elaboration continuum (see the Figure). At the low end ofthe elaboration continuum are simple responses such as the retrieval of a verbal label, and/or Imagery as a Mnemonic Tool an image of a perceptual object. At the high end ofthe General Findings. One research stream indicates elaboration continuum are discursive processes such as counterarguments, attributions, and compositional that, compared to non-imagerj' processing strategies choice strategies (Wright 1980), and imagery processes such as verbal rehearsal, imagery can substantially en- such as daydreams, , and visual problem solv- hance memory for pairs of stimuli (Bower 1970, 1972; ing (Hilgard 1981; Richardson 1983). The same factors Paivio 1969, 1971; Yates 1966). A factor explaining the that influence elaborated discursive processing also effect of imagery on paired associate learning is that stimulate elaborated imagery processing. For example, imagery provides a holistic construction that serves to involvement in an activity (Greenwald and Leavitt 1984; Klinger 1977; Mitchell 1981; Sarbin 1972) and 'One reviewer noted that a related but separate line of inquio' prior knowledge (Burnkrant and Sawyer 1983; Ca- would not seek to disentangle the experimentally divisible process cioppo and Petty 1984; Petty and Cacioppo 1983; Smith and content domains, but would instead focus on consumers' use of imagerj' in naturalistic settings. THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH 476

FIGURE ANTECEDENTS AND CONSEQUENCES OF IMAGERY PROCESSING

Consequences Antecedents -*• Processing

Low elaboration Effects on memory Pictures Childers and Houston 1984" Alesandrini and Sheikh 1983* Emest 1977* -Discursive Childers and Houston 1984" Imagery - Lute and Lute 1977" Lutz and Lute 1978* Lute and Lute 1978' Paivio 1971*" High elaboration Miller etal. 1960" Rossiter 1982' Paivio 1969" Shepard 1967" Paivio (1971)'" Paivio and Csapo 1973" Concrete words Paivio and Foth 1970" Alesandrini and Sheikh 1983' Moderators: Individu^ Differences in Yates 1966"' Cartwright 1980" tmagery Processing Cartwright et al. 1978" Effects on incidental learning Lutz and Lute 1978* Imagery vividness Bower 1972" Paivio etal. 1968" Marks 1972* Butter 1970" Paivio and Foth 1970" Sheehan 1972" Rossiter 1982' Marks 1973" Sheehan 1967" Sheehan and Neisser 1969" Instructions to imagine Singer and Antrobus 1972" Swann and Miller 1982" Carroll 1978" Controttabitity of imagery Effects on problem framing Rossiter 1982' Gordon 1949" Simon and Hayes 1976 Sherman et al. 1984" Holbrook and Moore 1981b Wright and Rip 1980" Richardson 1969" Effects on probability assessment Guided imagery tmagery content Carroll 1978" Wolltnan1981' Singer and Antrobus 1963" Singer and Antrobus 1972" Einhom and Hogarth 1984" Current concerns Kahneman and Tversky 1982"' Spatiat abitity Sherman etal. 1984" Klinger 1977*"' Singer 1978' Shepard and Melteer 1971" Effects on intentions and purchase timing Needs for enhanced or Processing styte Anderson 1983" reduced stimutation Childers et al. 1985" Bandura 1982" Cautela and McCuiiough 1978* Doob 1972' Holbrook et al. 1984" Kunzendorf 1982" Lang 1977" Singer 1978* Lang 1979* Tushup and Zuckerman Paivio 1971*"' McMahon 1973* 1977" Richardson 1977" Mischel 1974* Mischel and Moore 1973' Staats and Lohr 1979' Wolpe 1958" Imagery as a consumption experience Arieti 1976' Doob 1972* Hilgard 1978*" Hirschman and Holbrook 1982' Holbrook etal. 1984" Undauer 1972* Undauer 1983* Singer 1978*

• Review article. " Empirical article. ' Theoretical article.

unify object pairs in a meaningful association (Bower imagining a stimulus in a bizarre image. The unusual 1970, 1972). Thus, memory for a pair of words like image created by a bizarre association may create a more "tub" and "bug" can be etihanced by imagining a bug distinctive memory trace that enhances the likelihood taking a bath. These memory effects may be substantial that subjects will recall the appropriate target word if the unification ofthe object pairs results in a bizarre (Nelson, Reed, and McEnvoy 1977). Paivio and Csapo image. Miller, Galanter, and Pribram (1960) found that (1973) found that memory for objects increased as the subjects could remember as many as 500 responses by concreteness of the stimulus words increased. Thus, IMAGERY PROCESSING 477

