Visual Form Discrimination from Color Or Motion Cues: Functional Anatomy by Positron Emission Tomography (Regional Cerebral Blood Flow/Human Visual Cortex/Form) B

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Load more

Proc. Nati. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 91, pp. 9965-9969, October 1994 Neurobiology Visual form discrimination from color or motion cues: Functional anatomy by positron emission tomography (regional cerebral blood flow/human visual cortex/form) B. GULYAS*t, C. A. HEYWOODt, D. A. POPPLEWELL*, P. E. ROLAND*, AND A. COWEYt *Laboratory for Brain Research and Positron Emission Tomography, Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, S-171 77 Stockholm, Sweden; and tDepartment of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, South Parks Road, Oxford, OX1 3UD, United Kingdom Communicated by James M. Sprague, June 29, 1994 (receivedfor review February 8, 1994) ABSTRACT To explore the extent to which various corti- mans using various visual form-related psychophysical par- cal functional pathways are Involved In processing and ana- adigms have shown a large number of cortical regions to be lyzing different types of information that yield the same active during the different form tasks (20-24). perceptual entity, we mapped anatomical structures in the With the intention of exploring in the human visual system human brain participating in the dimination ofvisual forms (i) whether motion and color are analyzed by identical, mediated either by motion or color cues. Changes in regional overlapping, or different cortical substrata and (ii) whether cerebral blood flow were measured in 10 young male volunteers discrimination of visual form mediated by motion and color with positron emission tomography and with [15O1butanol. cues engage the same cortical regions, we measured regional During the measurements, the subjects performed four visual cerebral blood flow (rCBF), as an indicator of regional discrimination tasks (form-from-motion, motion alone, form- cerebral metabolism, with PET, using [150]butanol as tracer, from-color, and color alone discrimination). The individual in 10 healthy male volunteers while they performed a series regional cerebral blood flow images were standardized in shape of visual discrimination tasks. and size with the help of a computerized brain atlas. Subtrac- tion Images were determined and averaged across data from all subjects. The resulting images were analyzed for statistically METHODS signiicant changes between specific and reference tasks. The Subjects. Ten healthy male volunteers (age, 28.2 ± 2.7 discimiation of form by means of motion cues activated years, mean ± SD) participated. They were fully informed functional fields bilaterally in the inferior and lateral occipital about the objectives, details, and risks ofthe experiment and gyri, in the lingual, anterior iate, middle frontal and gave a written consent, in agreement with the Helsinki orbitofrontal gyri, and in the left fusiform and right inferior Declaration and the Code of Federal Regulations: Protection temporal gyri. Form discrimination by color cues resulted in of Human Subjects (25, 26). The study was approved by the activation bilaterally in the inferior temporal, lateral occipital, Ethical and Radiation Safety Committees of the Karolinska and orbitofrontal gyri, the left precuneus and intraparletal Hospital. Nine subjects were right handed and one subject sulcus, and the right precentral gyrus. The regions engaged in was left handed, as assessed the the two kinds of form discrimination did not overlap, demon- by Edinburgh handedness strating that differences in visual forms mediated by color or inventory (27). Seven subjects were emmetropes, and three motion cues are processed and analyzed by disparate networks had appropriate spectacle correction. The subjects' color of functional fields in human cerebral cortex. vision was tested before the experiment using a Velhagen chart (28). None of them had any manifest or hidden color vision or visual field deficit. Several physiological observations suggest that neuronal Four PET measurements were taken on the subjects while signals about color and motion are processed and analyzed by visual different functional pathways at early levels in the primate they performed discrimination tasks. The tasks were visual system (1-7). Although there are interactions between interleaved in different orders in different subjects. During the systems coding color and motion (8-10), they and their the scan, the subjects were comfortably placed in supine underlying pathways are regarded as initially independent position on the camera bed. With the exception of the (11, 12). Both the color and motion systems provide signals ambient noise of the scanners, the electroencephalogram that can elicit the same categories ofvisual perceptual entity, (EEG) apparatus, and the arterial pump, there was no noise such as contour or form. in the room, which was kept at 230C. The auditory environ- In monkeys, several studies show that cells sensitive to ment was not disturbed by any movements or speech by the visual form are located in cortical regions along a "ventral" personnel who were in a remote control room. The subjects occipitotemporal visual pathway and it has often been were required