The ''Goldilocks Factor'' in Food Webs

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The ''Goldilocks Factor'' in Food Webs COMMENTARY The ‘‘Goldilocks factor’’ in food webs Eric L. Berlow*†‡, Ulrich Brose†§, and Neo D. Martinez† *University of California, Merced, Sierra Nevada Research Institute, Yosemite National Park, CA 95389; †Pacific Ecoinformatics and Computational Ecology Laboratory, 1604 McGee Avenue, Berkeley, CA 94703; and §Department of Biology, Darmstadt University of Technology, Schnittspahnstrasse 10, 64287 Darmstadt, Germany n a well known children’s tale, the harvesting, of one species might cause little girl, Goldilocks, nearly gets dramatic changes in the abundances of herself eaten by bears by boldly other species. Recent work (16–20) has choosing between the three bears’ shown how the body sizes of predators Iporridge bowls: one too hot and another and their prey can determine feeding rates too cold. In this issue of PNAS, the article and biomass flows in food webs. Although ‘‘Size, foraging, and food web structure,’’ large flows of biomass from prey to pred- by Petchey et al. (1), tells a similar story ator may not indicate strong predator con- of food webs in which predator species trol of prey populations (21), such flows choose between prey species that are too can still drive population dynamics (22). large or too small. In doing so, the au- Thus, the coupling of foraging theory with thors show how an easily observed species allometric considerations of metabolic trait—body size—can address long- Fig. 1. Cross-scale integration from individuals to theory may unite the structure of food standing ecological questions about who ecosystems. Petchey et al. (1) model organisms’ webs with biomass flows to predict spe- eats whom in complex natural communi- size-based foraging and integrate it with the net- cies’ population dynamics, which in turn work structure of food webs (red arrow). This work ties. Although previous models explain can elucidate the strengths of dynamic contributes to a broad research program that uses coupling between species (8). In this case, well the general network properties of body size to integrate different levels of trophic food webs, Petchey et al. go further by organization and interactions (blue arrows). species’ influence on each other and on explaining how the particular links found the outcomes of species loss are seen as between species within specific food webs emergent properties of population dynam- can result from optimal foraging of preda- The specific idea that the network ics, driven by foraging behavior and tors on prey that are neither too large nor structure of food webs may emerge from biomass flows that are constrained by spe- too small. Like Goldilocks, optimal preda- optimally foraging individuals is intuitively cies’ body size (15). tors choose food that is ‘‘just right.’’ appealing but technically challenging. Body size also allows integration of the In choosing their food, predators and Petchey et al.’s (1) two mathematical for- overall dynamic persistence of food webs other consumers create food webs—the aging functions require that three or four with the diversity and complexity of food feeding networks among species that sus- parameters be fitted to each food web. web structure (10). The mathematical im- tain the life support systems on earth. Further work is needed to address the probability of large, complex networks Ecologists continue to discover fascinating unexplained variability in these parame- persisting dynamically was recognized regularities in the structure of this interde- ters (up to 26 orders of magnitude) and early on (4). More recent theory finds pendent complexity (2, 3). Elucidating the to reduce the analytical complexity of for- that body size ratios between predators mechanisms responsible for these regulari- mulating and fitting them. Nonetheless, an and their prey occupy a Goldilocks-type ties is a fundamental step in tackling long- important contribution is their linking of ‘‘sweet spot’’ that enables diversity to standing questions in ecology that range body size to prey-handling times within enhance both system-level (9, 16) and from ‘‘What confers stability to complex the foraging functions, both of which fac- subsystem-level (22) stability. These stabi- lizing predator–prey body mass ratios are ecosystems?’’ (4) to ‘‘How does species tors set upper bounds to predators’ prey remarkably consistent with those found in loss alter the abundance of other spe- size. The more successful of the two func- the most comprehensive empirical data cies?’’ (5). Surprisingly simple rule-based tions also sets a lower limit to the body available (23). Buffering by larger organ- models successfully capture the overall sizes of prey species. This concept of con- isms’ low metabolic rate (9, 22) and structure of real food webs (2, 6, 7) and sumers feeding on contiguous ranges of higher mobility among variably productive enable further theoretical exploration by prey body masses within boundaries is patches (24) appear to underlie such sta- stochastically generating webs that mimic deeply rooted in foraging theory (12, 13) bility. Petchey et al. (1) provide more and is highly consistent with observed the overall structure of real webs (6, mechanistic insight into such allometric food web structure (14). However, this 8–10). However, such models lack mecha- integration of link-level biomass flows research has lacked the mechanistic bridge nistic explanations for their input parame- with population dynamics and system-level between foraging theory and food web ters and poorly predict the actual links in stability by showing that these parametric real food webs. Petchey et al. (1) have topology. Petchey et al. provide this bridge sweet spots are consistent with optimal synthesized the allometry of body size by integrating both the foraging conse- foraging. with optimal foraging theory to address quences of body size and the energy con- Leaving dynamics aside, the connection both the first limitation (11), and now the tent of individual prey. between body size and comparative analy- second, by assuming that predators are Between the narrow scale of an individ- more efficient at consuming prey that are ual’s foraging and the broad scale of smaller than themselves, but not too community patterns lie the strengths of Author contributions: E.L.B., U.B., and N.D.M. wrote the small. This ‘‘Goldilocks’’ model of food interactions between populations (15)— paper. choice successfully predicts up to 65% of another area of trophic ecology being in- The authors declare no conflict of interest. the feeding links in real food webs, and in tegrated by the use of body size (Fig. 1). See companion article on page 4191. doing so helps to solve the notoriously Understanding interaction strength in- ‡To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: difficult problem of integrating ecology volves the critical ecological challenge of [email protected]. from individuals to ecosystems (Fig. 1). predicting when the extinction, or over- © 2008 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA www.pnas.org͞cgi͞doi͞10.1073͞pnas.0800967105 PNAS ͉ March 18, 2008 ͉ vol. 105 ͉ no. 11 ͉ 4079–4080 Downloaded by guest on October 1, 2021 ses of the network structure of ecosystems These failures offer important insights ganism is to subdue and digest. Still, eco- also benefits from the mechanistic detail into the limits of body size alone as an logical interactions involve much more that Petchey et al. (1) provide. Seminal explanatory panacea. Predaceous and than just feeding (e.g., mutualisms, ecosys- early work found that species’ diets within aquatic herbivorous links were, as tem engineering) (38), and feeding in- a food web can form an unbroken seg- expected, much better predicted than par- volves more than just prey body size. But ment, or interval, along a fixed sequence asitic, parasitoid, and pathogenic interac- body size and feeding links seem to be of all species within the food web (25). tions. Similarly, terrestrial herbivorous the ‘‘low-hanging fruit’’ of ecological com- Body size helps explain the hierarchy in interactions were not as well predicted as plexity research. Both are universal and one of the earliest structural food web aquatic ones, and the three nondesert ter- relatively easy to measure for many organ- models, which proposed that larger or- restrial webs were fit rather poorly. All of isms. All species are made up of organ- dered species can eat smaller ordered spe- this suggests that some foraging modes fit isms that have mass, and no species we cies, but not vice versa (26). These two size-based optimal foraging models better know of escapes the food web. As such, ideas were then synthesized by the ‘‘niche than others. Body size may be most im- food webs form an essential core of criti- model’’ that assigned consumers unbroken portant to gape-limited or visual predators cal interdependence created by the class feeding ranges on a niche dimension that and perhaps unimportant to insect herbi- of species interactions for which we tend tends to be located below the consumer vores that choose plants on the basis on to have the most comprehensive data (27). Although this model has been shown leaf chemistry or plant defenses. Similarly, (35). Similarly, body size elegantly links to successfully predict overall food web energy maximization may be balanced by energy content, feeding behavior, and structure (6, 7, 28), Petchey et al. provide avoiding the risk of being consumed (32) metabolic needs. Does this mean that we a testable mechanistic interpretation of or by stoichiometric preferences for par- have chosen to search for our lost coins the niche axis in terms of body mass [see ticular nutrients (33). To better interpret under the lamppost simply because that is also Stouffer et al. (14)]. Such mechanisms such success and failure, it would help to are also relevant to empirical analyses of know whether a random model with fewer where the light is? We don’t think so.
