The MACHO Project SMC Variable Star Inventory. I. the Second-Overtone Mode of Cepheid Pulsation from First/Second Overtone (1H/2

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The MACHO Project SMC Variable Star Inventory. I. the Second-Overtone Mode of Cepheid Pulsation from First/Second Overtone (1H/2 View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by CERN Document Server The MACHO Pro ject SMC Variable Star Inventory. I. The Second-overtone Mo de of Cepheid Pulsation From First/Second Overtone (1H/2H) Beat Cepheids 1;2 3 1;4 5 6 1;2;7 C. Alco ck , R.A. Allsman , D. Alves , T.S. Axelro d , A.C. Becker , D.P. Bennett , 1;2 5 2;8 2;8 1;2 K.H. Co ok , K.C. Freeman ,K. Griest , M.J. Lehner , S.L. Marshall , 5 10 5 12 11 B.A. Peterson , P.J. Quinn , A.W. Ro dgers , A. Rorab eck ,W. Sutherland , 6 2;8 A. Tomaney , T. Vandehei (The MACHO Collab oration) { 2{ Received ; accepted 1 Lawrence Livermore National Lab oratory, Livermore, CA 94550 E-mail: alcock, alves, dminniti, kcook, [email protected] 2 Center for Particle Astrophysics, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720 3 Sup ercomputing Facility, Australian National University, Canb erra, ACT 0200, Australia E-mail: [email protected] 4 DepartmentofPhysics, University of California, Davis, CA 95616 5 Mt. Stromlo and Siding Spring Observatories, Australian National University, Weston Creek, ACT 2611, Australia E-mail: tsa, kcf,peterson, [email protected] 6 Departments of Astronomy and Physics, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195 E-mail: austin, becker, [email protected] 7 Physics Department, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN 46556 E-mail: [email protected] du 8 DepartmentofPhysics, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093 E-mail: kgriest, tvandehei, [email protected] 9 DepartmentofPhysics, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106 10 Europ ean Southern Observatory, Karl-Schwarzchild Str. 2, D-85748, Garching, Germany E-mail: [email protected] 11 Department of Physics, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3RH, U.K. E-mail: [email protected] c.uk 12 Dept. of Physics & Astronomy, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, L8S 4M1 Canada E-mail: welch, [email protected] .ca { 3{ ABSTRACT We rep ort the discovery of 20 1H/2H and 7 F/1H b eat Cepheids in the SMC by the MACHO Pro ject. We utilize the 20 1H/2H stars to determine lightcurve shap e for the SMC second-overtone (2H) mo de of Cepheid pulsation. We predict, similar to the ndings of Alco ck et al. [1997, ApJ, submitted], that 2H Cepheids will have nearly or purely sinusoidal lightvariations; that the P {L relation for 2H Cepheids will not be distinguishable from the P {L relation for 1H Cepheids within photometric accuracy; and that 2H stars may b e discernable from F and 1H stars using the amplitude-p erio d diagram and Fourier parameter progressions for p erio ds P < 0:7days, our current sample 2H p erio d limit. Subject headings: Cepheids | Magellanic Clouds | stars: fundamental parameters | stars: oscillations { 4{ 1. Intro duction The second-overtone (2H) mo de of Cepheid pulsation has b een predicted to exist theoretically since Stobie (1969a, 1969b)'s pioneering investigations. Yet, since then, we have found only scant evidence for 2H mo de excitation in our Galaxy. CO Aur was recognized as a rst-overtone/second-overtone (1H/2H) b eat Cepheid by Mantegazza (1983), and later con rmed as such by various studies (e.g.,Antonello & Mantegazza 1984; Bab el & Burki 1987). On the other hand, HR 7308 is a prop osed singly-p erio dic 2H Cepheid whose mo dal status remains uncertain, despite many investigations (Burki et al. 1986; Fabregat, Suso, & Reglero 1990; Simon 1985; Bersier 1996; Bersier & Burki 1996). This paucity of Galactic 2H Cepheids is not unexp ected. From a theoretical standp oint, Galactic 2H Cepheids should havelow masses and luminosities (Chiosi, Wo o d & Capitano 1993). As well, they are exp ected to be the shortest-p erio d Cepheids at a given luminosity (Chiosi et al. 1993), so that they should app ear in greater frequency in lower metallicityenvironments than our own (see e.g., the p erio d frequency distributions of Cepheids in Lipunova 1992). Observationally,inour own galaxy, CO Aur's semi-amplitude of pulsation for its 2H mo de is only 0:043 0:002 mag (Pardo & Poretti 1996)|so that, even if we observe these faint stars, we might not detect their variability. The advent of large-scale astronomical surveys has improved our chances of observing 2H Cepheids. As by-pro ducts of