The Challenge of Addressing Vivid Lies Spread by America's Enemies in The
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
A Test of Inoculation Theory
RESISTANCE TO ETHICALLY SUSPICIOUS PARODY VIDEO ON YOUTUBE: A TEST OF INOCULATION THEORY By Joon Soo Lim and Eyun-Jung Ki Having observed the proliferation of corporate stealth campaigns employ- ing parody videos on YouTube, we attempted to examine the effectiveness of refutational preemption in providing viewers with ability to detect unduly manipulative intent and in conferring viewer resistance to the influence of the campaign. We also compared the relative efficacy of the inoculation pretreatment to post-hoc refutation. Results showed that subjects who received a preemptive inoculation message were better able to detect unduly manipulative intent in a parody video, were more resist- ant to altering their original attitudes toward the issue, and demonstrat- ed less favorable attitudes toward the sponsor of the parody video than did their counterparts in the control group. With the evolution of Web media from digital interactive media' to social media, new ethical issues involving the use of social media's unique characteristics have also emerged. Increasingly, corporate front groups have been abusing the characteristics of social media, such as user-generated content (UGC), word-of-mouth tactics, and anonymity. In conjunction with such trends, some professional public relations or lobbying firms have started to engage in clandestine campaigns that conceal their true identity or the sponsors of these campaigns. Parodies and spoofs have become popular formats for such clandestine campaign messages, thanks largely to the popularity of YouTube, a popular video-sharing Web site.^ One such example has been dubbed the "YouTube Penguin Army scandal." Recently, a video clip spoofing An Inconvenient Truth, Al Gore's documentary film aimed at raising public awareness of global warming, appeared on YouTube with the claim that it was an amateur production created by a 29- year-old Californian. -
"Firehose of Falsehood" Propaganda Model
Perspective C O R P O R A T I O N Expert insights on a timely policy issue The Russian “Firehose of Falsehood” Propaganda Model Why It Might Work and Options to Counter It Christopher Paul and Miriam Matthews ince its 2008 incursion into Georgia (if not before), there We characterize the contemporary Russian model for propa- has been a remarkable evolution in Russia’s approach to ganda as “the firehose of falsehood” because of two of its distinctive Spropaganda. This new approach was on full display during features: high numbers of channels and messages and a shameless the country’s 2014 annexation of the Crimean peninsula. willingness to disseminate partial truths or outright fictions. In It continues to be demonstrated in support of ongoing conflicts in the words of one observer, “[N]ew Russian propaganda entertains, Ukraine and Syria and in pursuit of nefarious and long-term goals confuses and overwhelms the audience.”2 in Russia’s “near abroad” and against NATO allies. Contemporary Russian propaganda has at least two other In some ways, the current Russian approach to propaganda distinctive features. It is also rapid, continuous, and repetitive, and builds on Soviet Cold War–era techniques, with an emphasis on it lacks commitment to consistency. obfuscation and on getting targets to act in the interests of the Interestingly, several of these features run directly counter to propagandist without realizing that they have done so.1 In other the conventional wisdom on effective influence and communica- ways, it is completely new and driven by the characteristics of the tion from government or defense sources, which traditionally contemporary information environment. -
The Sleeper Effect: Hypnotism, Mind Control, Terrorism
John Frankenheimer, dir. The Manchurian Candidate , 1962. Raymond’s mother manipulates and controls newspaper and television images that affect the whole nation. 88 Downloaded from http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/GREY_a_00051 by guest on 23 September 2021 The Sleeper Effect: Hypnotism, Mind Control, Terrorism STEFAN ANDRIOPOULOS Anxieties about clandestine terror cells have been prevalent in popular culture and political discourse since the attacks of September 11, 2001. Television shows such as Sleeper Cell: American Terror and 24 center on the frightening figure of the “sleeper”—a terrorist who lies dormant in our midst, living an ostensibly normal life while secretly plotting acts of destruction and mayhem. Corresponding to and possibly inspired by these fictional scenarios was the real-life but failed attempt to explode a bomb-packed car in New York’s Times Square by Faisal Shahzad, a natu - ralized U.S. citizen who lived in a small town in Connecticut. But the fear-provoking figure of the sleeper has a long prehistory—in Cold War culture and at the end of the nineteenth century. The Manchurian candidate Raymond Shaw seems to be a patriotic American, a war hero who earned the Medal of Honor fighting Communist North Korea. But as readers of Richard Condon’s best-selling novel know, he is in reality under “remote control,” waiting to be activated by his “operator” who will compel him to commit murder and to participate in an elaborate plot that is meant to destroy American democracy. 1 This fictional Cold War scenario of a brainwashed clandes - tine assassin coincided with contemporaneous warnings against the hypnotic power of advertising and theories of propaganda that described the delayed and clandestine workings of unreliable information as the so-called sleeper effect. -