Recommended publications
  • Foraging Modes of Carnivorous Plants Aaron M
    Israel Journal of Ecology & Evolution, 2020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22244662-20191066 Foraging modes of carnivorous plants Aaron M. Ellison* Harvard Forest, Harvard University, 324 North Main Street, Petersham, Massachusetts, 01366, USA Abstract Carnivorous plants are pure sit-and-wait predators: they remain rooted to a single location and depend on the abundance and movement of their prey to obtain nutrients required for growth and reproduction. Yet carnivorous plants exhibit phenotypically plastic responses to prey availability that parallel those of non-carnivorous plants to changes in light levels or soil-nutrient concentrations. The latter have been considered to be foraging behaviors, but the former have not. Here, I review aspects of foraging theory that can be profitably applied to carnivorous plants considered as sit-and-wait predators. A discussion of different strategies by which carnivorous plants attract, capture, kill, and digest prey, and subsequently acquire nutrients from them suggests that optimal foraging theory can be applied to carnivorous plants as easily as it has been applied to animals. Carnivorous plants can vary their production, placement, and types of traps; switch between capturing nutrients from leaf-derived traps and roots; temporarily activate traps in response to external cues; or cease trap production altogether. Future research on foraging strategies by carnivorous plants will yield new insights into the physiology and ecology of what Darwin called “the most wonderful plants in the world”. At the same time, inclusion of carnivorous plants into models of animal foraging behavior could lead to the development of a more general and taxonomically inclusive foraging theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Foraging : an Ecology Model of Consumer Behaviour?
    This is a repository copy of Foraging : an ecology model of consumer behaviour?. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/122432/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Wells, VK orcid.org/0000-0003-1253-7297 (2012) Foraging : an ecology model of consumer behaviour? Marketing Theory. pp. 117-136. ISSN 1741-301X https://doi.org/10.1177/1470593112441562 Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record for the item. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ Foraging: an ecology model of consumer behavior? Victoria.K.Wells* Durham Business School, UK First Submission June 2010 Revision One December 2010 Revision Two August 2011 Accepted for Publication: September 2011 * Address for correspondence: Dr Victoria Wells (née James), Durham Business School, Durham University, Mill Hill Lane, Durham, DH1 3LB & Queen’s Campus, University Boulevard, Thornaby, Stockton-on-Tees, TS17 6BH, Telephone: +44 (0)191 334 0472, E-mail: [email protected] I would like to thank Tony Ellson for his guidance and Gordon Foxall for his helpful comments during the development and writing of this paper.
