
NeuroImage 50 (2010) 1148–1167 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain Svenja Caspers a,b,⁎, Karl Zilles a,b,c, Angela R. Laird d, Simon B. Eickhoff a,b,e a Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-2), Research Centre Jülich, 52425 Jülich, Germany b JARA-BRAIN, Jülich-Aachen Research Alliance, 52425 Jülich, Germany c C. and O. Vogt Institute for Brain Research, Heinrich-Heine-University Düsseldorf, 40001 Düsseldorf, Germany d Research Imaging Institute, University of Texas Health Science Center, San Antonio, TX, USA e Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, RWTH Aachen University, 52074 Aachen, Germany article info abstract Article history: Over the last decade, many neuroimaging studies have assessed the human brain networks underlying Received 5 November 2009 action observation and imitation using a variety of tasks and paradigms. Nevertheless, questions concerning Revised 23 December 2009 which areas consistently contribute to these networks irrespective of the particular experimental design and Accepted 24 December 2009 how such processing may be lateralized remain unresolved. The current study aimed at identifying cortical Available online 4 January 2010 areas consistently involved in action observation and imitation by combining activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis with probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps. Meta-analysis of 139 functional Keywords: Action observation magnetic resonance and positron emission tomography experiments revealed a bilateral network for both Imitation action observation and imitation. Additional subanalyses for different effectors within each network revealed Inferior parietal highly comparable activation patterns to the overall analyses on observation and imitation, respectively, Meta-analysis indicating an independence of these findings from potential confounds. Conjunction analysis of action Mirror neurons observation and imitation meta-analyses revealed a bilateral network within frontal premotor, parietal, and temporo-occipital cortex. The most consistently rostral inferior parietal area was PFt, providing evidence for a possible homology of this region to macaque area PF. The observation and imitation networks differed particularly with respect to the involvement of Broca's area: whereas both networks involved a caudo-dorsal part of BA 44, activation during observation was most consistent in a more rostro-dorsal location, i.e., dorsal BA 45, while activation during imitation was most consistent in a more ventro-caudal aspect, i.e., caudal BA 44. The present meta-analysis thus summarizes and amends previous descriptions of the human brain networks related to action observation and imitation. © 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Introduction neuroimaging experiments. Over the last decade, several studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron The neural bases of action observation and action imitation in the emission tomography (PET) have investigated different aspects of human brain have been a longstanding interest of neuroscientific action processing in the human brain (e.g., Buccino et al., 2004b; research. Increasing attention was focused on these functions and Iacoboni et al., 1999) that are conceptually related to “mirror” their neuronal correlates when “mirror neurons” were identified in properties, in particular action observation and imitation. the macaque brain using single-cell recordings (Gallese et al., 1996; Investigation into the human action observation network directly Fogassi et al., 2005). These neurons are active not only when relates to the properties of mirror neurons as defined in nonhuman performing an action but also when observing another subject primates. It is assumed that observing actions enables the mirror performing the same action (Gallese et al., 1996). This discovery in neuron system to understand the actions themselves as well as the the macaque brain raised the question of whether a comparable underlying intentions (e.g., Fabbri-Destro and Rizzolatti 2008; system also exists in humans (e.g., Rizzolatti et al., 2001). However, Rizzolatti 2005; Rizzolatti and Fabbri-Destro 2008). By understanding since single-cell recordings are rarely feasible in humans, a direct the action with one's own motor system, it is possible to infer on the demonstration of mirror properties for individual human neurons has intentions behind a motor act (e.g., Prinz 2006; Schütz-Bosbach and not yet been provided. Consequently, evidence for possible “mirror” Prinz, 2007), a mechanism that already has been proposed long before areas in humans is predominantly based on the results of functional the discovery of mirror neurons (e.g., Viviani and Terzuolo, 1973). Such ability is then seen as a crucial step towards the development of complex interpersonal and social interactions as witnessed in humans ⁎ Corresponding author. Institut für Neurowissenschaften und Medizin, INM-2, Forschungszentrum Jülich, 52425 Jülich, Germany. Fax: +49 2461 612990. but also other primates (Iacoboni 2009; Rizzolatti and Fabbri-Destro, E-mail address: [email protected] (S. Caspers). 