memory was greatest for pictures, less for concrete Stract and concrete pairs. However, in the incidental words, and least for abstract words. In sum, imagery learning situation, subjects recalled significantly more has greater effects on paired associate learning when the concrete than abstract words. Sheehan (1972) proposes stimuli are concrete (particularly if they can be unified that imagery-eliciting stimuli in general (e.g., pictures, in an interactive image). concrete words, imagery instructions), and not merely Marketing Applications. In marketing, the effects of concrete words, enhance incidental learning. Consistent imagery on paired associate learning have been applied with Sheehan's hypothesis. Bower (1972) found that in several ways. Lutz and Lutz (1977) found that pic- imagery instructions facilitated incidental learning. Half tures (imagery-eliciting stimuli) in advertisements had of the subjects were told to memorize 20 noun pairs a positive effect on memory for product-relevant infor- and expect a recall test (intentional learning). The others mation when the brand (company) name and product were told to rate the vividness of mental images gen- attribute were unified in a picture (interactive image). erated by the word pairs. No mention of a recall task Memory was reduced when items in the pair did not was made to this group (incidental learning). The results interact. Childers and Houston (1984) found that re- indicated that imagery-orienting instructions at encod- dundancy between pictorial and visual information in ing significantly reduced the gap between intentional advertisements produced a superior effect on recall for and incidental learning. the advertisement, particularly when a sensory pro- cessing strategy, as opposed to a verbal (symbolic) pro- Marketing applications. The effects of low-elabo- cessing strategy, was used. The authors proposed that ration imagery on consumer learning and memory offer because of chunking at encoding, interactive images a rich domain for future research. For example, given create a stronger memory trace that facilitates retrieval. the often cited notion that much of consumer behavior is ofthe "low involvement" variety (e.g., Batra and Ray While much of the research on imagery effects upon 1983), studies ofthe role of imagery in enhancing in- intentional learning has focused on memory for noun cidental learning would be quite informative. Since both or item pairs, Wright and Rip (1980) examined the role imagery vividness and stimulus concreteness affect the of imagery in learning a problem-framing rule. They usefulness of imagery in incidental learning tasks hypothesized that imagery instructions might enhance (Swann and Miller 1982), marketers need to explore learning by making the attributes more concrete and the factors under their control that can influence im- salient, and hence, more memorable. However, the ef- agery vividness and concreteness and that thus affect fects of imagery on learning were modest at best. One consumers' abilities to remember product-related in- important factor influencing these results may be that formation. the subjects, as novices, had little schematic information about the attributes or the problem situation. For them, attribute-based information was abstract, not concrete. ELABORATED IMAGERY: REVIEW Thus, their ability to generate imagery may have been AND EXTENSIONS limited. In a consumer context, the role of imagery processing at high levels of the elaboration continuum remains Imagery Efiects on Incidental Learning illusory, although it is also important. Research in other fields indicates that elaborated imagery experiences General Findings. Several studies indicate that im- (such as daydreams, fantasies, and visual problem-solv- agery-eliciting stimuli (e.g., pictures, concrete words, ing) are ubiquitous across cultures (Doob 1972) and instructions to imagine) enhance incidental learning, age groups (Giambra 1977). Elaborated imagery pro- and thus reduce the gap between incidental and inten- cessing can help an individual anticipate future situa- tional learning-task conditions (Bower 1972; Butter tions (Kolers 1983; Singer 1974. 1978; Singer and An- 1970; Sheehan 1972; Sheehan and Neisser 1969). Stim- trobus 1972) and work out solutions to current prob- ulus concreteness is one factor that may help to reduce lems (Klinger 1977), and can influence affective this gap. Butter (1970) asked subjects to read pairs of experiences. Few consumer researchers have examined words that were either abstract or concrete. Subjects the types of consumption experiences that may evoke did not expect a recall test, making the task an incidental elaborated imagery processing or the types of con- learning situation. Subjects recalled a significantly sumption-related experiences that are processed as im- greater number of concrete than abstract word pairs. agery.'' Moreover, little is known about whether elab- Butter hypothesized that concrete words elicit imagery that enhances incidental learning. In several studies re- ported in Sheehan 1972, Sheehan manipulated both the concreteness of the word pairs (concrete vs. abstract) 'Exceptions are studies by Holbrook el al. (1984) and Rethans and Hastak (1981). Holbrook et al. found that individuals enjoyed games and the learning conditions of subjects (incidental vs. more when their preferred cognitive style (imagery vs. verbalization) intentional learning). In these studies, subjects in the matched the game format (visual vs. verbal). Rethans and Hastak intentional learning condition remembered both ab- found that a significant portion of consumers' about product hazards is evoked as imagery' experiences. THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH 478 orated imagery produces effects that differ from dis- or explicit summary of features or attributes using some combination rules, imagery is holistic. A particularly cursive processing. interesting study suggests the impact of mental imaging The following section reviews studies on elaborated on the presence of cue configurality (feature interac- imagery processing made elsewhere and develops prop- tions) in a product evaluation task (Holbrook and ositions about the potentially unique effects of elabo- Moore 1981b).' While the study focused predominantly rated imagery on selected consumer behaviors. In par- on processing differences for verbal versus pictorial ticular, the influence of elaborated imagery on problem presentations, the subject's claimed processing strategy framing, the assessment of probabilities, purchase in- (discursive vs. imagery) was found to exert a significant tentions, and affect are explored. Research relevant to moderating effect on the presence of cue configurality this discussion is listed in the Figure. for different input formats. As such, imagery processing does not readily lend itself to piecemeal comparisons Consumer Imagery and Problem Framing across brands. Hence, as a tool for framing problems, imagery is more appropriately characterized as a brand- Research in human problem solving indicates that based processing strategy. problem framing is an important part of subsequent As a within-brand problem-framing tool, imagery problem-solving activities. Both discursive and imagery becomes an effective strategy for making a single out- processes can be used to frame problems (Simon and come or scenario specific and concrete. For example, Hayes 1976). For example, an individual may visually the individual may first simply imagine (visualize) the imagine a product in use and use that evoked scenario product. Having made this construction, the image may as a basis for subsequent problem-solving activities (e.g., be elaborated as the individual develops a scenario of attribute evaluations, risk assessment). Alternatively, the product being used. Of course, alternative scenarios using discursive processing, an individual might ex- might be imagined to reflect multiple-usage situations. amine a series of brands and attributes in a matrix and There is obviously a tradeoff between evaluating a single invoke a choice heuristic (e.g., a lexicographic strategy) brand thoroughly and evaluating multiple brands using to make a product selection decision. Research has fewer criteria. Since imagery encourages a focus on a demonstrated that the way in which a problem is rep- single brand, resources available for processing infor- resented (visually or otherwise) can have a dramatic mation about other brands are reduced.' It is important impact on the amount of time it takes to solve a prob- to remember, however, that the use of imagery in choice lem. It also affects the solution strategies used (Simon contexts does not imply that the individual ignores in- and Hayes 1976). While research in the choice and de- formation about other brands. Specifically, imagery cision making literature has been extensive in marketing may be used as part of a phased decision-making strat- (e.g., Bettman and Park 1980; Bettman and Zins 1977; egy along with other choice rules (Cohen 1982; Park Grether and Wilde 1984; Lussier and Olshavsky 1984; and Mittal 1985). For example, the individual may use Park and Lessig 1981; Wright 1974), little is known discursive processing to reduce the number of alter- about whether imagery is used in these decision making natives and then use imagery to evaluate the few that activities and whether imagery influences decision remain. making strategies and outcomes. The Effect of Imagery Processing on Brand Evalua- Decision Complexity and Imagery Processing. The tion Strategies. Most research has assumed that con- extent to which consumers use imagery as an alternative sumers either combine attributes to make an overall to complex information-integration rules is not known. brand evaluation (choice by processing brands) or The issue is an important one, however, because it has compare all brands on one or more attributes, such as implications for consumers' abilities to deal with a large price or quality (denoted as choice by processing attri- array of data for a single product. Using a linear conti- butes; Bettman 1979). Discursive processing obviously pensatory choice model, there is a positive relationship allows for both types of brand-evaluation strategies. between the number of attributes or brands considered Research has demonstrated that the structure of the and the complexity of the problem: the greater the task, information presentation format, and the prior number of attributes or brands, the more information experience and learning goals ofthe consumer ali affect that must be evaluated, and hence, the more complex the use of brand- and attribute-evaluation strategies the decision. This guideline has been implicit in re- (Bettman and Kakkar 1977; Biehal and Chakravarti searchers' understanding of information load. While 1982; Brucks 1985; Johnson and Russo 1980). The effect of imagery processing on brand evaluation strategies has not been explored. However, Park and 'The presence of feature interactions is consistent with holistic and Mittal (1985) indicate that elaborated imagery pro- integrative processing. 'This effect may be heightened if visualizing the brand has arousal cessing is likely to differ in important ways from elab- value (i.e., it's exciting and fun to imagine use of the brand). The orated discursive processing, in particular, while dis- arousal effects of imagery are discussed in subsequent sections of this cursive processing can be characterized as an implicit article. IMAGERY PROCESSING 479 consumers in general seem to be able to handle a few mation for the brand and then determining value. The more attributes than alternatives before information following propositions summarize this discussion ofthe overload sets in, both attributes and alternatives affect influence of elaborated imagery on problem framing*: decision complexity (Malhotra 1982). Using imagery, it is much easier to consider a larger PI: When imagery processing is used, consumers set of attributes than to evaluate a larger set of brands.' will be more likely to rely on within-brand While considering a larger set of brands requires the processing strategies as opposed to across- construction of additional images, a larger set of attrib- brand strategies. ute information may be helpful in "filling out" or en- hancing an image. In fact, for imagery processing, there P2: Consumers will evaluate fewer brands when may be an inverse relationship between the number of imagery processing rather than discursive attributes and the complexity of the decision. When processing is used, controlling for processing attributes are not given, the image is likely to be vague, . making the evocation ofthe image and the evaluation P3: There will be an inverse relationship between ofthe product more difficult. In contrast, greater num- the number of attributes given and perceived bers of attributes add clarity and precision to the image, decision complexity when imagery process- which could facilitate the evaluation ofthe product. Of ing is used. In contrast, there will be a positive course, as noted in previous sections, this would depend relationship between the number of attri- on the concreteness and hence imagability ofthe attri- butes given and perceived decision complex- butes. Comparable to the novelist's detailed description ity when a discursive processing strategy is of characters, detailed information on the features of a used. product may make visualizing the product and its uses much easier. P4: When imagerj' processing is used, consumers will infer missing attributes based on existing Missing Information and Imagery Processing. Fi- information about the product rather than nally, since imager>' processing encourages within-brand on estimation ofthe mean value ofthe miss- product-evaluation strategies, it has some important ing attribute from information on other implications for how consumers deal with missing in- brands. formation in product selection decisions. Recent re- search suggests that consumers use information about Imagery Effects on Probability Assessment comparable brands to fill in missing information on a and Consumer Satisfaction partially described alternative (Huber and McCann In framing consumption problems and product al- 1982; Meyer 1981, 1982). For example, consumers us- ternatives, consumers are likely to elaborate on the ing an across-brand strategy (or processing by attributes) consequences of product use (Ajzen and Fishbein 1980: may assign a discounted mean value to the missing at- Wright 1980). In using imagery to elaborate on product tribute (Huber and McCann 1982). Other research sug- or purchase outcomes, systematic effects in probability gests that consumers use information about other at- assessment and behavioral intentions may arise. These tributes ofthe partially described alternative to fill in effects are explored in the sections that follow. the missing values (within-brand strategy: Ford and Smith 1985). Both types of inferences have been dem- Conjunctive Probability Assessment. Recent re- onstrated, but the conditions that lead to across-brand search suggests that individuals overestimate conjunc- versus within-brand inferences have not been deter- tive probabilities (i.e., the likelihood that two events mined. Understanding the type of processing used could will occur together: Fischhoff 1985: Tversky and improve predictions of the likelihood of differing in- Kahneman 1983: Wallsten and Budescu 1983). While ferential strategies. While consumers using discursive there are many possible sources of biases in predictive processing might infer price by substituting a discounted judgments of this sort, one ofthe most important ap- average price ofthe product across brands, consumers pears to be elaborated imagery. Recent research by using imagery would be more likely to infer price by Kahneman and Tversky (1982) indicates that when in- first representing the product based on available infor- dividuals imagine a future scenario they attach proba- bilities to the scenario as a whole, not to the individual sequences of events that make up the scenario. As 'This would not necessarily generalize lo sequential tasks (i.e.. where Kahneman and Tversky note, "the cumulative proba- additional attributes are provided at a second exposure) since the bility of at least one fatal failure in the sequence of nature ofthe information stored in memory for imaginal versus dis- cursive processing may vary (see Fiske 1982). Little is known about the factors that affect information use in memory-based choice, but 'It is recognized that discursive processing refers to a broad set of there is some evidence that consumers use prior overall evaluations processing strategies, including cognitive responses and cognitive al- and that the extent of brand-attribute feature processing in general gebra. For ease of presentation, these (and subsequent) propositions is reduced (Biehal and Chakravarti 1986; Lingle and Ostrom 1979; are simply formulated around the general term, without specifying Loken and Hoverstad 1985). the specific form of discursive processing. THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH 480

