Télécharger Article

Télécharger Article

Revue de Traduction & Langues Volume 17 Numéro 2/2018, pp. 51-75 مجلة الترجمة واللغات Journal of Translation & Languages ISSN (Print): 1112-3974 EISSN (Online): 2600-6235 On Lexical Obsolescence in Tacawit: The Case of Six Berber Fauna Terms CHAIRA Farid University of Constantine 1 Frères Mentouri – Algeria Laboratoire National de Didactique du Français LANADIF [email protected] Pr. HAMADA Hacène Ecole Normale Supérieure Constantine – Algeria Laboratoire National de Didactique du Français LANADIF [email protected] Received: 03/ 09/ 2018; Accepted: 30/ 12/ 2018, Published: 31/12/2018 Abstract: The present paper gives an account of a cross-regional study of lexical obsolescence in Tacawit. It aims to compare the rates of lexical erosion across three regions: the Aurès Massif, Occidental Aurès and Oriental Aurès. The data of the study were collected as part of a doctoral research project on contact-induced lexical erosion in Tacawit. For purposes of brevity, however, this paper is confined to the analysis of the data obtained from one semantic domain, namely animal lexicon. Six basic concepts denoting six species were examined. The study revealed significant differences in the rates of lexical erosion between the three regions. The Massif retained most of the Berber variants, and, to a lesser degree, Occidental Aurès. In Oriental Aurès, however, there was a general tendency towards lexical replacement. This regional variation in lexical maintenance reflects different social tendencies within Aurès towards the effects of contact between Berber and Arabic. Keywords: language contact - lexical borrowing - lexical erosion - Tacawit. Résumé : Cet article présente une étude interrégionale de l'obsolescence lexicale dans le chaoui. Il vise à comparer les taux des pertes lexicaux entre trois régions: le massif de l’Aurès, l’Aurès occidental et l’Aurès oriental. Les données de l'étude ont été collectées dans le cadre d'une recherche de doctorat sur l'érosion lexicale induite par le contact des langues. Par souci de concision, cet article se limite à l’analyse des données obtenues dans un seul domaine sémantique, à savoir le lexique animal. Six concepts de base indiquant six espèces ont été examinés. L'étude a révélé des différences Corresponding author : Chaira Farid 51 Revue de Traduction & Langues Journal of Translation & Languages significatives dans les taux d'érosion lexicale entre les trois régions. Le Massif a conservé la plupart des variantes berbères et, dans une moindre mesure, l’Aurès occidental. Cependant, dans l’Aurès oriental, il y avait une tendance générale au remplacement lexical. Cette variation régionale de la maintenance lexicale reflète les tendances sociales au sein de l'Aurès en ce qui concerne l'intensité du contact entre le berbère et l’Arabe. Mots clés : contact des langues - érosion lexicale - emprunt lexical - le Chaoui. 1. Introduction Words are the least stable elements in the language system and, for this reason, the first to transfer when two cultures come into contact (Thomason and Kaufman, 1988; Winford, 2010). In contact situations, both minority and dominant languages borrow words from one another. Nevertheless, evidence from hundreds, if not thousands, of studies revealed that minority languages borrow more. The more intense the contact, the more a minority language imports to its lexical storehouse, and the deeper. By definition, borrowing entails importing items that one does not possess, but as will be seen later, lexical borrowing in minority languages often goes beyond the transfer of words denoting new referents. In intense contact situations, speakers of minority languages engage in borrowing words that denote basic notions, such as body parts, natural phenomena, basic human actions, and the like (Haugen, 1953; Brahimi, 2000; Kossmann, 2009, 2013). In this regard, it is interesting to consider the fate of the words duplicated. Linguists refer to this phenomenon using different terminology, such as lexical obsolescence, lexical erosion, lexical attrition, lexical replacement, and the like. Large-scale lexical replacement, which is subsequent to massive lexical borrowing, is viewed by linguists, and even lay native speakers, as a sign of a language losing ground to a more prestigious dominant one, or, to phrase it more accurately, a symptom of language death. 