gravitational microlensing searches in the Galactic bulge and Magellanic Clouds, the MACHO and EROS Collab orations have found 45 1H/2H and at least 37 F/1H b eat Cepheids in the LMC (Alco ck et al. 1995, 1997; Beaulieu et al. 1997), and 27 1H/2H and 10 F/1H b eats (counting this work and Beaulieu et al. 1997) in the SMC to date. Concurrent analyses of these, and other ndings, has allowed investigations of the 2H mo de of Cepheid pulsation. Pardo & Poretti (1996) re-analyzed the comp osite lightcurve of CO Aur, the sole 1H/2H b eat Cepheid in the Galaxy, and noted that its 2H { 5{ mo de app eared as a purely sinusoidal light variation. Alco ck et al. (1997) analyzed 45 rst-overtone/second-overtone b eat Cepheids in the LMC, showing (1) that the 2H mo de 1 < 0:2) lightcurves ; (2) that LMC resulted in sinusoidal, or nearly sinusoidal (0 R 21 2H Cepheids could be distinguished from LMC 1H and F Cepheids in Fourier space for P < 1:4 days; (3) that 2H Cepheids should overlap the short-p erio d edge of the 1H P {L sequence; and (4) that the lo cation of 2H pulsators in the log L{log T plane dep ended e signi cantly on the adopted M {L relation, and would have to come from observation. Finally,Antonello & Kanbur (1997) haveinvestigated the 2H mo de of Cepheid pulsation by non-linear pulsation mo dels appropriate to the LMC (Z 0:01). They con rmed that 2H Cepheids should be more numerous for lower metallicities, and pro duced theoretical R {P 21 sequences which agreed qualitatively with the sequences for LMC 1H/2H b eat Cepheids in Welch et al. (1997). They also predicted a resonance of the R {P and {P sequences 21 21 near P =1 day. With the recent reduction of SMC photometry by the MACHO pro ject, we are in a p osition to add to our knowledge of the 2H mo de. We rep ort the discovery of 20 1H/2H b eat Cepheids in the SMC (distinct from the stars in Beaulieu et al. 1997), and their implications for the 2H mo de of Cepheid pulsation. We compare our ndings to the 2H mo de characterizations in the LMC and Galaxy to date, and provide guidance on how to discern 2H from F and 1H Cepheids. 1 R = V =V is the relative amplitude of the rst harmonic and `base' frequency mo del k 1 k 1 P O amplitudes in a truncated Fourier series V (t) = V + V cos(2k t + ), while the 0 k k k =1 phase di erence = k . For b eat Cepheids, R and are calculated for each k 1 k 1 21 21 mo de of pulsation. { 6{ 2. Observations and Analysis We refer the reader to Alco ck et al. (1995) for a description of our two-bandpass photometry (the MACHO V and R bands) and b eat Cepheid identi cation pro cess. The b eat Cepheids rep orted in this pap er were selected by our alert system software and not by a full analysis run. Therefore, the total number of b eat Cepheids in these elds is likely to be 4-5 greater than rep orted here. To be identi ed as an alert, a star must be 7 sigma brighter than the template and have increased in brightness by at least 0.35 mag. SMC observations of these Cepheids span 3years; lightcurves consist of anywhere from 163{1306 observations, which are free of p ossible cosmic ray events, bad or missing pixels, or data su ering from p o or image quality. This pap er utilizes MACHO V -band photometry for all results. We sub jected each star to our co ding of the CLEANest algorithm (Foster 1995, 1996a, 1996b) for joint frequency analysis and lightcurve mo delling. This metho d avoids having to cho ose a truncated Fourier series order a priori, as discussed in Pardo & Poretti (1997) and Alco ck et al. (1997). Brie y, CLEANest uses the date-comp ensated discrete Fourier transform (DCDFT) of Ferraz-Mello (1981) on a time series to pro duce a power sp ectrum 1 1 for test frequencies from = (2T ) to = (2 min(t)) in steps of (the res span max res frequency resolution), where T is the total timespan of the observations for a star, span and t the time separation between successive observations. If any of the frequencies in the power sp ectrum are adopted as signi cant, they are mo deled by cos(2 t) and sin(2 t) terms (plus a constant) as in Foster (1995). The resultant mo del is subtracted from the data, these residuals are sub jected to another DCDFT, and the pro cess is iterated until no signi cant frequencies remain. Each time a DCDFT of the data or residuals has b een p erformed, CLEANest seeks to nd the n-tuple of frequencies which gives the b est description of the data. Op erationally, frequency space is searched for frequencies in the { 7{ neighb ourho o d of the currently adopted ones for a maximum of Foster (1996a, 1996b)'s mo del amplitude.