images could be overwhelmingly high, even though the with nebulous, difficult-to-imagine conditions. When probability of each individual cause of failure is negli- these two groups were asked to imagine themselves gible" (1982, pp. 207-208). Relatedly, Einhorn and contracting the , only those given the easy-to- Hogarth (1984) found that individuals tend to assign a imagine symptoms increased their estimated likelihood high probability to conjunctive events that they can of getting it. (Those given the difficult-to-imagine readily imagine and pay little attention to the number symptoms actually reduced their perceived chance of of contingent outcomes between the two events. More- contracting the disease.) These results suggest that im- over, the authors suggest that imagery may be one of agery can upwardly bias perceived likelihoods, partic- the most frequently used methods for estimating con- ularly when subjects are provided with concrete infor- junctive probabilities. Thus, when asked about the mation that enhances the vividness of images. probability of event 1 and event 2 co-occuring, respon- While several studies suggest that the presence (vs. dents may decide about the plausibility ofthe scenario absence) of imagery can influence likelihood assess- by imagining (visualizing) a scenario in which the events ments, the results do not clearly establish whether elab- co-exist. The research discussed earlier on the relation- orated discursive processing (such as the construction ship between the presence of feature interactions and of explanations or reasons) and elaborated imagery have processing strategy (discursive vs. imagery) also provides equivalent or differing effects on perceived likelihoods. indirect support for the importance of processing strat- For example, Carroll's 1978 study suggests only that egy in conjunctive probability assessment (Holbrook the effects of imagery are not enhanced by the addition and Moore 1981b). of verbal arguments. Moreover, the extent to which the effect is the consequence ofthe nature of imagery pro- Likelihood Assessment. Because of its impact on cessing itself or the indirect effect of imagery on content conjunctive probability assessment, imagery is likely to is also unclear. Certainly, providing a construction of increase the perceived likelihood that the visualized explanations or reasons for a particular outcome differs scenario will unfold as imagined. That is, probabilities from visualizing the outcome—not only in processing of individual events that make up the scenario are dis- style, but also in content. counted in judging the likelihood ofthe imagined sce- There are several reasons to suspect that elaborated nario. However, imagery also affects perceived likeli- imagery may affect perceived likelihoods more than hoods more directly. Some research suggests that the would elaborated discursive processing. For example, very of visualizing an event can make the event imagery is likely to be more concrete and bear a closer seem more likely. Carroll (1978) conducted experiments resemblance to perceptual phenomena (perhaps in- in which individuals were asked to imagine a specific creasing the salience of imagined outcomes over verbal outcome for a future event (e.g., the outcome of a pres- arguments). Nevertheless, it is clear that both types of idential election or a football game). The remaining processing can affect likelihood estimation, and there subjects were given no imagery instructions. For sub- is, at present, no unambiguous test of which type of jects who imagined the outcome, a portion was asked processing has the greater impact. to verbally elaborate on the reasons for the imagined outcome, while the remainder engaged in imagery Extremity of Decision Outcomes Considered and without verbal elaboration. The dependent variable was Consumer Satisfaction. In addition to its potentially the perceived likelihood that the event would occur as biasing effect on likelihood assessment and estimates imagined. Subjects who imagined the future outcome of conjunctive probabilities, elaborated imagery- may perceived a higher likelihood ofthe event occurring than lead to a positivity bias with regard to future decision did subjects who did not imagine the future event. outcomes (O'Neal 1974). The fact that imagery provides Moreover, verbal elaboration (a discursive processing a sensory analogue makes it natural for consumers to strategy) had no additional effect on expectations. This focus on outcomes that feel good. Note that the posi- effect has been explained in terms of the availability tivity bias is an indirect effect of imagery processing. heuristic: imagining outcomes increases their perceived The sensory analogue properties of imagery processing likelihood by making the outcomes more salient and cause consumers to focus on positive consequences. easier to recall. If imagery does promote a positivity bias, it has se- Several other studies have demonstrated that imag- rious implications for consumer satisfaction, especially ining or explaining a hypothetical future event increases when examined in conjunction with the biases discussed subjects' subjective probability estimates that the event above. For example, as a brand-based evaluation strat- will actually occur (Anderson 1983; Gregory, Cialdini. egy, imagery may reduce the number of brands that and Carpenter 1982; Hoch 1984; Sherman et al. 1981, consumers consider in their evoked set. Instead of thor- 1983, 1984). Sherman et al. (1984) told college students oughly evaluating a number of alternatives, consumers about a new disease that was increasingly prevalent on may focus on a single brand, imagining one or several campus. The disease was described to one group of sub- scenarios that are both positively biased and seen as jects in terms of several concrete, easy-to-imagine highly likely (e.g., conjunctive biases and perceived symptoms. To the other group, the disease was described likelihoods). The likelihood that the imagined scenario IMAGERY PROCESSING 481 will unfold as planned is quite low. Even if the actual imagery (such as daydreams) affects intentions to act outcome is favorable, it is likely to differ from the imag- (see McMahon 1973). Research in some areas has sub- ined outcome. Deviations ofthe actual outcome from stantiated this effect (Cautela and McCuiiough 1978; the imagined outcome give rise to surprise. Deviations Staats and Lohr 1979; Wolpe 1958). In clinical psy- of this sort have been noted in the satisfaction literature chology, research indicates that elaborated imagery of as an imponant cause of consumer dissatisfaction (An- a feared outcome is a major factor perpetuating phobias derson 1973). The effect of imagery on the extremity (intentions to avoid an object). Moreover, imagery- of the decision outcomes considered before purchase based treatment strategies can successfully reverse these and post-purchase satisfaction has not been investi- intentions (Bandura 1982; Cautela and McCuiiough gated. 1978; Wolpe 1958). At least in clinical contexts, imagery Individual difTerences in product- or purchase-related processing has been found to be more effective than knowledge are likely to influence the magnitude of a discursive processing in influencing behavioral inten- "halo effect" and the number of alternative scenarios tions (Cautela and McCuiiough 1978). The latter ex- generated. A better developed knowledge base allows plain imagery's effect on intentions as a form of "covert for the construction of greater numbers of alternative conditioning," suggesting that people approach (avoid) scenarios. Consequently, if consumers are willing and objects that they associate with positive (negative) re- sufficiently knowledgeable to imagine multiple scena- wards via imager>'. Similarly, Staats and Lohr (1979) rios, they are less likely to expect any one of these to view imagery as a stimulus that can elicit approach unfold as planned (Carroll 1978). Moreover, in a prob- (avoidance) responses. abilistic sense, there is an enhanced likelihood that at On the basis of several experiments, Lang (1977, least one of the scenarios will unfold as planned. In 1979) concludes that intentions are affected only if there addition, an experienced consumer might not only draw is an emotional reaction evoked by the imagined sce- favorable outcomes but may develop unfavorable out- nario. On the basis of experimental results he argues come scenarios as well. These imagined negative sce- that emotional reactions to imagined scenarios that in- narios not only affect the direction of expectations; they clude feared objects are highest when elaborated im- also make it possible for the individual to engage in pre- agery processing is used. In particular, the strongest ef- purchase behaviors that reduce the likelihood that the fects were observed when subjects imagined themselves negative imagined event will occur (risk-reduction interacting with the feared object (rather than simply strategies).' imagining the feared object or reading about the feared Based on this discussion, several propositions for the object). effects of imagery on probability assessment and con- Outside ofthe clinical context, there is little research sumer satisfaction can be developed: on the connection between imager> processing and be- P5: Consumers will estimate higher conjunctive havioral intentions. In one study, however, Gregor\ et probabilities for decision outcomes when an al. (1982) found that subjects who imagined themselves imagery processing mode as opposed to a enjoying the benefits of cable TV were more likely to subscribe to cable TV service than were subjects who discursive processing mode is used. only heard of these benefits. Anderson (1983) had sub- P6: Consumers will estimate higher likelihoods jects imagine either themselves, a liked friend, or a dis- for the decision outcomes when an imagery liked acquaintance performing (or not performing) a processing mode as opposed to a discursive set of behaviors (e.g., donating blood, taking a trip). processing mode is used. The effect on intentions was most dramatic for sell"- P7: When imagery processing is used, the level related imagery. Subjects who imagined themselves of prior knowledge moderates the relation- performing the set of behaviors significantly changed ship between imagery and satisfaction. Spe- their behavioral intentions. Furthermore, greater cifically, the lower the level of prior knowl- changes in intentions were found the longer subjects edge, tiie higher the level of purchase dissat- imagined themselves performing the behavior. The ef- isfaction (for a given level of decision fect of self-oriented imagery on intentions was also quite involvement). enduring (at least 3 days). The effect of self-related imagery- on intentions might Imagery Effects on Purchase Intentions be explained in two ways: by the concreteness of the and Purchase Timing imagined scenarios and/or by the greater emotionality ofthe imagery. It might be that individuals can imagine Imagery Effects on Purchase Intentions. Since an- their reactions to constructed scenarios better than they tiquity, philosophers have suggested that elaborated can imagine the reactions of others. Thus, the imagined scenario (including responses) is more concrete (An- 'Other individual differences (e.g., vs. ), past derson 1983). Alternatively, the effect might occur be- experiences, and particular circumstances may also affect the extent cause imagery is more emotionally provocative when of a posiliviiy bias. it is self- versus other-oriented. Recall that Lang's (1977. Ail THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