The Duration of Framing Effects
UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Framing politics Lecheler, S.K. Publication date 2010 Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Lecheler, S. K. (2010). Framing politics. General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl) Download date:28 Sep 2021 Chapter 3 GETTING REAL: THE DURATION OF FRAMING EFFECTS Manuscript under review Abstract A growing number of studies test the effects of news framing on citizens’ understanding of politics. By employing experimental designs, these studies report significant effects for a multitude of issues and frames. However, what happens to the framing effect after initial exposure? Based on a “classic” framing experiment (n = 625), this paper traces framing effects across a number of delayed time points: after one day, one week, and two weeks. -
The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion : Developing Health Promotions for Sustained Behavioral Change
7 THE ELABORATION LIKELIHOOD MODEL OF PERSUASION : DEVELOPING HEALTH PROMOTIONS FOR SUSTAINED BEHAVIORAL CHANGE RICHARD E. PETTY JAMIE BARDEN S. CHRISTIAN WHEELER To appear in: R. J. DiClemente, R. A. Crosby, & M. Kegler (Eds.), Emerging theories in health promotion practice and research (2nd ed.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 2 Influencing Behavioral Change Roughly half of all causes of mortality in the United States are tied to social and behavioral variables including smoking, alcohol use, diet and sedentary lifestyle (Institute of Medicine, 2000). Because of this, health promotion campaigns and research are typically designed to induce positive change in health-related behaviors. For example, a media campaign might attempt to convince people to use their seatbelts or to stop smoking. However, studies of the effectiveness of media and direct interventions have provided inconsistent results. In particular, efforts in critical areas such as drug and alcohol abuse and AIDS prevention have sometimes proven to be disappointing in terms of concrete successes. This challenge has led to a number of responses. Notably, there is a growing awareness of the importance of health promotion programs that establish sustained behavioral change, as distinct from merely impacting initiation of behavior change (Rothman, 2000). For example, between 2003 and 2007 the Office of Behavioral and Social Sciences Research awarded $53.8 million to support research investigating, “Maintenance of Long-Term Behavioral Change,” (Solomon, 2005). To understand why certain interventions with high face validity fail to provide sustained behavioral change, health promotion researchers and practitioners have sought insight from basic research on influence processes. In line with this view, funding agencies have identified the importance of uncovering the mechanisms underlying cause-effect relationships between specific intervention components and outcomes. -
A Framework for the Study of Persuasion
IPR Working Paper Series WP-21-28 A Framework for the Study of Persuasion James Druckman Northwestern University and IPR Version: May 19, 2021 DRAFT Please do not quote or distribute without permission. Institute for Policy Research ● 2040 Sheridan Rd., Evanston, IL 60208 ● 847.491.3395 ● [email protected] Abstract Persuasion is a vital part of politics – who wins elections and policy disputes often depends on which side can persuade more people. Given this centrality, the study of persuasion has a long history with an enormous number of theories and empirical inquiries. But it is fragmented, with few generalizable findings. Druckman unifies previously disparate dimensions of this topic by presenting a framework focusing on actors (speakers and receivers), treatments (topics, content, media), outcomes (attitudes, behaviors, emotions, identity), and settings (competition, space, time, process, culture). This “Generalizing Persuasion (GP) Framework” organizes distinct findings and offers researchers a structure in which to situate their work. He concludes with a discussion of the normative implications of persuasion. Forthcoming in: Annual Review of Political Science INTRODUCTION The study of persuasion has a long and storied history, dating back to Aristotle and the Greeks (Barnes 1984). In the 20th century, social scientists joined modern philosophers and rhetoricians (Foucault 1980, Habermas 1984, Booth 2004) in the pursuit of better understanding persuasion. Like the ancients, modern social scientists focus on how speakers’ characteristics, what they say, and how recipients think determine whether a given persuasive attempt is successful. The framework – characterized long ago by Lasswell’s (1948) “who says what, to whom, in which channel” – captures how much of the social sciences treat persuasion (e.g., O’Keefe 2016, Lau 2020).