not to move or activate their muscles, to say claimed that the detailed analysis of visual forms occurs, in anything, or to change the respiratory rhythms after the great part, in the inferior temporal cortex (13-17). It has also infection, with the exception of pressing a response key to been shown that there are form-selective neurons in the register their responses with their right middle or index macaque inferior temporal cortex that are equally activated fingers. The visual field of the subjects, including the pro- by visual forms formed by luminance, motion, or texture cues jection screen, was isolated from the rest ofthe room by black (18). However, claims that there is a center or are centers in drapes. the brain [the "form area(s)"] exclusively specialized for the Visual Stimulation. The stimulus displays were presented perception of complex forms, such as faces, have recently on a 20-inch color monitor [Microvitec 2038 with a screen been challenged by lesion studies in monkeys (19). Further- more, positron emission tomography (PET) studies in hu- Abbreviations: PET, positron emission tomography; rCBF, regional cerebral blood flow; gCBF, global cerebral blood flow; EEG, elec- troencephalogram; MR, magnetic resonance; PaCO2, arterial partial The publication costs ofthis article were defrayed in part by page charge CO2 pressure; PaO2, arterial partial 02 pressure; CBA, computerized payment. This article must therefore be hereby marked "advertisement" brain atlas. in accordance with 18 U.S.C. §1734 solely to indicate this fact. tTo whom reprint requests should be addressed. 9965 Downloaded by guest on September 28, 2021 9966 Neurobiology: Gulyds et al. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91 (1994) resolution of 768 x 576 pixels and a viewing distance of 125 task, squares and rectangles were defined by the coherent cm (10 = 2.2 cm; 1 inch = 2.54 cm)]. The screen subtended motion of all their constituent pixels and the form discrimi- 180 x 160. For each task a horizontal row of three stimuli, nation was identical in other respects to the form-from-color separated by 30, was presented on the screen against a discrimination. stationary background composed of a yellow [CIE (Commis- Brain Scanning. A high-resolution nuclear magnetic reso- sion International d'tclairage) coordinates: X = 0.510; Y = nance (MR) scan (GE 3514 MR camera, 1.5 Tesla) was made 0.426] 10%1 random pixel pattern. The luminance of the pixels ofeach brain (both proton-enhanced and T2-weighted images: was 10.0 cd/M2. Form-from-motion and form-from-color spatial resolution, 0.5 mm; slice thickness, 4 mm; interslice discriminations were each paired with their reference tasks, distance, 6.5 mm; spin echo, TR = 2300 msec and TE = 25 motion discrimination and color discrimination, respectively. msec and 90 msec). The head fixations in the MR and PET For image analysis, the images revealed by the reference task scans were identical (29, 30) so that the corresponding MR were subtracted from those revealed by the corresponding and PET images were superimposable. The PET scanning [15 discrimination task. transaxial slices at 6.5 mm (distance)] was made by a Scan- The subject had to indicate the odd-one-out, which ap- ditronix PC2048-15B positron emission tomograph, having a peared either to the right or left of the row of stimuli, by 5-mm in-plane spatial resolution (31, 32). [150]Butanol, a pressing one of two buttons. There was no time pressure on freely diffusible flow tracer, was used to measure rCBF (33). the subject and a new trial immediately followed a response. During the experiments EEG, electrooculogram (saccadic The difficulty of the discrimination was varied from trial to eye movements of an amplitude >1 degree), arterial ra- trial. Three consecutive correct responses resulted in a more diotracer concentrations, arterial partial 02 pressure (PaO2) difficult discrimination. An easier discrimination followed a and arterial partial CO2 pressure (PaCO2) levels were con- single error. Predetermined thresholds for each discrimina- tinuously monitored. The response latencies and perfor- tion ensured that the subject's performance during PET mance levels were calculated on-line. rCBF and global ce- measurements tracked his threshold. rebral blood flow (gCBF) measurements were taken during In the color discrimination task (Fig. 1), the stimuli were the tasks. Differences in rCBF between the tasks resulting three squares (2.50 x 2.50) composed of the same random dot from differences in PaCO2 were corrected (34). Other aspects pattern as the background. The red and green guns were of the method, including the adequacy of resolution param- modulated, while maintaining isoluminance, to define the eters to the were as described or vice versa. The hue present study, (35). three squares, two red and one green, Image Analysis. The MR and PET images were transferred difference was titrated by halving or doubling the depth of into the brain atlas ofBohm and Greitz modulation of the guns, with 'y correction, after correct or computerized (CBA) For the form-from-color (36, 37) and transformed into standard size and shape.
Recommended publications
  • Toward a Common Terminology for the Gyri and Sulci of the Human Cerebral Cortex Hans Ten Donkelaar, Nathalie Tzourio-Mazoyer, Jürgen Mai