    [Show full text]
  • Water Stress Inhibits Plant Photosynthesis by Decreasing
    letters to nature data on a variety of animals. The original foraging data on bees Acknowledgements (Fig. 3a) were collected by recording the landing sites of individual We thank V. Afanasyev, N. Dokholyan, I. P. Fittipaldi, P.Ch. Ivanov, U. Laino, L. S. Lucena, bees15. We find that, when the nectar concentration is low, the flight- E. G. Murphy, P. A. Prince, M. F. Shlesinger, B. D. Stosic and P.Trunfio for discussions, and length distribution decays as in equation (1), with m < 2 (Fig. 3b). CNPq, NSF and NIH for financial support. (The exponent m is not affected by short flights.) We also find the Correspondence should be addressed to G.M.V. (e-mail: gandhi@fis.ufal.br). value m < 2 for the foraging-time distribution of the wandering albatross6 (Fig. 3b, inset) and deer (Fig. 3c, d) in both wild and fenced areas16 (foraging times and lengths are assumed to be ................................................................. proportional). The value 2 # m # 2:5 found for amoebas4 is also consistent with the predicted Le´vy-flight motion. Water stress inhibits plant The above theoretical arguments and numerical simulations suggest that m < 2 is the optimal value for a search in any dimen- photosynthesis by decreasing sion. This is analogous to the behaviour of random walks whose mean-square displacement is proportional to the number of steps in coupling factor and ATP any dimension17. Furthermore, equations (4) and (5) describe the W. Tezara*, V. J. Mitchell†, S. D. Driscoll† & D. W. Lawlor† correct scaling properties even in the presence of short-range correlations in the directions and lengths of the flights.
    [Show full text]
  • Predation Risk and Feeding Site Preferences in Winter Foraging Birds
    Eastern Illinois University The Keep Masters Theses Student Theses & Publications 1992 Predation Risk and Feeding Site Preferences in Winter Foraging Birds Yen-min Kuo This research is a product of the graduate program in Zoology at Eastern Illinois University. Find out more about the program. Recommended Citation Kuo, Yen-min, "Predation Risk and Feeding Site Preferences in Winter Foraging Birds" (1992). Masters Theses. 2199. https://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/2199 This is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Theses & Publications at The Keep. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses by an authorized administrator of The Keep. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THESIS REPRODUCTION CERTIFICATE TO: Graduate Degree Car1dichi.tes who have written formal theses. SUBJECT: Permission to reproduce theses. The University Library is receiving a number of requests from other institutions asking permission to reproduce dissertations for inclusion in their library holdings. Although no copyright laws are involved~ we feel that professional courtesy demands that permission be obtained from the author before we ~llow theses to be copied. Please sign one of the following statements: Booth Library o! Eastern Illinois University has my permission to lend my thesis tq a reputable college or university for the purpose of copying it for inclul!lion in that institution's library or research holdings. Date I respectfully request Booth Library of Eastern Illinois University not allow my thesis be reproduced because ~~~~~~-~~~~~~~ Date Author m Predaton risk and feeding site preferences in winter foraging oirds (Tllll) BY Yen-min Kuo THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF Master of Science in Zoology IN TH£ GRADUATE SCHOOL, EASTERN ILLINOIS UNIVfRSITY CHARLESTON, ILLINOIS 1992 YEAR I HEREBY R£COMMEND THIS THESIS BE ACCEPTED AS FULFILLING THIS PART or THE GRADUATE DEGREE CITED ABOVE 2f vov.
    [Show full text]
  • Photosynthetic Pigments Estimate Diet Quality in Forage and Feces of Elk (Cervus Elaphus) D
    51 ARTICLE Photosynthetic pigments estimate diet quality in forage and feces of elk (Cervus elaphus) D. Christianson and S. Creel Abstract: Understanding the nutritional dynamics of herbivores living in highly seasonal landscapes remains a central chal- lenge in foraging ecology with few tools available for describing variation in selection for dormant versus growing vegetation. Here, we tested whether the concentrations of photosynthetic pigments (chlorophylls and carotenoids) in forage and feces of elk (Cervus elaphus L., 1785) were correlated with other commonly used indices of forage quality (digestibility, energy content, neutral detergent fiber (NDF), and nitrogen content) and diet quality (fecal nitrogen, fecal NDF, and botanical composition of the diet). Photosynthetic pigment concentrations were strongly correlated with nitrogen content, gross energy, digestibility, and NDF of elk forages, particularly in spring. Winter and spring variation in fecal pigments and fecal nitrogen was explained with nearly identical linear models estimating the effects of season, sex, and day-of-spring, although models of fecal pigments were 2 consistently a better fit (r adjusted = 0.379–0.904) and estimated effect sizes more precisely than models of fecal nitrogen 2 (r adjusted = 0.247–0.773). A positive correlation with forage digestibility, nutrient concentration, and (or) botanical composition of the diet implies fecal photosynthetic pigments may be a sensitive and informative descriptor of diet selection in free-ranging herbivores. Key words: carotenoid, chlorophyll, diet selection, digestibility, energy, foraging behavior, nitrogen, phenology, photosynthesis, primary productivity. Résumé : La compréhension de la dynamique nutritive des herbivores vivant dans des paysages très saisonniers demeure un des défis centraux de l’écologie de l’alimentation, peu d’outils étant disponibles pour décrire les variations du choix de plantes dormantes ou en croissances.