1979) results suggested that the extent to which imagery P9: Self-related imagery will affect intentions affects intentions is related to the emotionality of the more than will imagery that does not include image. Self-oriented imagery may also make more in- the self. formation available. Mowen (1980) examined the re- PIO: The more concrete and emotional the im- lationship between imagery and purchase intentions, agery, the greater the change in behavioral hypothesizing that imagery would make information intentions. more available and, hence, impact intentions. He found no effects. According to Mowen, however, several as- Pll: Controlling for ability to buy, elaborated pects ofthe study and the manipulations used may have imagery will reduce the delay between pur- led to the null effects. Thus this hypothesis deserves chase consideration and actual purchase. further testing. PI 2: The degree of elaboration moderates the re- Imagery Effects on Purchase Timing. Research on lationship between imagery and purchase delay of gratification (Mischel 1974; Mischel and Moore motivation. Specifically: 1973) indicates that the degree of elaboration invoked a: Controlling for ability to buy, elaborated in imagery can influence subjects' abilities to delay imagery will reduce the delay between gratification. It finds that imagery of having and using purchase consideration and actual pur- a desired object produces a positive sensory and emo- chase. tional experience, which in turn enhances desires for the object. The induced by elaborated imager% b: Controlling for ability to buy, visualizing reduce subjects' abilities to delay gratification. The im- the desired object (low-elaboration im- plicit reasoning is that while the imagery itself offers a agery) will increase the delay between positive sensory and emotional experience, actual con- purchase consideration and actual pur- sumption ofthe product would offer more tangible sen- chase. sory and, thus, emotional benefits. Research by Mischel also suggests that when subjects simply generate static Imagery as a Consumption Experience images of the desired object, but do not engage in an elaboration of these images, imagery enhances their One of the most important and unique qualities of abilities to delay gratification. While it is easy to see high-elaboration imager>' is that it is a sensory analogue how using imagery to produce a positive sensory and that provides the imager with a surrogate experience emotional experience would decrease the ability to delay (Arieti 1976). Doob (1972) proposes that high-elabo- gratification, it is not intuitive why imagining a static ration imagery- enables people to secure "substitute sat- image ofthe desired object (versus imagining nothing isfaction." Hilgard (1978), Lindauer (1972, 1983), and at all) would increase the ability to delay gratification. Singer (1978) provide evidence that high-elaboration Mischel (1974) proposes that generating an image of imagery offers a form of self-entertainment, gratifica- the object serves to remind individuals that the object tion, and stimulation. The ability to secure substitute is still there to be acquired at some future time. Because satisfaction through imager>' applies not only to pre- the image is not elaborated, emotional responses (and consumption activites. but to activities throughout the resulting frustrations) are kept to a minimum. phases of consumption: preconsumption. consumption, The relationship between elaborated imagery and and post-consumption. enhanced purchase desire is directly relevant to pro- Preconsitmpiion Imagery. At the preconsumption motional strategy. To be effective in reducing the delay stage, imager>' processing allows vicarious consumption between purchase consideration and actual purchase, through browsing or shopping. While researchers have persuasive communication strategies need to focus on noted that experiential pleasures can result from helping imagine the positive sensory and browsing and shopping (Bloch, Sherrell, and Ridgway emotional experiences they would derive from having 1986; Hirschman 1980), the extent to which imagery the object. Just reminding consumers ofthe product's affects satisfaction with these browsing and shopping existence is not sufficient to stimulate desires. In fact, activities has received little attention. it may even help them to delay purchase. Despite the In facilitating vicarious consumption, elaborated intuitive importance of the relationship between elab- imager)' is likely to be most valued and prevalent when orated imagery and reduced purchase acquisition time, consumers perceive actual consumption to be blocked this relationship has not been investigated in consumer by situational contingencies. For example, inability to research. buy, risk associated with product use, or other personal Summarizing the preceding discussion of imagerj^ ef- circumstances make imager>' an important tool for at- fects on purchase intentions and timing in the form of taining some of the entertainment, gratification, and propositions: stimulation that would derive from actual consumption. P8: There will be a greater change in behavioral The implication is that elaborated imagery is most use- intentions when elaborated imagerj' as op- ful as a substitute experience when actual consumption posed to discursive processing is used.' is not a viable alternative and when some ofthe benefits IMAGERY PROCESSING 483 of consumption can be attained from imagined con- keters include the role of imagery in the stimulation of sumption. Obviously, there are limits to the benefits hedonic and symbolic consumption, and satisfaction that can be attained with imagined consumption. In with hedonic and symbolic consumption activities. particular, since imagery offers a sensory substitute, it Understanding the fantasies consumers associate with is most useful when the benefits of product use involve the consumption of hedonic and symbolic products can sensor>' stimulation (as opposed to, for example, func- facilitate the promotion of these products. Further, un- tional benefits). Thus, elaborated imagery is most useful derstanding consumption imagery can provide mar- as a substitute experience when the benefits of con- keters with an important way of increasing satisfaction sumption are sensory and high, and the costs and/or with the consumption experience.'" If imagery enhances risks of product acquisition are also high. In this case the value of the consumption experience, products consumers can attain some of the benefits of product (especially hedonic and symbolic ones) can be designed consumption without incurring the costs. Interestingly, to stimulate imagery. Even consumption of certain whereas elaborated imagery with ability to buy can in- functional products may be enhanced by associating crease frustration and reduce the delay between pur- the product with positive imagery. For example, the chase consideration and actual purchase, elaborated communications research manager at Coke summarizes imager>- without the ability to buy may provide satis- one advertising strategy as imbuing the object with im- faction through a substitute sensory and emotional ex- agery to give it added value (Wall Street Journal 1984). perience. Thus, whether elaborated imagery processing He goes on to note that concrete cues can be associated leads to reduced or enhanced purchase desire depends with positive imagery such that the presence of these on whether actual consumption is viewed as possible cues in other circumstances elicits positive imagery re- or not (given the costs and risks of consumption). garding the product. Post-Consumption Imagery. Remembered con- Consumption Imagery. Imagery can also an sumption has long been viewed as an important input important role during the actual consumption experi- into decisions about future consumption activities (En- ence. Many products are purchased explicitly for the gel and Blackwell 1982). Nevertheless, little attention imagery they generate (Hirschman and Hol- has focused on the nature of remembered consumption. brook 1982; Holbrook and Hirschman 1982). Products Tendencies to save mementos, ticket stubs, and photos are often viewed as important for their role in stimu- indicates consumers' desires to relive consumption ex- lating imagery (e.g., records, books, movies). Playful periences. Imagery can play an important role in reliv- consumption, which includes leisure activities, sports, ing these experiences. Recent work indicates that valued games, etc, is a domain where imagery appears to play possessions are often treasured for the imagery expe- a panicularly important value-enhancing role (Hol- riences and they generate (Csikszentmihalyi brook et al. 1984). As such, the value of many products and Rochberg-Halton 1981: Wallendorf 1984). Consis- is a function of both intrinsic qualities and qualities tent with earlier discussions, research has indicated that imbued by imagery during the consumption experience. imagery of past experiences is most often associated Thus, imagery in the consumption phase has the po- with experiences and events that were particularly sa- tential for increasing product satisfaction. Lindauer lient and/or emotional at the time (Marks 1972). Hil- (1983) provides an extensive review of the use of im- gard's (1978) work refers to "reintegrating stimuli"— agery experiences in the pursuit of hedonic consump- stimuli that reinvoke a state-dependent retrieval ex- tion activities (e.g., listening to music, through litera- perience. She observes that stimuli are most successful ture, etc.). Research has suggested that some individuals in retrieving a memory image if they include some of report substantial levels of imagery when engaged in the direct perceptual properties (e.g., sights, sounds, hedonic consumption activities (Hilgard 1978). Nev- smells) associated with the remembered experience. ertheless, the connection between imagery processing These stimuli serve as concrete cues that elicit imagery. during consumption and levels of consumption satis- faction has not been established. Some indirect evidence Imager>' used to relive consumption experiences in consumer behavior of this connection is the finding should be of interest to marketers for several reasons. that enjoyment of games (positive feelings and pleasure) First, using imagery to relive a valued consumption ex- is a function of the congruency between the type of perience suggests that a consumption experience can game and the preferred cognitive style (imagery or dis- iive well beyond its typically conceived duration. Flow- cursive). Hence, at least for people who like to engage ers, champagne corks, photographs, and other items in imagery, products that facilitate imagery provide may be saved for a lifetime and used to relive important more pleasure and fun (Holbrook et al. 1984). events. Huge expenditures for events like weddings, proms, and special vacations are often justified not The relationship between imagery and hedonic and solely by their present value, but by their future value. symbolic consumption experiences offers a rich terrain for future consumer research. Unfortunately, as Lin- '"This would probably come as no surprise to the novelist or movie dauer (1983) notes, much ofthe research thus far has director concerned with helping audiences "live the story." Nev- been poorly conceptualized and methodologically ertheless, the importance of imager>- in other consumption activities flawed. Important and interesting questions for mar- has been overlooked. 484 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

If marketers want to encourage investment in appar- and Lutz and Lutz 1978 for reviews). The sources de- ently transient consumption experiences, they may find scribed briefly here are: pictures, concrete words, in- it useful to emphasize the lasting power of these expe- structions to imagine, and guided imagery. Throughout riences through imagery. Moreover, elaborated imagery this discussion it is useful to keep in mind that imagery after consumption can increase the remembered satis- can also occur in the absence of external stimuli simply faction of consumption. The same kind of "halo" effect via information voluntarily retrieved from memory. that appears to operate with imagery processing in gen- "Current concerns" or "unfinished business" often eral is likely to operate with imagery of remembered trigger daydreams of potential, anticipated, or future events. In this way, consumption experiences that in- outcomes (Klinger 1977). Needs for stimulation caused cluded a mixture of good and bad events may be re- by sensory deprivation also increase spontaneous im- membered in a positive light. Finally, elaborated im- agery (Singer 1978; Tushup and Zuckerman 1977). Fi- agery after consumption can encourage repurchase be- nally, needs to escape an excessively stimulating envi- havior. Recall that elaborated imagery, by providing a ronment can also give rise to an internal focus on im- positive sensory and emotional experience, is likely to agery (Singer 1978). 1 reduce ability to delay gratification when controlling for ability to buy." The relationship between elaborated Pictures. Pictures are well-established predictors of imagery and consumption experiences can be sum- imagery (Bugelski 1983; Finke 1980; Paivio 1971; Ros- marized in several propositions: siter 19'82; Shepard 1967; Singer 1978). The fact that visual information tends to be remembered better than PI3: Vicarious consumption facilitated by imag- verbal information (the picture superiority effect) has er>' will be most satisfying when the perceived also been explained through imagery (Alesandrini and benefits from actual consumption are sen- Sheikh 1983; Childers and Houston 1984; Lutz and sory in nature and the cost/risks of actual Lutz 1977; Paivio 1971). However, pictures need not consumption are perceived as prohibitive. induce only imagery processing (Kieras 1978; Rossiter and Percy 1983); hence, it is dangerous to equate the P14: Consumption satisfaction for hedonic and mode of information presentation with its representa- symbolic products will be higher when elab- tion in working memory (Rossiter and Percy 1983). orated imagery as opposed to discursive pro- With the increased focus on the impact of pictures in cessing is used. consumer information processing (e.g.. Childers and P15: Consumers will report higher levels of sat- Houston 1984; Edell and Staelin 1983; Kisielius and isfaction with the recall of a past consump- Sternthal 1984; Rossiter 1982; Rossiter and Percy 1983). tion experience when elaborated imagery as understanding the conditions under which pictures opposed to discursive processing is used. produce imagery becomes important. PI6: Elaborated imagery of a past consumption Concrete Hord.s. Concrete words can stimulate the experience will increase intention to repur- generation of imagery as well (Paivio and Csapo 1973: chase (controlling for ability to buy). Paivio and Foth 1970; Paivio. Yuille. and Madigan 1968: Richardson 1980). Ratings ofthe concreteness of CONDUCTING RESEARCH words are highly related to their rated imagery value ON IMAGERY (Paivio et al. 1968). Recent work by Cartwright (1980: Canwright. Marks, and Durrett 1978) indicates that The final section addresses issues involved in the abstract words can be made imaginal by instructing conduct of research on imagery. Intended as a brief subjects to think of an imager>'-based exemplar. For overview of research on the manipulation and mea- example, abstract words like "peace" and "quarrel" can surement of imagery processing, the section's primary' be made concrete by evoking an image of a dove (an purpose is to acquaint the unfamiliar reader with the exemplar of peace) or an image of people actually en- basic tools for conducting imagery research. Two do- gaged in a quarrel. Thus, the relationship between con- mains, summarized in the Figure, are reviewed: (1) crete and abstract words and imagery' may be more strategies for manipulating imagery, and (2) strategies complex than initially thought. for measuring the extent of imagery processing. Instructions to Imagine. Instructions to imagine Imagery-Eliciting Strategies represent another external source to the generation of imagery. Some marketing researchers (Mowen 1980; As the Figure indicates, a number of external sources Wright and Rip 1980) have relied on instructions to can induce imagery (see Alesandrini and Sheikh 1983 imagine in stimulating imagery. These studies reported negligible effects on learning and attitudes from the im- "In this conlcxl it is useful to note that certain consumption ex- agery manipulation. However, other studies using this periences may be viewed as unrepeatable and hence positive imagery manipulation have reported important and significant would not imply repurchase. Howeyer. as one reviewer noted, a pos- itive imagery experience may generate word-of-mouth communica- effects (cf. Carroll 1978: Gregory et al. 1982; Sherman tions that affect others" purchase ofthe consumption experience. et al. 1984). Rossiter (1982) suggests that in an adver- IMAGERY PROCESSING 485