    Toward a Common Terminology for the Gyri and Sulci of the Human Cerebral Cortex Hans Ten Donkelaar, Nathalie Tzourio-Mazoyer, Jürgen Mai

    Toward a Common Terminology for the Gyri and Sulci of the Human Cerebral Cortex Hans ten Donkelaar, Nathalie Tzourio-Mazoyer, Jürgen Mai To cite this version: Hans ten Donkelaar, Nathalie Tzourio-Mazoyer, Jürgen Mai. Toward a Common Terminology for the Gyri and Sulci of the Human Cerebral Cortex. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, Frontiers, 2018, 12, pp.93. 10.3389/fnana.2018.00093. hal-01929541 HAL Id: hal-01929541 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01929541 Submitted on 21 Nov 2018 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. REVIEW published: 19 November 2018 doi: 10.3389/fnana.2018.00093 Toward a Common Terminology for the Gyri and Sulci of the Human Cerebral Cortex Hans J. ten Donkelaar 1*†, Nathalie Tzourio-Mazoyer 2† and Jürgen K. Mai 3† 1 Department of Neurology, Donders Center for Medical Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, Netherlands, 2 IMN Institut des Maladies Neurodégénératives UMR 5293, Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France, 3 Institute for Anatomy, Heinrich Heine University, Düsseldorf, Germany The gyri and sulci of the human brain were defined by pioneers such as Louis-Pierre Gratiolet and Alexander Ecker, and extensified by, among others, Dejerine (1895) and von Economo and Koskinas (1925).
  • Quantity Determination and the Distance Effect with Letters, Numbers, and Shapes: a Functional MR Imaging Study of Number Processing

    Quantity Determination and the Distance Effect with Letters, Numbers, and Shapes: a Functional MR Imaging Study of Number Processing

    AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 23:193–200, February 2003 Quantity Determination and the Distance Effect with Letters, Numbers, and Shapes: A Functional MR Imaging Study of Number Processing Robert K. Fulbright, Stephanie C. Manson, Pawel Skudlarski, Cheryl M. Lacadie, and John C. Gore BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: The ability to quantify, or to determine magnitude, is an important part of number processing, and the extent to which language and other cognitive abilities are involved with number processing is an area of interest. We compared activation patterns, reaction times, and accuracy as subjects determined stimulus magnitude by ordering letters, numbers, and shapes. A second goal was to define the brain regions involved in the distance effect (the farther apart numbers are, the faster subjects are at judging which number is larger) and whether this effect depended on stimulus type. METHODS: Functional MR images were acquired in 19 healthy subjects. The order task required the subjects to judge whether three stimuli were in order according to their position in the alphabet (letters), position in the number line (numbers), or size (shapes). In the control (identify task), subjects judged whether one of the three stimuli was a particular letter, number, or shape. Each stimulus type was divided into near trials (quantity difference of three or less) and far trials (quantity difference of at least five) to assess the distance effect. RESULTS: Subjects were less accurate and slower with letters than with numbers and shapes. A distance effect was present with shapes and numbers, as subjects ordered the near trials slower than far trials. No distance effect was detected with letters.
  • 01 05 Lateral Surface of the Brain-NOTES.Pdf