2008). 1053-8119/$ – see front matter © 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.112 S. Caspers et al. / NeuroImage 50 (2010) 1148–1167 1149 Understanding an action and its intention might also provide an Lancaster, 2002, Laird et al., 2005b) and a PubMed literature search important link between the sole observation of an action and its (www.pubmed.org, search strings: “mirror neurons”, “imitation”, and subsequent imitation by directly copying the observed action (e.g., “action observation”) on functional magnetic resonance imaging Fabbri-Destro and Rizzolatti, 2008; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) experiments. The Rumiati et al., 2005). Furthermore, imitation offers a potential literature cited in the obtained papers was also assessed to identify mechanism for learning from the early stages of life. The motor additional neuroimaging studies dealing with action observation or system can learn how specific actions are carried out by imitating imitation processing. Only studies that reported results of whole- them (e.g., Bandura and Wood, 1989; Brass and Heyes, 2005; Iacoboni, brain group analyses as coordinates in a standard reference space 2005), a mechanism that has long been discovered much earlier in (Talairach/Tournoux, MNI) were analysed, while single-subject human neonates (Meltzoff and Moore, 1977). Furthermore, just like reports were excluded. Based on these criteria, 87 articles (reporting action understanding, imitation processes play an important role 83 fMRI and 4 PET studies) were designated as suitable for meta- during social interactions: people also tend to imitate behaviours of analysis. Together, these studies included data from 1289 subjects and their social partners (either consciously or subconsciously) to adapt to reported 139 experiments with 1932 activation foci (Table 1). a given social situation (e.g., Bargh et al., 1996; Iacoboni, 2009; The reported tasks were subsumed into two main categories: Niedenthal et al., 1985; Schilbach et al., 2008a). “action observation” and “action imitation”: 104 experiments Therefore, assessment of the neural substrates of both action reported action observation tasks (1061 subjects, 1390 activation observation and action imitation is not only important for under- foci), and 35 reported imitation tasks (459 subjects, 542 activation standing action-related processes but also holds further implications foci). Action observation comprised those experiments in which for cognitive and social neuroscience. In spite of the considerable subjects were instructed to observe the action performed by others number of neuroimaging studies on these action-related topics, the without performing their own motor act. In this first analysis, the organisation of the respective networks in the human brain and their general action observation brain network was assessed. There are, anatomical correlates are still disputed (Dinstein et al., 2008; Iacoboni, however, several possible confounds that may influence the analysis 2005, 2009; Keysers and Gazzola, 2009). One controversial aspect is across the whole sample of observation and imitation experiments, the role of Broca's region in action- related processes (Brass and like effectors, instructions or the involvement of an object. To explore Heyes, 2005; Molenberghs et al., 2009; Molnar-Szakacs et al., 2005; the effects of these potential confounds, we subdivided the studies Vogt et al., 2007). Another is the hemispheric dominance of such into several subgroups. These were then analysed separately to reveal functions, as arguments have been made for a leading role of either the neural correlates of different forms of action observation and hemisphere as well as for a bilateral distribution (e.g., Iacoboni and compared among each other by contrast and conjunction analyses: Dapretto, 2006). Finally, since observation and imitation are closely observation of hand actions (‘right hand’ (37 experiments), ‘left hand’ related, the question of whether they are sustained by the same (2 experiments), ‘both hands’,or‘hand not specified’ (23 experi- neuronal networks or engage different brain areas is still disputed ments)), observation of right hand actions, observation of face actions,
Details
-
File Typepdf
-
Upload Time-
-
Content LanguagesEnglish
-
Upload UserAnonymous/Not logged-in
-
File Pages20 Page
-
File Size-