Recommended publications
  • Significance of Beating Observed in Earthquake Responses of Buildings
    SIGNIFICANCE OF BEATING OBSERVED IN EARTHQUAKE RESPONSES OF BUILDINGS Mehmet Çelebi1, S. Farid Ghahari2, and Ertuğrul Taciroǧlu2 U.S. Geological Survey1 and University of California, Los Angeles2 Menlo Park, California, USA1 and Los Angeles, California, USA2 Abstract The beating phenomenon observed in the recorded responses of a tall building in Japan and another in the U.S. are examined in this paper. Beating is a periodic vibrational behavior caused by distinctive coupling between translational and torsional modes that typically have close frequencies. Beating is prominent in the prolonged resonant responses of lightly damped structures. Resonances caused by site effects also contribute to accentuating the beating effect. Spectral analyses and system identification techniques are used herein to quantify the periods and amplitudes of the beating effects from the strong motion recordings of the two buildings. Quantification of beating effects is a first step towards determining remedial actions to improve resilient building performance to strong earthquake induced shaking. Introduction In a cursory survey of several textbooks on structural dynamics, it can be seen that beating effects have not been included in their scopes. On the other hand, as more earthquake response records from instrumented buildings became available, it also became evident that the beating phenomenon is common. As modern digital equipment routinely provide recordings of prolonged responses of structures, we were prompted to visit the subject of beating, since such response characteristics may impact the instantaneous and long-term shaking performances of buildings during large or small earthquakes. The main purpose in deploying seismic instruments in buildings (and other structures) is to record their responses during seismic events to facilitate studies understanding and assessing their behavior and performances during and future strong shaking events.
    [Show full text]
  • Helmholtz's Dissonance Curve
    Tuning and Timbre: A Perceptual Synthesis Bill Sethares IDEA: Exploit psychoacoustic studies on the perception of consonance and dissonance. The talk begins by showing how to build a device that can measure the “sensory” consonance and/or dissonance of a sound in its musical context. Such a “dissonance meter” has implications in music theory, in synthesizer design, in the con- struction of musical scales and tunings, and in the design of musical instruments. ...the legacy of Helmholtz continues... 1 Some Observations. Why do we tune our instruments the way we do? Some tunings are easier to play in than others. Some timbres work well in certain scales, but not in others. What makes a sound easy in 19-tet but hard in 10-tet? “The timbre of an instrument strongly affects what tuning and scale sound best on that instrument.” – W. Carlos 2 What are Tuning and Timbre? 196 384 589 amplitude 787 magnitude sample: 0 10000 20000 30000 0 1000 2000 3000 4000 time: 0 0.23 0.45 0.68 frequency in Hz Tuning = pitch of the fundamental (in this case 196 Hz) Timbre involves (a) pattern of overtones (Helmholtz) (b) temporal features 3 Some intervals “harmonious” and others “discordant.” Why? X X X X 1.06:1 2:1 X X X X 1.89:1 3:2 X X X X 1.414:1 4:3 4 Theory #1:(Pythagoras ) Humans naturally like the sound of intervals de- fined by small integer ratios. small ratios imply short period of repetition short = simple = sweet Theory #2:(Helmholtz ) Partials of a sound that are close in frequency cause beats that are perceived as “roughness” or dissonance.