    01 05 Lateral Surface of the Brain-NOTES.Pdf

    Lateral Surface of the Brain Medical Neuroscience | Tutorial Notes Lateral Surface of the Brain 1 MAP TO NEUROSCIENCE CORE CONCEPTS NCC1. The brain is the body's most complex organ. LEARNING OBJECTIVES After study of the assigned learning materials, the student will: 1. Demonstrate the four paired lobes of the cerebral cortex and describe the boundaries of each. 2. Sketch the major features of each cerebral lobe, as seen from the lateral view, identifying major gyri and sulci that characterize each lobe. NARRATIVE by Leonard E. WHITE and Nell B. CANT Duke Institute for Brain Sciences Department of Neurobiology Duke University School of Medicine Overview When you view the lateral aspect of a human brain specimen (see Figures A3A and A102), three structures are usually visible: the cerebral hemispheres, the cerebellum, and part of the brainstem (although the brainstem is not visible in the specimen photographed in lateral view for Fig. 1 below). The spinal cord has usually been severed (but we’ll consider the spinal cord later), and the rest of the subdivisions are hidden from lateral view by the hemispheres. The diencephalon and the rest of the brainstem are visible on the medial surface of a brain that has been cut in the midsagittal plane. Parts of all of the subdivisions are also visible from the ventral surface of the whole brain. Over the next several tutorials, you will find video demonstrations (from the brain anatomy lab) and photographs (in the tutorial notes) of these brain surfaces, and sufficient detail in the narrative to appreciate the overall organization of the parts of the brain that are visible from each perspective.
  • Common and Distinct Functional Brain Networks for Intuitive and Deliberate Decision Making

    Common and Distinct Functional Brain Networks for Intuitive and Deliberate Decision Making

    brain sciences Article Common and Distinct Functional Brain Networks for Intuitive and Deliberate Decision Making Burak Erdeniz 1,* and John Done 2 1 Department of Psychology, Izmir˙ University of Economics, 35330 Izmir, Turkey 2 Department of Psychology and Sports Sciences, School of Life and Medical Sciences, University of Hertfordshire, Hatfield AL 10 9AB, UK * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +902-324-888-379 Received: 5 June 2019; Accepted: 19 July 2019; Published: 20 July 2019 Abstract: Reinforcement learning studies in rodents and primates demonstrate that goal-directed and habitual choice behaviors are mediated through different fronto-striatal systems, but the evidence is less clear in humans. In this study, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data were collected whilst participants (n = 20) performed a conditional associative learning task in which blocks of novel conditional stimuli (CS) required a deliberate choice, and blocks of familiar CS required an intuitive choice. Using standard subtraction analysis for fMRI event-related designs, activation shifted from the dorso-fronto-parietal network, which involves dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) for deliberate choice of novel CS, to ventro-medial frontal (VMPFC) and anterior cingulate cortex for intuitive choice of familiar CS. Supporting this finding, psycho-physiological interaction (PPI) analysis, using the peak active areas within the PFC for novel and familiar CS as seed regions, showed functional coupling between caudate and DLPFC when processing novel CS and VMPFC when processing familiar CS. These findings demonstrate separable systems for deliberate and intuitive processing, which is in keeping with rodent and primate reinforcement learning studies, although in humans they operate in a dynamic, possibly synergistic, manner particularly at the level of the striatum.
  • Online Tables (PDF)

    Online Tables (PDF)

    On-line Table 1: Description of cortical and sulcation abnormalities in 40 patients Patient Description 1 Bilateral schizencephaly, extensive fcd, abnormal Sylvian fissures 2 Abnormal R intraparietal-occipital sulcus and open sylvian fissures, sylvian cortex and R occipital cortex 3 Unusual sulcation R postcingulate and transmantle bands to Sylvian fissures 4 Bilateral peri-Sylvian pmg, abnormal Sylvian fissures, thick and blurred cortex 5 Dysplastic L cingulate adjacent to transmantle dysplasia 6 Transmantle band left frontal and above right caudate 7 L frontal transmantle band 8 Abnormal Sylvian fissures, L Ͼ R 9 R posterior quadrantic dysplasia, abnormal posterior sulcation and R Sylvian fissure, linear band 10 Abnormal L Sylvian fissure 11 Linear bands from posterior atrium to occipital white matter, unusual stellate sulcal configuration joining R intraparietal sulcus 12 Short Sylvian fissures, absent L intra-parieto-occipital fissure and simplified gyral pattern, areas of increased cortical thickness 13 Unusual sulcation R postcentral–thick but only 3 years of age terminal zones 14 Abnormal L Sylvian fissure, abnormal sulcus from L frontal lobe joins it with stellate shape 15 Dysplastic cingulate gyrus R abnormal L calcarine with branching, N thickness, still young 16 Abnormal Sylvian fissures bilaterally, elongated with terminal branches; difficult to assess thickness because not myelinated yet 17 Abnormal L Sylvian fissure 18 Thick gray linear transmantle bands from nodules to adjacent Sylvian cortex, thickened but no pmg 19 Unusual sulcation
  • (OCD) Using Treatment-Induced Neuroimaging Changes