    [Show full text]
  • Interpreting Temporal Variation in Omnivore Foraging Ecology Via
    Functional Ecology 2009 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2009.01553.x InterpretingBlackwell Publishing Ltd temporal variation in omnivore foraging ecology via stable isotope modelling Carolyn M. Kurle*† Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Department, University of California, 100 Shaffer Road, Santa Cruz, CA 95060, USA Summary 1. The use of stable carbon (C) and nitrogen (N) isotopes (δ15N and δ13C, respectively) to delineate trophic patterns in wild animals is common in ecology. Their utility as a tool for interpreting temporal change in diet due to seasonality, migration, climate change or species invasion depends upon an understanding of the rates at which stable isotopes incorporate from diet into animal tissues. To best determine the foraging habits of invasive rats on island ecosystems and to illuminate the interpretation of wild omnivore diets in general, I investigated isotope incorporation rates of C and N in fur, liver, kidney, muscle, serum and red blood cells (RBC) from captive rats raised on a diet with low δ15N and δ13C values and switched to a diet with higher δ15N and δ13C values. 2. I used the reaction progress variable method (RPVM), a linear fitting procedure, to estimate whether a single or multiple compartment model best described isotope turnover in each tissue. Small sample Akaike Information criterion (AICc) model comparison analysis indicated that 1 compartment nonlinear models best described isotope incorporation rates for liver, RBC, muscle, and fur, whereas 2 compartment nonlinear models were best for serum and kidney. 3. I compared isotope incorporation rates using the RPVM versus nonlinear models. There were no differences in estimated isotope retention times between the model types for serum and kidney (except for N turnover in kidney from females).
    [Show full text]
  • Foraging Ecology of a Winter Bird Community in Southeastern Georgia
    Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Fall 2018 Foraging Ecology of a Winter Bird Community in Southeastern Georgia Rachel E. Mowbray Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.georgiasouthern.edu/etd Part of the Behavior and Ethology Commons, and the Population Biology Commons Recommended Citation Mowbray, Rachel E., "Foraging Ecology of a Winter Bird Community in Southeastern Georgia" (2018). Electronic Theses and Dissertations. 1861. https://digitalcommons.georgiasouthern.edu/etd/1861 This thesis (open access) is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of at Digital Commons@Georgia Southern. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons@Georgia Southern. For more information, please contact [email protected]. FORAGING ECOLOGY OF A WINTER BIRD COMMUNITY IN SOUTHEASTERN GEORGIA by RACHEL MOWBRAY (Under the Direction of C. Ray Chandler) ABSTRACT Classical views on community structure emphasized deterministic processes and the importance of competition in shaping communities. However, the processes responsible for shaping avian communities remain controversial. Attempts to understand distributions and abundances of species are complicated by the fact that birds are highly mobile. Many species migrate biannually between summer breeding grounds and wintering grounds. The goal of this study was to test four hypotheses that attempt to explain how migratory species integrate into resident assemblages of birds (Empty-Niche Hypothesis, Competitive-Exclusion Hypothesis, Niche-Partitioning Hypothesis, and Generalist-Migrant Hypothesis). I collected data on birds foraging during the winter of 2017-2018 in Magnolia Springs State Park, Jenkins County, Georgia, U.S.A.