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Tracking of the Musical Beat Is Enhanced by Low-Frequency Sounds
    Neural tracking of the musical beat is enhanced by low-frequency sounds Tomas Lenca, Peter E. Kellera, Manuel Varleta, and Sylvie Nozaradana,b,c,1 aMARCS Institute for Brain, Behaviour, and Development, Western Sydney University, Penrith, NSW 2751, Australia; bInstitute of Neuroscience (IONS), Université Catholique de Louvain, 1200 Woluwe-Saint-Lambert, Belgium; and cInternational Laboratory for Brain, Music, and Sound Research (BRAMS), Département de Psychologie, Faculté des Arts et des Sciences, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC H3C 3J7, Canada Edited by Dale Purves, Duke University, Durham, NC, and approved June 28, 2018 (received for review January 24, 2018) Music makes us move, and using bass instruments to build the content (8, 9). Bass sounds are also crucial in music that en- rhythmic foundations of music is especially effective at inducing courages listeners to move (10, 11). people to dance to periodic pulse-like beats. Here, we show that There has been a recent debate as to whether evolutionarily this culturally widespread practice may exploit a neurophysiolog- shaped properties of the auditory system lead to superior tem- ical mechanism whereby low-frequency sounds shape the neural poral encoding for bass sounds (12, 13). One study using elec- representations of rhythmic input by boosting selective locking to troencephalography (EEG) recorded brain responses elicited by the beat. Cortical activity was captured using electroencephalog- misaligned tone onsets in an isochronous sequence of simulta- raphy (EEG) while participants listened to a regular rhythm or to a neous low- and high-pitched tones (12). Greater sensitivity to the relatively complex syncopated rhythm conveyed either by low temporal misalignment of low tones was observed when they tones (130 Hz) or high tones (1236.8 Hz).
    [Show full text]
  • Thomas Young the Man Who Knew Everything
    Thomas Young The man Who Knew Everything Andrew Robinson marvels at the brain power and breadth of knowledge of the 18th-century polymath Thomas Young. He examines his relationship with his contemporaries, particularly with the French Egyptologist Champollion, and how he has been viewed subsequently by historians. ORTUNATE NEWTON, happy professor of natural philosophy at childhood of science!’ Albert the newly founded Royal Institution, F Einstein wrote in 1931 in his where in 1802-03 he delivered what foreword to the fourth edition of is generally regarded as the most far- Newton’s influential treatise Opticks, reaching series of lectures ever given originally published in 1704. by a scientist; a physician at a major London hospital, St George’s, for a Nature to him was an open book, quarter of a century; the secretary of whose letters he could read without the Admiralty’s Board of Longitude effort. ... Reflection, refraction, the and superintendent of its vital Nauti- formation of images by lenses, the cal Almanac from 1818 until his mode of operation of the eye, the death; and ‘inspector of calculations’ spectral decomposition and recom- for a leading life insurance company position of the different kinds of in the 1820s. In 1794, he was elected light, the invention of the reflecting a fellow of the Royal Society at the telescope, the first foundations of age of barely twenty-one, became its colour theory, the elementary theory foreign secretary at the age of thirty, of the rainbow pass by us in and turned down its presidency in procession, and finally come his his fifties.