    (OCD) Using Treatment-Induced Neuroimaging Changes

    Neurosurgery J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp-2020-324478 on 27 April 2021. Downloaded from Review Defining functional brain networks underlying obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) using treatment- induced neuroimaging changes: a systematic review of the literature Kelly R. Bijanki ,1 Yagna J. Pathak,2 Ricardo A. Najera,1 Eric A. Storch,3 Wayne K Goodman,3 H. Blair Simpson,4 Sameer A. Sheth1 ► Additional material is ABSTRACT Aberrant signalling in cortico- striato- thalamo- published online only. To view Approximately 2%–3% of the population suffers from cortical (CSTC) circuits is considered to be an please visit the journal online obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). Several brain important pathological mechanism underlying (http:// dx. doi. org/ 10. 1136/ [S1- S3] jnnp- 2020- 324478). regions have been implicated in the pathophysiology OCD. These circuits are composed of glutama- of OCD, but their various contributions remain unclear. tergic and GABAergic projections that connect fron- 1 Department of Neurosurgery, We examined changes in structural and functional tocortical and subcortical brain areas.[S4- S9] Within Baylor College of Medicine, the CSTC loop, the direct pathway from striatum Houston, Texas, USA neuroimaging before and after a variety of therapeutic 2Department of Neurological interventions as an index into identifying the underlying to internal pallidum to thalamus and back to cortex Surgery, Columbia University networks involved. We identified 64 studies from 1990 exerts a net excitatory effect, and the indirect Medical Center, New York, New to 2020 comparing pretreatment and post-treatment pathway, which additionally includes the external York, USA 3 imaging of patients with OCD, including metabolic pallidum and subthalamic nucleus, produces a Menninger Department of net inhibitory effect.[S4] A lack of balance in these Psychiatry and Behavioral and perfusion, neurochemical, structural, functional Sciences, Baylor College of and connectivity-based modalities.
  • Brain Networks for Confidence Weighting and Hierarchical

    Brain Networks for Confidence Weighting and Hierarchical

    Brain networks for confidence weighting and PNAS PLUS hierarchical inference during probabilistic learning Florent Meyniela,1 and Stanislas Dehaenea,b,1 aCognitive Neuroimaging Unit, NeuroSpin Center, Institute of Life Sciences Frédéric Joliot, Fundemental Research Division, Commissariat à l’Énergie Atomique et aux Énergies Alternatives, INSERM, Université Paris–Sud, Université Paris–Saclay, 91191 Gif/Yvette, France; and bChair of Experimental Cognitive Psychology, Collège de France, 75005 Paris, France Contributed by Stanislas Dehaene, March 20, 2017 (sent for review September 23, 2016; reviewed by Stephen M. Fleming and Charles R. Gallistel) Learning is difficult when the world fluctuates randomly and and normative solution to this problem requires weighting each ceaselessly. Classical learning algorithms, such as the delta rule with source of information according to its reliability (3–12). According constant learning rate, are not optimal. Mathematically, the optimal to this Bayes-optimal solution, any discrepancy between a new ob- learning rule requires weighting prior knowledge and incoming servation and a learned estimate should lead to an update of this evidence according to their respective reliabilities. This “confidence internal estimate, but the size of this update should decrease as the weighting” implies the maintenance of an accurate estimate of the prior confidence in this internal estimate increases. Furthermore, reliability of what has been learned. Here, using fMRI and an ideal- this prior confidence should depend on two factors: the precision of observer analysis, we demonstrate that the brain’s learning algorithm the current internal estimate and a discounting factor that takes into relies on confidence weighting. While in the fMRI scanner, human account the possibility that a change occurred.
  • Neural Arbitration Between Social and Individual Learning Systems