tising context, instructions to imagine are less effective pling," where the experimenter interrupts individuals as imagery-eliciting strategies than are "high imagery involved in imagery processing and asks them to de- visuals." However, this interpretation should be qual- scribe the content of their imaginal experiences. In a ified. Instructions to imagine may be ineffective in sit- related procedure called "event sampling," imager>' uations where little schematic knowledge exists (Wright content is assessed by asking subjects to indicate when- and Rip 1980) or where external stimulation interferes ever a certain type of image is aroused. Smith et al. with the generation of imagery (Mowen 1980). How- (1984) asked subjects to write out their script for visiting ever, imagery instructions may be an important ma- the school placement office and indicate whether im- nipulation strategy when consumers are allowed the agery was present or absent and high or low in vividness. time to generate vivid imagery, when cues are concrete Rethans and Hastak (1981) relied on a free-elicitation (Paivio and Csapo 1973; Richardson 1983), when in- procedure described by Olson and Muderrisoglu (1979) structions focus on subjects' reactions to the image to tap imagery associated with product hazards. It is (Lang 1979), and when consumers have sufficient possible to criticize these measurements on several knowledge to generate imagery about reactions (Re- counts. First, most require that respondents translate thans and Hastak 1981; Smith, Houston, and Childers imager}' processing into a discursive mode (e.g., verbal 1983). responses), confusing even more the distinction between the processing modes. Moreover, individual differences Guided Imagery. "Guided imagery" (Wollman in verbosity or vocabulary can influence the nature of 1981) is an imager>'-eliciting strategy that resembles this translation. In addition, studies that ask individuals imager>-based methods used in clinical settings. Par- to detail imager\' scenarios may not be assessing the ticipants are first asked to relax and then practice de- extent of detail contained in the image, but rather sub- veloping vivid imagery scenarios. To facilitate imagery jects' abilities to control their image so that details can evocation and vividness, participants are given cues that be specified. help to guide their imagery. The procedure is repeated for several trials, and finally, subjects are asked to Physiological reactions are also used as indicators of imagine the object that is the focus ofthe study. Woll- imagery processing. wave patterns (see Ley 1983) man (1981) warns that the usefulness of this procedure are presumed to indicate the presence of imagery, but may be confined to those who have vivid, controllable are extremely difficult to interpret and highly intrusive. imagery\ Demand characteristics may also operate with Some have suggested that muscular ac- this procedure. tivation mirrors imagery content and have therefore used electromyogram readings to assess imagery- content (see Ley 1983). Again, however, these measures are in- Measuring Imagery Processing ferential and highly obtrusive. Moreover, assessments require a considerable level of technical sophistication. Measuring the presence and content of imagery rep- resents a significant challenge to researchers. Several .Measuring Individual Differences in Imagery Pro- different methods have been used to measure imagery cessing. A number of scales have been developed to processing, each with its own unique limitations. Cur- study the role that individual differences play in imager\ rently, few guidelines for imager>' measurement have processing (see Ernest 1977 and Sheehan et al. 1983 for been established, but a considerable amount of research reviews of several of these scales). The scales can be has focused on developing scales to measure individual classified into four basic categories: (1) scales of imager>- differences in imagery- processing. The next section ability (vividness and controllability). (2) scales of im- provides a brief overview of this research. agery content, (3) scales of spatial ability, and (4) scales of imagery vs. verbal processing styles. Overview of Imagery-Measurement Approaches. Scales of imagery ability assess individual differences Many researchers rely on criterion-based responses, not in imagery vividness and controllability. Imagery viv- manipulation checks, in assessing imagery processing. idness is a known moderator of imagery effects in in- Thus. imager>' processing is manipulated by one ofthe cidental learning (Ernest 1977: Sheehan and Neisser strategies already discussed and inferred from the results 1969: Swann and Miller 1982). It also moderates the (cf. Anderson 1983; Carroll 1978; Lutz and Lutz 1978; relationship between imagery and emotional and phys- Paivio 1971; Sheehan 1972). An important exception iological arousal (Drummond. White, and Ashton 1978: is the research by Lutz and Lutz (1977). They manip- Grossberg and Wilson 1968). Two commonly used ulated mental imagery with the use of pictures, but also scales to assess individual differences in imagery viv- provided simple manipulation checks to ensure that idness are The Betts Questionnaire Upon Mental Im- groups provided only with verbal stimuli generated less agery (QMI: see Durndell and Wetherick 1976: Rich- mental imager\^ than the experimental groups. ardson 1977; Sheehan 1967; Westcoff and Rosenstock Other researchers have given explicit consideration 1976) and Marks' Vividness of Visual Imagery Ques- to measuring the content of imagery processing using tionnaire (see Childers et al. 1985; Marks 1973: Rossi both simple and complex verbal responses. Klinger 1977). The controllability of imagery has also been (1978) recommends a procedure called "thought sam- demonstrated to influence a number of cognitive pro- THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH 486 cesses, including mental rotations (Ernest 1977). A Empirical evidence is mounting that imagery processing commonly used measure of imagery control was de- affects a multitude of cognitive, physiological, and be- veloped by Gordon (1949) and modified by Richardson havioral phenomena. In a marketing context, imagery (1969; see Childers et al. 1985; Westcott and Rosenstock processing is likely to be important in understanding incidental learning, numerous facets ofthe choice pro- Scales of imagery content have been developed by cess, the likelihood and timing of purchases, and the Singer and Antrobus (1963, 1972) and Huba et al. nature of many hedonic and symbolic consumption ex- (1982). The shorter version ofthe Imaginal Process In- periences and re-experiences (remembered consump- ventory (IPl; Huba et al. 1982) taps imagery vividness, tion). The purpose of this article has been to review the use of imagery to relieve boredom, general feelings research on imagery processing, contrast it with dis- about the usefulness of imagery, the use of imagery in cursive (symbolic, language-like) processing, and sug- anticipating the future and solving problems, fantasy gest important ways in which imagery impacts con- imagery, distractibility, and emotional arousal from sumers' learning, choices, and satisfaction. The article imagery. The scale is infrequently used in research ap- has also considered the role that imagery is likely to plications, but has adequate internal consistency and play throughout the phases of consumption. Although test-retest reliabilities. other research suggests that imagery is pervasive, little Spatial ability tests are designed to assess individuals' is known about its relative impact in consumption con- abilities to hold pictures of visual objects in memory texts. and perform mental operations (e.g., mental rotations). A review ofthe research on imagery processing sug- While clearly related to the controllability of imagery, gests several important possibilities to consumer be- tests of spatial ability are generally independent of tests havior researchers. First, imagery is likely to have a of imagery vividness (Richardson 1983). Several tests positive impact on incidental learning. Since much of of spatial ability are available and commonly used (see consumer learning is incidental, imagery may be even more important in consumer contexts than in other Bennett, Seashore, and Wesman 1947; Likert and problem-solving contexts. Second, imagery processing Quasha 1970; Shepard and Meltzer 1971; Thurstone may encourage within-brand product evaluation strat- 1938). egies. Within-brand strategies, in turn, have implica- Finally, scales to measure processing style are de- tions for how missing information is dealt with. Third, signed to assess preferences for using imagery versus the conditions for decision complexity are likely to differ verbal processing in thinking, studying, and problem between imagery processing and cognitive algebra solving. The Visualizer-Verbalizer Questionnaire (WQ) models. In particular, imagery processing seems best is a commonly used measure developed by Richardson suited for the evaluation of very few brands on many (1977). The VVQ has been shown to influence imagery attributes. Fourth, imagery niay bias estimates of con- effects on cognitive processing (Ernest 1977) and to me- junctive probabilities and estimates of the likelihood diate the relationship between visually presented ads and and positivity of outcomes. These biased estimates may measures of attitudes (Rossiter and Percy 1978). Two lead to disappointment with post-purchase outcomes. measures have been developed in consumer behavior that Fifth. imager>' (especially self-related imagery') affects appear to overcome some of the criticisms of the VVQ. purchase intentions and the timing of purchases (by Childers et al. (1985) developed a new measure called the inhibiting the ability to delay gratification). Finally, Style of Processing Questionnaire (SOP), which more imagery can offer a positive sensory and emotional ex- clearly differentiates ability dimensions from processing perience that can substitute for consumption (e.g., vi- preferences. The SOP scale has been related to measures carious consumption), enhance or supplement con- of advertising recognition and recall (Childers et al. 1985). sumption (i.e., make hamburger taste more like steak), Another scale, the V/V Index, takes as its starting point and play an important role in remembered consump- the Prevalence of Visual Imagery Test (PVIT) developed tion and intention to repurchase. by Kunzendorf (1981, 1982) to resolve some ofthe dif- ficulties with the VVQ. Developed by Holbrook et al. While this article has outlined some promising do- (1984), the V/V Index differs from the PVIT by using mains for imagery research and provided a basic over- both pictorial and verbal anchors for each scale, thus view of some of the tools of imagery research, much overcoming some of the earlier criticisms of measuring remains to be done. Even such basic issues as what fac- imagery preferences on verbal scales. tors stimulate imagery processing and how imagery processing is to be measured are far from resolved. Re- search is needed to explore the types of consumption SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS problems consumers represent with imagery and the ways that imagery interacts with consumption experi- Even though there is considerable debate about the ences. Similarly, while this article focused on concep- meaning of imagery processing and the causal role of tually distinguishing imagery processing from discursive imagery in problem-solving and sensory experiences, processing, there are likely to be important interplays there is little doubt that imagery seems to play an im- between these two processing modes. Research inves- portant role in information processing (Block 1981). tigating the stages in the decision process where imagery IMAGERY PROCESSING 487