    [Show full text]
  • Nocturnal and Diurnal Foraging Behaviour of Brown Bears (Ursus Arctos) on a Salmon Stream in Coastal British Columbia
    Color profile: Disabled Composite Default screen 1317 Nocturnal and diurnal foraging behaviour of brown bears (Ursus arctos) on a salmon stream in coastal British Columbia D.R. Klinka and T.E. Reimchen Abstract: Brown bears (Ursus arctos) have been reported to be primarily diurnal throughout their range in North America. Recent studies of black bears during salmon migration indicate high levels of nocturnal foraging with high capture efficiencies during darkness. We investigated the extent of nocturnal foraging by brown bears during a salmon spawning migration at Knight Inlet in coastal British Columbia, using night-vision goggles. Adult brown bears were observed foraging equally during daylight and darkness, while adult females with cubs, as well as subadults, were most prevalent during daylight and twilight but uncommon during darkness. We observed a marginal trend of increased cap- ture efficiency with reduced light levels (day, 20%; night, 36%) that was probably due to the reduced evasive behaviour of the salmon. Capture rates averaged 3.9 fish/h and differed among photic regimes (daylight, 2.1 fish/h; twilight, 4.3 fish/h; darkness, 8.3 fish/h). These results indicate that brown bears are highly successful during nocturnal foraging and exploit this period during spawning migration to maximize their consumption rates of an ephemeral resource. Résumé : Les ours bruns (Ursus arctos) sont généralement reconnus comme des animaux à alimentation surtout diurne dans toute leur aire de répartition. Les résultats d’études récentes sur les ours noirs durant la migration des saumons indiquent qu’ils font une quête de nourriture intense pendant la nuit et que l’efficacité de leurs captures est élevée à l’obscurité.
    [Show full text]
  • Optimal Foraging Theory and the Psychology of Learning Alan Kamil
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Avian Cognition Papers Center for Avian Cognition 1983 Optimal Foraging Theory and the Psychology of Learning Alan Kamil Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/biosciaviancog Part of the Animal Studies Commons, Behavior and Ethology Commons, Cognition and Perception Commons, Forest Sciences Commons, Ornithology Commons, and the Other Psychology Commons This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Center for Avian Cognition at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Avian Cognition Papers by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. digitalcommons.unl.edu Optimal Foraging Theory and the Psychology of Learning Alan C. Kamil Departments of Psychology and Zoology University of Massachusetts Amherst, Massachusetts 01003 Synopsis The development of optimization theory has made important contri- butions to the study of animal behavior. But the optimization approach needs to be integrated with other methods of ethology and psychology. For example, the ability to learn is an important component of efficient foraging behavior in many species, and the psychology of animal learn- ing could contribute substantially to testing and extending the predic- tions of optimal foraging theory. Introduction When the animal behavior literature of the 1960s is compared with the literature of today, it is apparent that a revolution has taken place. Many of the phenomena of behavioral ecology and sociobiology that are taken for granted today, that are presented in undergraduate text- books (e.g., Krebs and Davies, 1981), would have seemed implausible just 15 to 20 years ago.