    Neural Arbitration Between Social and Individual Learning Systems

    RESEARCH ARTICLE Neural arbitration between social and individual learning systems Andreea Oliviana Diaconescu1,2,3,4†*, Madeline Stecy1,2,5†, Lars Kasper1,2,6, Christopher J Burke2, Zoltan Nagy2, Christoph Mathys1,7,8, Philippe N Tobler2 1Translational Neuromodeling Unit, Institute for Biomedical Engineering, University of Zurich & ETH Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland; 2Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research, Department of Economics, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland; 3University of Basel, Department of Psychiatry (UPK), Basel, Switzerland; 4Krembil Centre for Neuroinformatics, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH), University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada; 5Rutgers Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, New Brunswick, United States; 6Institute for Biomedical Engineering, MRI Technology Group, ETH Zu¨ rich & University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland; 7Interacting Minds Centre, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark; 8Scuola Internazionale Superiore di Studi Avanzati (SISSA), Trieste, Italy Abstract Decision making requires integrating knowledge gathered from personal experiences with advice from others. The neural underpinnings of the process of arbitrating between information sources has not been fully elucidated. In this study, we formalized arbitration as the relative precision of predictions, afforded by each learning system, using hierarchical Bayesian modeling. In a probabilistic learning task, participants predicted the outcome of a lottery using recommendations from a more informed advisor and/or self-sampled outcomes.
  • Parietal Dysgraphia: Characterization of Abnormal Writing Stroke Sequences, Character Formation and Character Recall

    Parietal Dysgraphia: Characterization of Abnormal Writing Stroke Sequences, Character Formation and Character Recall

    Behavioural Neurology 18 (2007) 99–114 99 IOS Press Parietal dysgraphia: Characterization of abnormal writing stroke sequences, character formation and character recall Yasuhisa Sakuraia,b,∗, Yoshinobu Onumaa, Gaku Nakazawaa, Yoshikazu Ugawab, Toshimitsu Momosec, Shoji Tsujib and Toru Mannena aDepartment of Neurology, Mitsui Memorial Hospital, Tokyo, Japan bDepartment of Neurology, Graduate School of Medicine, University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan cDepartment of Radiology, Graduate School of Medicine, University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan Abstract. Objective: To characterize various dysgraphic symptoms in parietal agraphia. Method: We examined the writing impairments of four dysgraphia patients from parietal lobe lesions using a special writing test with 100 character kanji (Japanese morphograms) and their kana (Japanese phonetic writing) transcriptions, and related the test performance to a lesion site. Results: Patients 1 and 2 had postcentral gyrus lesions and showed character distortion and tactile agnosia, with patient 1 also having limb apraxia. Patients 3 and 4 had superior parietal lobule lesions and features characteristic of apraxic agraphia (grapheme deformity and a writing stroke sequence disorder) and character imagery deficits (impaired character recall). Agraphia with impaired character recall and abnormal grapheme formation were more pronounced in patient 4, in whom the lesion extended to the inferior parietal, superior occipital and precuneus gyri. Conclusion: The present findings and a review of the literature suggest that: (i) a postcentral gyrus lesion can yield graphemic distortion (somesthetic dysgraphia), (ii) abnormal grapheme formation and impaired character recall are associated with lesions surrounding the intraparietal sulcus, the symptom being more severe with the involvement of the inferior parietal, superior occipital and precuneus gyri, (iii) disordered writing stroke sequences are caused by a damaged anterior intraparietal area.
  • True and False Recognition Memories of Odors Induce Distinct Neural Signatures