is most commonly used would be important in describ- Arieti, Silvano (1976), : The .\fagic Synthesis. New ing part ofthe interplay between imagery and discursive York: Basic Books. processing. Bandura, Alben (1982), "Self-Efficacy Mechanisms in Human Significant challenges lie ahead in imager>' research. Agency," .4wencan . 37 (2), 122-147. Methodological practices need improvement, and Batra, Rajeev and Michael L. Ray (1983), "Advenising Sit- theoretical grounding is necessary. Because the science uations: The Implications of Differential Involvement and Accompanying Affect Responses," in Informaiion is in its infancy, imagery research—and elaborated im- Processing Research in Advertising, ed. Richard J. Harris, agery processing, in particular—offers an opportunity Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 127-152. to construct theory. Consumption-related choices and Bennett, George K., Harold G. Seashore, and Alexander G. experiences provide a rich domain for theory construc- Wesman (1947), Differential .Aptitude Tests. New York: tion. The nature of learning, problem-solving, and ex- The Psychological Corporation. periences in the consumer domain are well suited to Bettman, James R. (1979), An Information Processing Theory exploration of elaborated imagery processing. Con- of Consumer Choice, Reading. MA: Addison-Wesley. sumers often make decisions with less than full infor- and Pradeep Kakkar (1977), "Effects of Informaiion mation, considering only one or two alternatives. Presentation Format on Consumer Information Acqui- Moreover, in many cases evaluation ofthe decision re- sition Strategies," Journal of Consumer Research. 3 quires information about sensor>' and emotional reac- (March), 233-240. tions to the experience. For example, in the choice of and C. Whan Park (1980), "The Effects of Prior Knowledge and Experience and Phase of the Choice many services and symbolic or hedonic products, the Process on Consumer Decision Processes," Journal of most important part of the choice involves assessing Consumer Research, 7 (December), 234-248. how it will feel (the sensations surrounding the antici- and Michael .A. Zins (1977). "Constructive Processes pated consumption). Imagery processing provides a in Consumer Choice." Journal of Consumer Research. uniquely appropriate mode for evaluating the sensory 4 (September). 75-87. qualities of products. Finally, a considerable proponion Biehal. Gabriel and Dipankar Chakravani (1982). "Infor- of people's hopes and focuses on consumption mation-Presentation Format and Learning Goals as De- related experiences—spending a million dollars, owning terminants of Consumers' Memor>- Retrieval and Choice a fast car, building their own home. As such, consumer Processes," Journal of Consumer Research. 8 (March), 431-442. researchers are provided with a natural environment in and Dipankar Chakravani (1986), "Consumers" Use which to explore some of consumers' most important of Memory and External Information in Choice: Macro imager>' experiences. and Micro Perspectives," Journal of Consumer Research 12 (March). 382-405. [Received February 1985. Revised August 1986.] Bloch. Peter H.. Daniel L. Sherrell. and Nancy M. Ridgway (1986), "Consumer Search: .An Extended Framework." Journal of Consumer Research. 13 (June), 119-127. Block. Ned (1981). Imagery. Cambridge. MA: MIT Press. Bower. Gordon (1970). "Imagery as a Relational Organizer REFERENCES in Associative Learning." Journal ofl'erhal Learning and ierbal Behavior. 9. 529-533. .'Vbelson. Roben P. (1976). "Script Processing in .Attitude —(1972), "Mental Imagery and Associative Learning." Formation and Decision Making." in and So- in Cognition in Learning and .Memory, ed. Lee Gregg, cial Behavior eds. John S. Carroll and John W. Payne. .New York: John Wiley. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 33-45. Brucks. Merrie (1985). "The Effectsof Product Class Knowl- Ajzen. Icek and Manin Fishbein (1980), Understanding .At- edge on Information Search Behavior." Journal of Con- titudes and Predicting Social Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, sumer Research. 12 (June). 1-16. NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bugelski, B. Richard (1983), "Imagery and the Thought Pro- Alesandrini, Kathy and Aness Sheikh (1983), "Research on cesses," in Imagery: Current Theory. Research and .Ap- Imagery: Applications to Advenising." in Imagery: Cur- plication, ed. Aness Sheikh. New '^'ork: John Wiley. 72- rent Theory. Research and Application, ed. Aness Sheikh. 95. New York: John Wiley, 535-556. Burnkrant, Roben E. and Alan G. Sawyer (1983), "Effects of Anderson. Craig A. (1983), " and Expectation: Involvement and Message Content on Information Pro- The Effect oflmagining Behavioral Scripts on Personal cessing Intensity," in Information Processing Research Intentions." Journal of Personality and Social Psvclwl- in.Advertising, ed. Richard J. Harris, Hillsdale, NJ: Law- ogy. 45 (2), 293-305. rence Erlbaum, 43-64. Anderson. John R. (1978), "Arguments Concerning Repre- Butter, M. Johnna (1970), "Differential Recall of Paired As- sentations for Mental Imagery," Psvchological Review sociates as a Function of Arousal and Concreteness of 85.249-277. Imagery Levels." Journal of E.xperimental Psvcholoi:v. and Gordon H. Bower (1973). Human .A.'ssociative 84. 252-256. .A/fwory. Washington. D.C.: Winston. Cacioppo, John T. and Richard E. Petty (1984). "The Elab- .Anderson. Rolph E. (1973), "Consumer Dissatisfaction: The oration Likelihood Model of ." in .Advances Effect of Disconfirmed Expectancy on Perceived Product in Consumer Research, Vol. II, ed. Thomas Kinnear, Imponance." Journal of .Marketing Research. 10 (Feb- Ann Arbor. MI: Association for Consumer Research ruary), 38-44. 673-675. THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH 488

Calder, Bobby J. (1978), "Cognitive Response, Imagery and Reported Imagery to Cognitive Performance." British Scripts: What is the Cognitive Basis ofAttitude?" in Ad- Journal of , 67,501 -506. vances in Consumer Research, Vol. 5, ed. H. Keith Hunt, Edell, Julie A. and Richard Staelin (1983), "The Information Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research, Processing of Pictures in Print Advertisements," Journal 630-634. of Consumer Research, 10 (June), 45-61. Carroll, John (1978), "The Effect oflmagining an Event on Einhorn, Hillel J. and Robin M. Hogarth (1984), "Probable Expectations for the Event: An Interpretation in Terms Cause: A Decision Making Framework," unpublished ofthe Availability Heuristic," Journat of Experimental paper. University of Chicago, Graduate School of Busi- , 14,88-96. ness, Center for Decision Research. Engel, James F. and Roger D. Blackwell (1982), Consumer Cartwright, Desmond S. (1980), "Exploratory Analyses of Verbally Stimulated Imagery of the Self," Journal of Behavior, New York: Dryden. Mental Imagery, 4, 1-21. Ernest, Carole (1977), "Irhagery Ability and Cognition: A , Mao' E. Marks, and John H. Durrett (1978). "Def- Critical Review," Journal of Mental Imagery, 1. 181- inition and Measurement of Three Processes of Imagery 216. Representation: Exploratory Studies of Verbally Stim- Finke, Ronald A. (1980), "Levels of Equivalence in Imagery ulated Imagery," Multivariate Behaviorat Research. 13, and ," Psychological Review. 87 (March), 113- 449-473. 132. Cautela, Joseph R. and Leigh McCuiiough (1978), "Covert Fischhoff, Baruch (1985), "Judging Unlikely Conjunctions." Conditioning: A Learning Theory Perspective on Im- working paper. Decision Research Institute, Eugene. agery," in The Power of Human Imagination, eds. J.L. Oregon. Singer and K.S. Pope, New York: Plenum, 227-250. Fiske, Susan T. (1982), "Schema-Triggered Affect: Applica- Chaiken, Shelly (1980), "Heuristic vs. Systematic Information tions to Social Perception," in Affect and Cognition, the Processing and the Use of Source vs. Message Cues in Seventeenth Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition. Persuasion," Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- eds. Margaret Sydt»or Clark and Susati T. Fiske. Hillsdale. ogy. 39 (5), 752-766. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 55-77. Childers, Terry L. and Michael Houston (1982), "Imagery Fodor, Jerry A. (1981), "Imagistic Representation," in Im- Paradigms forConsumer Research: Alternative Perspec- agerv, ed. Ned Block, Cambridge. MA: MIT Press, 63- tives from ," in Advances in Con- 86. • sumer Research, Vol. 10, eds. Alice M. Tybout and Ford, Gary T. and Ruth Ann Smith (1985), "Inferential Beliefs Richard P. Bagozzi, Ann Arbor, MI: Association for In Consumer Evaluations: An Assessment of Alternative Consumer Research, 59-64. Processing Strategies," working paper, American Uni- and Michael Houston (1984), "Conditions for a Pic- versity. ture Superiority Effect on Consumer Memory," Journal Giambra, Leonard M. (1977), "A Factor Analytic Study of of Consumer Research. II (September), 643-655. Daydreaming, Imaginal Processes and Temperament: A -, Michael J. Houston, and Susan E. Heckler (1985), Replication on an Adult Male Life-Span Sample," Jour- "Measurement of Individual Differences in Visual vs. nal of Gerontology. 32, 675-680. Verbal Processing." Journal of Consumer Research. 12 Gordon. Rosemary (1949), "An Investigation into Some of (September), 125-134. the Factors that Favour the Formation of Stereotyped Cohen, Joe! B. (1982), "The Role of Affect in Categorization: Images." British Journal of Psychology. 39, 156-167. Toward a Reconceptualization of the Concept of Atti- Greenwald. Anthony G. (1968), "Cognitive Learning. Cog- tude," in Advances in Consumer Research. Vol. 9, ed. nitive Response to Persuasion and Attitude Change," in Andrew A. Mitchell, Ann Arbor, Ml: Association for Psychological Foundations of Attitudes, eds. Anthony Consumer Research, 94-100. Greehwald, Timothy C. Brock, and Timothy C. Ostrom. Craik. Fergus I.M. and Robert S. Lockhart (1972), "Levels New York: Academic Press. of Processing: A Framework for Memory Research," and Clark Leavitt (1984), " Involvement in Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, Advertising: Four Levels." Journal of Consumer Re- 671-684. search. 11 (June), 581-592. and (1975), "Depth of Processing and Gregory, W. Larry, Robert B. Cialdini, and Kathleen M. Car- the Retention of Words in Episodic Memory." Journal penter (1982), "Self-relevant Scenarios as Mediators of of : General. 104, 268-294. Likelihood Estimates and Compliance: Does Imagining Csikszentmihalyi. Mihaly and Eugene Rochberg-Halton Make It So?" Journal of Personality and Social P.u- (1981), The Meaning of Things: Domestic Symbols and chology, 43, 89-99. the Self New York: Cambridge University Press. Grether, David and Louis Wilde (1984), "An Analysis of Deckert, Gordon H. (1964), "Pursuit Eye Movements in the Conjunctive Choice: Theory and Experiments," Journal Absence of Moving Visual Stimulus." Science, 143. of Consumer Research. 10(4), 373-385. 1192-1193. Grossberg, John M. and Helen F. Wilson (1968). "Physio- Doob, Leonard (1972), "The Ubiquitous Appearance of Im- logical Changes Accompanying the Visualization of ages," in The Function and Nature of Imagery, ed. Peter Fearful and Neutral Situations," Journal of Personality Sheehan. New York: Academic Press. 311-331. and Social Psychology 10, 124-13 3. Drummond. Peter, Ken White, and Rod Ashton (1978). Hilgard, Ernest R. (1981). "Imagery and Imagination in "Imagerv' Vividness and Habituation Rate," Fsycho- American Psychology." Journal of Mental tmagery. 5. phvsiolo'gy. 15 (May), 193-195. 5-19. Durndell, A.J. and N.E. Wetherick (1976), "The Relation of Hilgard, Josephine R. (1978), : A Study of I magi- IMAGERY PROCESSING 489