    [Show full text]
  • Large Scale Sound Installation Design: Psychoacoustic Stimulation
    LARGE SCALE SOUND INSTALLATION DESIGN: PSYCHOACOUSTIC STIMULATION An Interactive Qualifying Project Report submitted to the Faculty of the WORCESTER POLYTECHNIC INSTITUTE in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Bachelor of Science by Taylor H. Andrews, CS 2012 Mark E. Hayden, ECE 2012 Date: 16 December 2010 Professor Frederick W. Bianchi, Advisor Abstract The brain performs a vast amount of processing to translate the raw frequency content of incoming acoustic stimuli into the perceptual equivalent. Psychoacoustic processing can result in pitches and beats being “heard” that do not physically exist in the medium. These psychoac- oustic effects were researched and then applied in a large scale sound design. The constructed installations and acoustic stimuli were designed specifically to combat sensory atrophy by exer- cising and reinforcing the listeners’ perceptual skills. i Table of Contents Abstract ............................................................................................................................................ i Table of Contents ............................................................................................................................ ii Table of Figures ............................................................................................................................. iii Table of Tables .............................................................................................................................. iv Chapter 1: Introduction .................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • The Concept of the Photon—Revisited
    The concept of the photon—revisited Ashok Muthukrishnan,1 Marlan O. Scully,1,2 and M. Suhail Zubairy1,3 1Institute for Quantum Studies and Department of Physics, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843 2Departments of Chemistry and Aerospace and Mechanical Engineering, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544 3Department of Electronics, Quaid-i-Azam University, Islamabad, Pakistan The photon concept is one of the most debated issues in the history of physical science. Some thirty years ago, we published an article in Physics Today entitled “The Concept of the Photon,”1 in which we described the “photon” as a classical electromagnetic field plus the fluctuations associated with the vacuum. However, subsequent developments required us to envision the photon as an intrinsically quantum mechanical entity, whose basic physics is much deeper than can be explained by the simple ‘classical wave plus vacuum fluctuations’ picture. These ideas and the extensions of our conceptual understanding are discussed in detail in our recent quantum optics book.2 In this article we revisit the photon concept based on examples from these sources and more. © 2003 Optical Society of America OCIS codes: 270.0270, 260.0260. he “photon” is a quintessentially twentieth-century con- on are vacuum fluctuations (as in our earlier article1), and as- Tcept, intimately tied to the birth of quantum mechanics pects of many-particle correlations (as in our recent book2). and quantum electrodynamics. However, the root of the idea Examples of the first are spontaneous emission, Lamb shift, may be said to be much older, as old as the historical debate and the scattering of atoms off the vacuum field at the en- on the nature of light itself – whether it is a wave or a particle trance to a micromaser.
    [Show full text]
  • PSYCHOACOUSTICS: Software Package for Psychoacoustics
    Acoust. Sci. & Tech. 41, 1 (2020) #2020 The Acoustical Society of Japan INVITED REVIEW PSYCHOACOUSTICS: Software package for psychoacoustics Aleksander P. Se˛k1;Ã and Brian C. J. Moore2;y 1Institute of Acoustics, Faculty of Physics, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan´, Poland 2Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom Abstract: Current research in the field of psychoacoustics is mostly conducted using a computer to generate and present the stimuli and to collect the responses of the subject. However, writing the computer software to do this is time-consuming and requires technical expertise that is not possessed by many would-be researchers. We have developed a software package that makes it possible to set up and conduct a wide variety of experiments in psychoacoustics without the need for time-consuming programming or technical expertise. The only requirements are a personal computer (PC) with a good- quality sound card and a set of headphones. Parameters defining the stimuli and procedure are entered via boxes on the screen and drop-down menus. Possible experiments include measurement of the absolute threshold, simultaneous and forward masking (including notched-noise masking), comodu- lation masking release, intensity and frequency discrimination, amplitude-modulation detection and discrimination, gap detection, discrimination of interaural time and level differences, measurement of sensitivity to temporal fine structure, and measurement of the binaural masking level difference. The software is intended to be useful both for researchers and for students who want to try psychoacoustic experiments for themselves, which can be very valuable in helping them gain a deeper understanding of auditory perception.