    True and False Recognition Memories of Odors Induce Distinct Neural Signatures

    ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 21 July 2011 HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00065 True and false recognition memories of odors induce distinct neural signatures Jean-Pierre Royet1*, Léri Morin-Audebrand 2,3, Barbara Cerf-Ducastel 4, Lori Haase 4, Sylvie Issanchou 2, Claire Murphy 4, Pierre Fonlupt 5, Claire Sulmont-Rossé 2 and Jane Plailly1 1 INSERM, U1028, UMR5292 CNRS, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Université Lyon, Lyon, France 2 Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l’Alimentation, UMR6265 CNRS, UMR1324 INRA, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France 3 Institute of Life Technologies, University of Applied Sciences Valais, Sion, Switzerland 4 Lifespan Human Senses Laboratory, Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA 5 Dynamique Cérébrale et Cognition, INSERM, U280, University Lyon1, Lyon, France Edited by: Neural bases of human olfactory memory are poorly understood. Very few studies have Hans-Jochen Heinze, University of examined neural substrates associated with correct odor recognition, and none has tackled Magdeburg, Germany neural networks associated with incorrect odor recognition. We investigated the neural basis Reviewed by: Leslie J. Carver, University of California, of task performance during a yes–no odor recognition memory paradigm in young and elderly USA subjects using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. We explored four response Mercedes Atienza, University Pablo de categories: correct (Hit) and incorrect false alarm (FA) recognition, as well as correct (CR) and Olavide, Spain incorrect (Miss) rejection, and we characterized corresponding brain responses using multivariate *Correspondence: analysis and linear regression analysis. We hypothesized that areas of the medial temporal lobe Jean-Pierre Royet, INSERM, U1028, UMR5292 CNRS, Lyon Neuroscience were differentially involved depending on the accuracy of odor recognition.
  • Shape-Specific Activation of Occipital Cortex in an Early Blind

    Shape-Specific Activation of Occipital Cortex in an Early Blind

    Neuropsychologia ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] 1 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect 2 3 4 Neuropsychologia 5 6 journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia 7 8 9 10 11 12 Shape-specific activation of occipital cortex in an early blind 13 echolocation expert 14 a,n b c d c 15 Q1 Stephen R. Arnott , Lore Thaler , Jennifer Milne , Daniel Kish , Melvyn A. Goodale 16 a 17 The Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest, 3560 Bathurst Street, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, M6A 2E1 b 18 Department of Psychology, Durham University, Durham, UK c Department of Psychology, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada, N6A 5C2 19 d World Access for the Blind, Encino, California 91316, USA 20 21 22 article info abstract 23 24 Article history: We have previously reported that an early-blind echolocating individual (EB) showed robust occipital 25 Received 22 June 2012 activation when he identified distant, silent objects based on echoes from his tongue clicks (Thaler, 26 Received in revised form Arnott, & Goodale, 2011). In the present study we investigated the extent to which echolocation 27 21 January 2013 activation in EB’s occipital cortex reflected general echolocation processing per se versus feature- Accepted 27 January 2013 28 specific processing. In the first experiment, echolocation audio sessions were captured with in-ear 29 microphones in an anechoic chamber or hallway alcove as EB produced tongue clicks in front of a 30 Keywords: concave or flat object covered in aluminum foil or a cotton towel. All eight echolocation sessions Auditory (2 shapes  2 surface materials  2 environments) were then randomly presented to him during a sparse- 31 Blind temporal scanning fMRI session.
  • Functional Anatomy of the Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus: from Historical Reports to Current Hypotheses Guillaume Herbet, Ilyess Zemmoura, Hugues Duffau

    Functional Anatomy of the Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus: from Historical Reports to Current Hypotheses Guillaume Herbet, Ilyess Zemmoura, Hugues Duffau

    Functional Anatomy of the Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus: From Historical Reports to Current Hypotheses Guillaume Herbet, Ilyess Zemmoura, Hugues Duffau To cite this version: Guillaume Herbet, Ilyess Zemmoura, Hugues Duffau. Functional Anatomy of the Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus: From Historical Reports to Current Hypotheses. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, Frontiers, 2018, 12, pp.77. 10.3389/fnana.2018.00077. hal-02313966 HAL Id: hal-02313966 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02313966 Submitted on 7 Jun 2021 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution| 4.0 International License fnana-12-00077 September 17, 2018 Time: 10:22 # 1 REVIEW published: 19 September 2018 doi: 10.3389/fnana.2018.00077 Functional Anatomy of the Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus: From Historical Reports to Current Hypotheses Guillaume Herbet1,2,3*, Ilyess Zemmoura4,5 and Hugues Duffau1,2,3 1 Department of Neurosurgery, Gui de Chauliac Hospital, Montpellier University Medical Center, Montpellier, France, 2 INSERM-1051, Team 4, Saint-Eloi Hospital, Institute for Neurosciences of Montpellier, Montpellier, France, 3 University of Montpellier, Montpellier, France, 4 Department of Neurosurgery, Tours University Medical Center, Tours, France, 5 UMR 1253, iBrain, INSERM, University of Tours, Tours, France The inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF) is a long-range, associative white matter pathway that connects the occipital and temporal-occipital areas of the brain to the anterior temporal areas.