native Involvement, Chicago, IL: University of Chicago (1981), "The Medium atid the Message in Mental Press. Imagery: A Theory," Psvchological Review, 88 (January), Hirschman, Elizabeth C. (1980), "Innovativeness, Novelty 46-66. Seeking, and Consumer Creativity," Journal of Con- , Steven Pinker, George E. Smith, and Steven Shwartz sumer Research, 1 (December), 283-295. (1981), "On Demystification of Mental Imagery," in and Morris B. Holbrook (1982), "Hedonic Con- Imagery, ed. Ned Block, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, sumption: Emerging Concepts, Methods, and Proposi- 131-150. tions," Journal of Marketing, 46 (Summer), 92-101. and James R. Pomerantz (1977), "Imagery, Propo- Hoch, Stephen J. (1984), "Availability and Interference in sitions and the Form of Internal Representations," Cog- Predictive Judgment," Journal of Experimental Psy- nitive Psychology, 9, 52-76. chology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10 (4), 649- , Brian Reiser, Martha Farah, and Sharon Fliegel 662. (1983), "Generating Visual Images: Units and Rela- Holbrook, Morris B., Robert W. Chestnut, Terence A. Oliva, tions," Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112 and Eric A. Greenleaf (1984), "Play as a Consumption (2), 278-303. Experience: The Roles of Emotions, Performance, and Kunzendorf, Robert G. (1981), "Individual Differences in Personality in the Enjoyment of Games," Journal of Imagery and Autonomic Control," Journat of Mental Consumer Research. 11 (September), 728-739. Imagery. 5 (Fall), 47-60. and Elizabeth C. Hirschman (1982), "The Experiential (1982), "Mental Images, Appreciation of Grammatical Aspects of Consumption: Consumer Fantasies, Feelings, Patterns and Creativity," Journal of Mental Imagery. 6. and Fun," Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (September), 183-202. 132-140. Lang, Peter J. (1977), "Imagery in : An Information and William L. Moore (1981a), "Cue Configurality Processing Analysis of ," Behavior Therapy, 8, 862- in Esthetic Responses," in Symbolic Consumer Behavior, 886. eds. Elizabeth C. Hirschman and Morris B. Holbrook, (1979), "A Bio-Informational Theory of Emotional Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research. 16- j',"' , 16 (6), 495-512. 25. Ley. Robert G. (1983), "Cerebral Laterality and Imager\," and William L. Moore(1981b), "Feature Interactions in Imagery: Current Theory. Research and .Application. in Consumer Judgments of Verbal Versus Pictorial Pre- ed. Aness Sheikh, New York: John Wiley, 252-287. sentations," Journal of Consumer Research, 8 (June), Likert, Rensis and Howard W. Quasha (1970), Revised Min- 103-113. nesota Paper Form Board Test. New York: The Psycho- Huba, G.J., J.L. Singer, C.S. Aneshensel, and J.S. Antrobus logical Corporation. (1982), Short Imaginat Processes Inventory, Port Huron, Lindauer, Martin S. (1972), "The Sensory Attributes and MI: Research Psychologists Press. Functionsof Imagery and Imagery Evoking Stimuli." in Huber, Joel and John McCann (1982), "The Impact of In- The Function and Nature of Imagery, ed. Peter Sheehan. ferential Beliefs on Product Evaluations." Journal of New York: Academic Press, 132-148. Marketing Research, 10 (August). 324-333. (1983). "Imagery' and the Arts." in Imagery: Current Johnson, Eric J. and J. Edward Russo (1980), "Product Fa- Theory. Research and .Application, ed. Aness Sheikh. New miliarity and Learning New Information," in .Advances York: John Wiley, 468-506. in Consumer Research, Vol. 8. ed. Kent B. Monroe. .Ann Lingle, John H. and Thomas Ostrom (1979), "Retrieval Se- Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research. 151- lectivity in Memory-Based Impression Judgments." 155. Journal of Personality and Social Psvclwlogv. 37 (2). Kahneman, Daniel R. and (1982), "The Sim- 180-194. ulation Heuristic," in Judgments Under Uncertainty: Loken, Barbara and Ronald Hoverstad (1985), "Relationships Heuristics and Biases, eds. Daniel R. Kahneman, Paul Between Information Recall and Subsequent Attitudes: Slovic, and Amos Tversky, New York: Cambridge Uni- Some Exploratory Findings," Journal of Consumer Re- versity Press, 201-208. search. 12 (September), 155-168. Kieras, David (1978), "Beyond Pictures and Words: Alter- Lussier, Denis A. and Richard W. Olshavsky (1979), "Task native Information Processing Models for Imagery Effects Complexity and Contingent Processing in Brand in Verbal Memory," Psychological Bulletin, 85,532-554. Choice," Journal of Consumer Research, 6 (September), Kisielius, Jolita and Brian Sternthal (1984), "Detecting and 154-165. Explaining Vividness Effects in Attitudinal Judgments." Lutz, Kathryn A. and Richard J. Lutz (1977), "The Effects Journal of Marketing Research, 21 (February), 54-64. of Interactive Imagery and Learning: Application to Ad- Klinger, Eric (1977), Meaning and Void: Inner Experience vertising," Journal of". 62, 493-498. and the Incentives in Peoptes' Lives. Minneapolis, MN: and Richard J. Lutz (1978), "Imagery-Eliciting Strat- University of Minnesota Press. egies: Review and Implications of Research," in Advances (1978), "Modes of Normal Conscious Flow," In The in Consumer Research. Vol. 5, ed. H. Keith Hunt, Ann : Scientific Investigations Into Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research, 611- the Flow of Human Experience, eds. Kenneth Pope and 620. Jerome Singer, New York: Plenum, 225-258. Malhotra, Naresh K. (1982), "Information Load and Con- Kolers, PaulA. (1983), "Perception and Representation," sumer Decision Making." Journat of Consumer Re- Annual Review of Psychology. 34, 129-166. search, S (March). 4\9-430. Kosslyn. Stephen M. (1975), "Information Representation in Marks, David F. (1972), "Individual Differences in the Viv- Visual Images," Cognitive Psychology. 7, 341-370. idness of Visual Imager)' and Their Effects on Function," 490 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