    [Show full text]
  • Musical Acoustics Timbre / Tone Quality I
    Musical Acoustics Lecture 13 Timbre / Tone quality I Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 1 Waves: review distance x (m) At a given time t: y = A sin(2πx/λ) A time t (s) -A At a given position x: y = A sin(2πt/T) Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 2 Perfect Tuning Fork: Pure Tone • As the tuning fork vibrates, a succession of compressions and rarefactions spread out from the fork • A harmonic (sinusoidal) curve can be used to represent the longitudinal wave • Crests correspond to compressions and troughs to rarefactions • only one single harmonic (pure tone) is needed to describe the wave Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 3 Phase δ $ x ' % x ( y = Asin& 2π ) y = Asin' 2π + δ* % λ( & λ ) Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 4 € € Adding waves: Beats Superposition of 2 waves with slightly different frequency The amplitude changes as a function of time, so the intensity of sound changes as a function of time. The beat frequency (number of intensity maxima/minima per second): fbeat = |fa-fb| Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 5 The perceived frequency is the average of the two frequencies: f + f f = 1 2 perceived 2 The beat frequency (rate of the throbbing) is the difference€ of the two frequencies: fbeats = f1 − f 2 € Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 6 Factors Affecting Timbre 1. Amplitudes of harmonics 2. Transients (A sudden and brief fluctuation in a sound. The sound of a crack on a record, for example.) 3. Inharmonicities 4. Formants 5. Vibrato 6. Chorus Effect Two or more sounds are said to be in unison when they are at the same pitch, although often an OCTAVE may exist between them.
    [Show full text]
  • The Perception of Pure and Mistuned Musical Fifths and Major Thirds: Thresholds for Discrimination, Beats, and Identification
    Perception & Psychophysics 1982,32 (4),297-313 The perception ofpure and mistuned musical fifths and major thirds: Thresholds for discrimination, beats, and identification JOOS VOS Institute/or Perception TNO, Soesterberg, TheNetherlands In Experiment 1, the discriminability of pure and mistuned musical intervals consisting of si­ multaneously presented complex tones was investigated. Because of the interference of nearby harmonics, two features of beats were varied independently: (1) beat frequency, and (2) the depth of the level variation. Discrimination thresholds (DTs) were expressed as differences in level (AL) between the two tones. DTs were determined for musical fifths and major thirds, at tone durations of 250, 500, and 1,000 msec, and for beat frequencies within a range of .5 to 32 Hz. The results showed that DTs were higher (smaller values of AL) for major thirds than for fifths, were highest for the lowest beat frequencies, and decreased with increasing tone duration. Interaction of tone duration and beat frequency showed that DTs were higher for short tones than for sustained tones only when the mistuning was not too large. It was concluded that, at higher beat frequencies, DTs could be based more on the perception of interval width than on the perception of beats or roughness. Experiments 2 and 3 were designed to ascertain to what extent this was true. In Experiment 2, beat thresholds (BTs)for a large number of different beat frequencies were determined. In Experiment 3, DTs, BTs, and thresholds for the identifica­ tion of the direction of mistuning (ITs) were determined. For mistuned fifths and major thirds, sensitivity to beats was about the same.
    [Show full text]
  • Discovery of Beat Cepheids in the Small Magellanic Cloud and the Effect of Metallicity on Pulsation
    Astron. Astrophys. 321, L5–L8 (1997) ASTRONOMY AND ASTROPHYSICS LETTER Letter to the Editor EROS VARIABLE STARS: Discovery of Beat Cepheids in the Small Magellanic Cloud and the effect of metallicity on pulsation J.P. Beaulieu1;2, M. Krockenberger3, D.D. Sasselov3, C. Renault4, R. Ferlet1, A. Vidal-Madjar1, E. Maurice5,L.Prevot´ 5, E. Aubourg4, P. Bareyre4, S. Brehin4, C. Coutures4, N. Delabrouille4,J.deKat4,M.Gros4, B. Laurent4, M. Lachieze-Rey` 4, E. Lesquoy4, C. Magneville4, A. Milsztajn4, L. Moscoso4, F. Queinnec4, J. Rich4 ,M. Spiro4, L. Vigroux4, S. Zylberajch4, R. Ansari6, F. Cavalier6, M. Moniez6,C.Gry7 The EROS Collaboration 1 Kapteyn Laboratorium, Postbus 800 9700 AV Groningen, The Netherlands 2 Institut d’Astrophysique de Paris, CNRS, 98bis Boulevard. Arago, F-75014 Paris, France 3 Harvard-Smithsonian Center for Astrophysics, 60 Garden St., Cambridge, MA 02138, USA 4 CEA, DSM/DAPNIA, Centre d’etudes´ de Saclay, F-91191 Gif-sur-Yvette, France 5 Observatoire de Marseille, 2 place Le Verrier, F-13248 Marseille 04, France 6 Laboratoire de l’Accel´ erateur´ Lineaire´ IN2P3, Centre d’Orsay, F-91405 Orsay, France 7 Laboratoire d’Astronomie Spatiale CNRS, Traversee´ du siphon, les trois lucs, F-13120 Marseille, France Received 10 January 1997 / Accepted 7 February 1997 Abstract. We report the discovery of eleven beat Cepheids in be used to make the Fourier analysis to show a clear beating of the Small Magellanic Cloud, using data obtained by the EROS two modes. Therefore, this kind of studies are very difficult. microlensing survey. Four stars are beating in the fundamental Over the past five years, a great effort has been made to ob- and first overtone mode (F/1OT), seven are beating in the first tain light curves of millions of stars in the Magellanic Clouds and second overtone (1OT/2OT).