    [Show full text]
  • Seabird Foraging Ecology
    2636 SEABIRD FORAGING ECOLOGY Ballance L.T., D.G. Ainley, and G.L. Hunt, Jr. 2001. Seabird Foraging Ecology. Pages 2636-2644 in: J.H. Steele, S.A. Thorpe and K.K. Turekian (eds.) Encyclopedia of Ocean Sciences, vol. 5. Academic Press, London. SEABIRD FORAGING ECOLOGY L. T. Balance, NOAA-NMFS, La Jolla, CA, USA Introduction D. G. Ainley, H.T. Harvey & Associates, San Jose, CA, USA Though bound to the land for reproduction, most G. L. Hunt, Jr., University of California, Irvine, seabirds spend 90% of their life at sea where they CA, USA forage over hundreds to thousands of kilometers in a matter of days, or dive to depths from the surface ^ Copyright 2001 Academic Press to several hundred meters. Although many details of doi:10.1006/rwos.2001.0233 seabird reproductive biology have been successfully SEABIRD FORAGING ECOLOGY 2637 elucidated, much of their life at sea remains a mesoscales (100}1000km, e.g. associations with mystery owing to logistical constraints placed on warm- or cold-core rings within current systems). research at sea. Even so, we now know a consider- The question of why species associate with different able amount about seabird foraging ecology in water types has not been adequately resolved. At terms of foraging habitat, behavior, and strategy, as issue are questions of whether a seabird responds well as the ways in which seabirds associate with or directly to habitat features that differ with water partition prey resources. mass (and may affect, for instance, thermoregula- tion), or directly to prey, assumed to change with Foraging Habitat water mass or current system.
    [Show full text]
  • ABSTRACT FAVREAU, JORIE M. the Effects Of
    ABSTRACT FAVREAU, JORIE M. The effects of food abundance, foraging rules and cognitive abilities on local animal movements. (Under the direction of Roger A. Powell.) Movement is nearly universal in the animal kingdom. Movements of animals influence not only themselves but also plant communities through processes such as seed dispersal, pollination, and herbivory. Understanding movement ecology is important for conserving biodiversity and predicting the spread of diseases and invasive species. Three factors influence nearly all movement. First, most animals move to find food. Thus, foraging dictates, in part, when and where to move. Second, animals must move by some rule even if the rule is “move at random.” Third, animals’ cognitive capabilities affect movement; even bees incorporate past experience into foraging. Although other factors such as competition and predation may affect movement, these three factors are the most basic to all movement. I simulated animal movement on landscapes with variable patch richness (amounts of food per food patch), patch density (number of patches), and variable spatial distributions of food patches. From the results of my simulations, I formulated hypotheses about the effects of food abundance on animal movement in nature. I also resolved the apparent paradox of real animals’ movements sometimes correlating positively and sometimes negatively with food abundance. I simulated variable foraging rules belonging to 3 different classes of rules (when to move, where to move, and the scale at which to assess the landscape). Simulating foraging rules demonstrated that variations in richness and density tend to have the same effects on movements, regardless of foraging rules. Still, foraging rules affect the absolute distance and frequency of movements.
    [Show full text]
  • A Practical Technique for Measuring the Behavior of Foraging Animals
    g k sTHow A PractIcal Technique for )D Do Measuring the Behavior of It _ . It Foraging hnlmalS RosemaryJ. Smith Joel S. Brown Animal behavior is well suited for havioral questions that can be readily ness (survivaland reproduction).If so, illustrating ecological and evolution- addressed gives students the opportu- an animal should leave a patch when ary principles. For ecology projects or nity to suggest their own hypotheses its benefits of continued feeding in the labs, animal behavior has a number of and experiments.The projectsempha- patch no longer exceed its cost of feed- advantages:It is inherently interesting size the methodology of a scientist ing in the patch. The formal frame- Downloaded from http://online.ucpress.edu/abt/article-pdf/53/4/236/45243/4449276.pdf by guest on 01 October 2021 to most students, it can be observed, it and, we hope, the excitement that work behind the assertion in the pre- is flexible and it can change quickly. accompanies creative thinking and vious sentence is called Optimal Unfortunately, meaningful observa- novel results. Foraging Theory (see Stephens & tions of behavior tend to be time- Krebs 1986). consuming and difficultto analyze. To We begin by elaborating a rule for overcome this handicap, we introduce Rationale when an animal should leave a patch an indirect procedure that uses the and then discuss how this rule can be foraging behavior of animals at exper- "Talking" to animals requires a common language, and feeding be- used to reveal aspects of that animal's imental food patches to address ques- ecology. An example from our own tions in animal behavior.
    [Show full text]