in The Function and Nature of Imagery, ed. Peter S. diators and Noun Concreteness in Paired Associate Sheehan, New York: Academic Press, 83-107. Learning: The Elusive Interaction," Journal of Verbal (1973), "Visual Imagery Differences in the Recall of Learning and Behavior, 9, 384-390. Pictures," British Journal of Psychology, 64 (1), 17-24. -, John C. Yuille, and Stephen A. Madigan (1968), McMahon, C.E. (1973), "Images as Motives and Motivators: "Concreteness, Imageo' and Meaningfulness Values for A Historical Perspective," Journal of Psychology, 86 (3), 925 Nouns," Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 465-490. 1-25. Mervis, Carolyn B., and Eleanor Rosch (1981), "Categori- Park. C. Whan and V. Parker Lessig (1981), "Familiarity and zation of Natural Objects," Annual Review of Psychology, its Impact on Consumer Decision Biases and Heuristics," 32,89-115. Journal of Consumer Research, 8 (September). 223-230. Meyer, Robert J. (1981), "A Model of Multiattribute Judg- and Banwari Mittal (1985). "A Theory of Involvement ments Under Attriljute Uncertainty and Informational in Consumer Behavior: Problems and Issues." in Re- Constraint." Journal of Marketing Research, 18 (No- search in Consumer Behavior, Vol. 1, ed. Jagdish N. vember). 428-441. Sheth, Greenwich. CT: JAI Press, 201-231. (1982). "A Descriptive Model of Consumer Infor- Perky, C.W. (1910), "An Experimental Study of Imagina- mation Search Behavior," .A/a/-A:e//ng SdeMCf, 1 (Winter), tion," Journal of Psychology, 21, 422-425. 93-121. Petty. Richard E. and John Cacioppo (1983). Attitudes and .Miller, George A., Eugene Galanter, and Karl H. Pribram Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary .Approaches, Du- (1960). Plans and the Structure of Behavior. New York: buque. IA: William C. Brown. Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Piaget. Jean and Barbel Inhelder (1973). .Memory and Intel- Mischel, Walter (1974), "Processes in Delay of Gratification," ligence, New York: Basic Books. in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol. 7, Pinker. Steven and Stephen M. Kosslyn (1983), "Theories of ed. L. Berkowitz, New York: Academic Press. 238-292. Mental Imagery," in Imagery: Current Theory. Research and Bert Moore (1973), "Effects of Attention to Sym- and Application, ed. Aness Sheikh, New York: John bolically Presented Rewards on Self-Control," Journat Wiley, 43-71. of'Personatity and Social Psychology 28, 172-179. Pylyshyn. Zenon W. (1973). "What the Mind's Eye Tells the Miichell. Andrew A. (1981). "The Dimensions of Advertising Mind's Brain: A Critique of Mental Imagery," Psycho- Involvement," in Advances in Consumer Research. Vol. logical Bulletin. 80(1). 1-22. 8, ed. Kent B. Monroe, Ann Arbor. Ml: Association for (1981). "The Imagery Debate: Analogue Media vs. Consumer Research, 25-30. Tacit Knowledge," Psychological Review. 85 (1), 16-45. Mowen. John C. (1980), "The Availability Heuristic: The Ef- Rethans, Arno and Manoj Hastak (1981), "Representations fect oflmagining the Use of a Product on Intentions to of Product Hazards in Consumer Memory," Working Purchase," unpublished paper. University of Oklahoma. Paper #116. Pennsylvania State University. .Nelson. Douglas L.. Valerie S. Reed, and Cathy L. McEnvoy Richardson, Alan (1969), .Mental Imagery. London: Rout- (1977), "Learning to Order Pictures and Words: .\ Model ledge and Kegan Paul. of Sensory and Semantic Encoding." Journal of Exper- (1977). "Verbalizer. Visualizer: A Cognitive Style Di- imental Psvchology: Human Learning and Memory. 3 mension." Journal of .Menial Imagery. 1. 109-125. (5). 485-497. • (1983). "Imagery: Definition and Types." in Imagery- Olson. Jerry C. and Aydin Muderrisoglu (1979), "The Stability Current Theory. Research and .Application, ed. .^ncss of Responses Obtained by Free Elicitation: Implications Sheikh. New York: John Wiley, 3-42. for Measuring Attribute Salience and Memory Struc- Richardson. John T. (1980). "Concreteness. Imagery, and ture." in .Advances in Consumer Research. Vol. 6. ed. Semantic Categorization." Journal of .Menial tmagery. William L. Wilkie, Ann Arbor, Ml: Association forCon- 4.51-58. sumer Research, 269-275. Rosch. Eleanor (1978). "Principles of Categorization." in ^. Daniel R. Toy, and Phillip A. Dover (1982). "Do Cognition and Categorization, eds. Eleanor Rosch and Cognitive Responses Mediate the Effectiveness of Ad- Barbara L. Lloyd. Hillsdale. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. vertising Content on Cognitive Structure?" Journal of Rossi. Joseph S. (1977). "Reliability of a Measure of Visual Consumer Research. 9 (December), 245-262. Imagery." Perceptual and .Motor Skills. 45. 694. O"Neal, Edgar (1974), "Influence of Future Choice Impor- Rossiter, John R. (1982), "Visual Imagery: Applications to tance and .\rousal Upon the Halo Effect," in Thought Advertising," in .Advances in Consumer Research, ed. and , eds. Harvey London and Richard Nisbett. Andrew A. Mitchell, Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Chicago. IL: Aldine, 27-43. Consumer Research, 101-106. Paivio, Allen (1969), "Mental Imagery and Associative and Larry Percy (1978). "Visual Imagining Ability as Learning and Memory," Psvchological Review. 76 (May), a Mediator of Advertising Response," in Advances in 241-263. Consumer Research, Vol. 5. ed. H. Keith Hunt. Ann (1971), Imagery and Verbal Processes, New ^'ork: Arbor, Ml: Association for Consumer Research, 621- Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. 629. (1975), "Perceptual Comparisons Through the Mind's and Larry Percy (1983). "Visual Communication in Eye," Memory and Cognition, 3 (6), 635-647. .^dvertising," in Information Processing Research in Ad- and K. Csapo (1973). "Picture Superiority in Free vertising, ed. Richard J. Harris. Hillsdale. NJ: Lawrence Recall: Imagery or Dual Coding," Cognitive Psvchologv, Erlbaum, 83-126. 5,176-206. Sarbin. Theodore R. (1972), "Imagining as Muted Role-Tak- and Dennis Foth (1970), "Imaginal and Verbal Me- ing: A Historical Linguistic Analysis," in The Function IMAGERY PROCESSING 491

and Nature of Imagery, ed. Peter Sheehan, New York: Smith, Ruth Ann, Michael J. Houston, and Terry L. Childers Academic Press, 333-354. (1984), "Verbal vs. Visual Processing Modes: An Em- Shaw, William A. (1940), "The Relation of Muscular Action pirical Test of the Cyclical Processing Hypothesis," in Potentials to Imaginal Weight Lifting," Archives of Psy- Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 11, ed. Thomas chology, 247, 50. Kinnear, Ann Arbor, MI: Association forConsumer Re- Sheehan, Peter (1967), "A Shortened Form of Betts' Ques- search, 75-80. tionnaire Upon Mental Imagery," Journal of Clinical Staats, Arthur W. and Jeffrey M. Lohr (1979), "Images, Lan- Psychology. 23, 386-389. guage, Emotions and Personality: Social 's (1972), "A Functional Analysis ofthe Role of Visual Theory," Journal of Mental Imagery, 3, 85-106. Imagery in Unexpected Recall," in The Function and Swann, William B. and Lynn C. Miller (1982), "Why Never Nature of Imagery, ed. Peter Sheehan, New York: Aca- Forgetting a Face Matters: Visual Imagery and Social demic Press. 149-174. Memory," Journal of Personality and Social Psvchologv, , R. Ashton, and K. White (1983), "Assessment of 41 (3), 475-480. Mental Imagery," in Imagery: Current Theory, Research Swasy, John L. and James M. Munch (1985), "Examining and Application, ed. Aness Sheikh, New 'i'ork: John Target Receiver Elaborations: Rhetorical Question Ef- Wiley, 189-221. fects on Source Processing and Persuasion," Journal of and (1969), "Some Variables Affecting Consumer Research, 11 (March), 877-886. the Vividness of Imagery in Recall," British Journal of Thurstone, L.L. (1938), "Primary Mental Abilities," P.'iy- Psychotogy, 60,1 \-S0. chometrika. Monographs No. 1. Shepard, Roger N. (1967), "Recognition Memory for Words. Tushup, Richard J. and Marvin Zuckerman (1977), "The Ef- Sentences and Pictures," Journal of Verbal Learning and fects of Stimulus Invariance on Daydreaming and Di- Verbal Behavior, 6, 156-163. vergent Thinking," Journal of .Mental Imagery, 2, 291- (1978), "The ,".^mer/ran Psychologist, 302. 33 (February), 125-137. Tversky. Amos and (1983), "Extensional and J. Meltzer (1971), "Mental Rotation of Three Versus Intuitive Reasoning: The Conjunction Fallacy." Dimensional Objects," Science. 171, 701-703. Psychological Review. 90. 293-315. Sherman, Steven J., R.B. Cialdini, D.F. Schwartzman, and Wallendorf. Melanie (1984). personal communication. K.D. Reynolds (1984), "Imagining Can Heighten Or Wallsten, Thomas S. and David V. Budescu (1983), "Encod- Lower the Perceived Likelihood of Contracting a Dis- ing Subjective Probabilities: A Psychological and Psy- ease," unpublished working paper, Arizona State Uni- chometric Analysis," Management Science. 29 (2), 151- versity. 173. , Richard B. Skov, Esther F. Hervitz, and Caryl B. Ha// Street Journal (1984), "Coca-Cola Turns to Pavlov Stock (1981), "The Effects of Explaining Hypothetical . . . ," January 19. p. 31. Future Events: From Possibility to Probability to Ac- Westcott. Thomas B. and Edward Rosenstock (1976), "Re- tuality and Beyond," Journal of Experimental Social liability of Two Measures of Imagery," Perceptual and Psychology 17, 142-158. .Motor Skills. 2. 245-252. -. Kim S. Zehmer, James Johnson, and Edward R. Hirt Wollman. Neil J. (1981). "The Employment of Imagery to (1983), "Social Explanation: The Role of Timing. Set Study Social Psychological Phenomena." Journat of and Recall on Subjective Likelihood Estimates," Journal .Mental Imagery, 5, 137-142. of Personality and Social Psychology. 44, 1127-1143. Wolpe. Jerome (1958), by Reciprocal Inhibi- Simon, Herbert .\. (1978), "What the Knower Knows: Al- tion, Stanford CA: Stanford University Press. ternative Strategies For Problem Solving Tasks," in Hu- Wright, Peter (1974), "The Harassed Decision Maker: Time man and Artificial , ed. F. Klix, Berlin: VEB Pressures, Distraction, and the Use of Evidence," Journal Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften, 89-100. of .Applied Psychotogy 59, 555-561. and John R. Hayes (1976), "The Understanding Pro- (1980), "Message-Evoked Thoughts: Persuasion Re- cess: Problem Isomorphs," Cognitive Psvchotogv. 8, 165- search Using Thought Verbalizations," Journal of Con- 190. sumer Research. 7 (September), 151-175. Singer. Jerome (1978). "Experimental Studies of Daydream- and Peter Rip (1980), "Product Class Advertising Ef- ing and the Stream of Thought," in The Streani of Con- fects on First-Time Buyers' Decision Strategies." Journal sciousness: Scientific Investigations into the Flow of Hu- of Consumer Research. 1 (September), 176-188. man Experience, eds. Kenneth Pope and Jerome Singer, Yalch, Richard F. and Rebecca Elmore Yalch (1984). "The New York: Plenum. Effect of Numbers on the Route to Persuasion," Journal and John Antrobus (1963), "Imaginal Process Inven- of Consumer Research, 11 (June), 522-527. tory," Perceptual and Motor Skills, Monograph Supple- Yates, Frances A. (1966), The Art of Memory. London: Rout- ment, 17(3), 187-209. ledge and Kegan Paul. and John Antrobus (1972), "Daydreaming, Imaginal Yuille, John C. and Michael J. Catchpole (1977), "The Role Processes and Personality: A Normative Study," in The of Imagery in Models of Cognition," Journal of Mental Function and Nature of Imagery, ed. Peter Sheehan, New Imagery, 1, 171-180. York: Academic Press, 174-232.