2. Lexical Borrowing and Lexical Replacement Contact linguistics is mainly concerned with the understanding of the different linguistic phenomena that take place when languages engage in what is referred to as language contact, i.e. the condition where two or more languages are used in the same 52 Revue de Traduction & Langues Journal of Translation & Languages setting. One of the most studied phenomenon within the area of language contact is borrowing. Linguistic borrowing is defined by Thomason and Kaufman (1988) as “the incorporation of foreign features in a group’s native language by the speakers of that language” (p. 21). Those features can be lexical, phonological, morphological or syntactic. Lexical borrowing, hence, can be defined, following Thomason and Kaufman (1988), as the incorporation of foreign lexical items in a language by its speakers. Linguists distinguish, in terms of the motives that may drive a speaker to copy words from another language or dialect, between two types of borrowing phenomena, cultural and core borrowing (Myers-Scotton, 1993a, 2002; Haspelmath, 2009; Kossmann, 2013). The former was first coined by Bloomfield (1933) to refer to the process of importing foreign items to designate meanings that do not have equivalents in the language of the recipient culture. Although, in theory, any language can generate native words for new referents by resorting to some other linguistic innovating processes, there is ample evidence in the literature that suggests that, in contact situations, borrowing is the norm. Weinreich (1953) states that “using ready-made designations is more economical than describing things afresh” (p. 57). Following Hockett (1958), linguists tend to agree that the most apparent motive for cultural lexical borrowing is of a need- filling nature (Hock, 1991; McMahon, 1994). Cultural borrowing is a bi-directional process in that both the minority and the dominant language borrow from one another (Bloomfield, 1933). The difference is that the rate of borrowing varies depending on the direction of lexical transfer (Bloomfield, 1933). In most, if not all, contact situations, minority languages borrow from the dominant languages they are in contact with more than the other way round. Interestingly, lexical borrowing is not always motivated by necessity. Speakers of minority languages do actually borrow words for meanings that already denoted in their language. Haugen (1953) states that “borrowing always goes beyond the actual needs of language” (p. 373). Bloomfield (1933) distinguishes between cultural borrowing and intimate borrowing that occurs between two languages that co-exist in one single community and which targets “speech forms that are not 53 Revue de Traduction & Langues Journal of Translation & Languages connected with cultural novelties” (p. 461). This process is known as core, or substitutive, borrowing (Haspelmath, 2009; Kossmaan, 2013). Myers-Scotton (1993a) states that core borrowings are “taken into a language even though the recipient language already has lexemes of its own to encode the concepts or objects in question” (p. 5). Myers-Scotton & Okeju (1973) argues that this type of lexical borrowing takes place in intense contact situations and presupposes a widespread bilingualism (broadly defined as the condition where individuals are able to communicate in more than one language). Myers-Scotton (1993b) contends that core borrowings are distinguished by the fact that there is “no urgent consensus” mandating their use on the same scale compared to cultural borrowings (p. 175). Core borrowing, contrary to cultural borrowing, is, most often, one-sided, from the dominant to the minority language (Bloomfield, 1933; Hockett, 1958). The question of why speakers of a given language borrow words for meanings that already exist in their native language has attracted the attention of many linguists. Most scholars seem to agree that the main motive for core borrowing is prestige (Hockett, 1958; Myers-Scotton, 2002; Haspelmath, 2009). McMahon (1994) argues that “the second major motivation for borrowing is essentially social, and depends on perceptions of prestige” (p. 202). Hockett (1958) distinguishes, in this regard, between three types of prestige: people ‘emulate those whom they admire’, wish to be ‘identified with’ a group of people, and seek ‘conformity with the majority’ (p. 404). Core lexical borrowing, counter to cultural borrowing which is additive in nature, leads usually in due time to a displacement of the native lexical equivalents duplicated by the loanwords, hence the appellation substitutive borrowing. Weinreich (1953: 54) points out that core borrowings affect the existing equivalents in ‘one of three ways’: (1) confusion between the content of the new and old word; (2) disappearance of the old word; (3) survival of both the new and old word, with a specialization in content”. Hock (1991) contends that while need borrowings may well enrich the lexicon of a given language, prestige borrowings may lead to a “competition between an inherited and an innovated form” and may also end in a marginalization of the inherited form. Replacement of basic vocabulary, thus, can

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    25 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us