    [Show full text]
  • Kindergarten
    Kindergarten Lesson 1: If You’re Happy and You Know It: https://youtu.be/oXMc2aAxh-0 - Tell me if you enjoyed this song! Lesson 2: Lesson 3: The p stands for soft. The f stands for loud. 1st Grade Lesson 1: Make Your Own Egg Shaker: https://youtu.be/g8BA-SOr384 - Submit a photo of your egg shaker! We will use them in the future. Lesson 2: Students in the first grade are learning about quarter notes (ta) and quarter rest (no sound/ think ‘rest’ in your head). Each of these types of notes gets one beat. Below, create three lines that have four total beats in them, one beat per box. Practice the four beat rhythm, saying the above syllables for each type of note. Do one line at a time. Quarter Note- TA Quarter Rest- REST Example TA REST TA REST Lesson 3: Check VLA for your assignment 2nd Grade Lesson 1: Fast & Slow: https://youtu.be/fTyD2pfAsaI - What does largo mean? - What does presto mean? Lesson 2: Students in the second grade are learning about quarter notes (ta), pair of eighth notes (ti-ti) and quarter rest (no sound/ think ‘rest’ in your head). Each of these types of notes gets one beat. Below create four lines that have four total beats in them, one beat per box. Practice each lines rhythms saying the above syllables for each type of note. Quarter Note- TA Quarter Rest- REST Pair of Eighth Notes-TI-TI Example TA REST TA TI-TI Lesson 3: Listen to these two songs. Then draw a picture and write a sentence about your favorite one.
    [Show full text]
  • Demonstration of Acoustic Beats in the Study of Physics
    Journal of Multidisciplinary Engineering Science and Technology (JMEST) ISSN: 2458-9403 Vol. 4 Issue 6, June - 2017 Demonstration Of Acoustic Beats In The Study Of Physics Aleksei Gavrilov Department of Cybernetics Division of Physics Tallinn University of Technology Tallinn, Ehitajate tee 5, Estonia e-mail: [email protected] Abstract ― The article describes a device for equals to the difference of frequencies of the added demonstration of acoustic beats effect at the oscillations. To eliminate these inconveniences, two lecture on physics separate generators of low frequency oscillations, an audio amplifier and a loudspeaker are often used. In Keywords—beating; acoustics; device; this article, we describe a small-sized and convenient demonstration; physics device, which combines all these functions. II. Construction of the device I. Introduction Schematically the device is quite simple (see Addition of two harmonic oscillations with close Fig.1). It consists of a stabilized power supply, two frequencies creates so-called beats. Oscillations must identical audio frequency generators and an amplifier occur in the same direction. Beats in this case are with a loudspeaker at the output. From the generator periodic changes in the amplitude of the oscillations outputs the sound frequency voltage is connected [1]. With acoustic waves, these are periodic simultaneously to the input of the amplifier. The oscillations of the sound intensity. Usually this effect is frequency of one of the generators can be slightly demonstrated by means of two tuning forks [2]. changed by a regulator in relation to the frequency of However, the volume level at such a demonstration is the other generator.
    [Show full text]