Developmental Differentiation of Executive Functions on the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery

Developmental Differentiation of Executive Functions on the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery

Neuropsychology © 2018 American Psychological Association 2018, Vol. 32, No. 7, 777–783 0894-4105/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000476 Developmental Differentiation of Executive Functions on the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery Natacha Akshoomoff and Timothy T. Brown Roger Bakeman University of California, San Diego Georgia State University Donald J. Hagler Jr. On Behalf of the University of California, San Diego Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics Study Objective: The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NTCB) is a brief computerized method for evaluating neuropsychological functions in children, adolescents, and adults. We examined how performance on the 2 executive function measures of cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control was related to performance on the other NTCB measures across development. Method: Participants were 1,020 typically developing individuals between the ages of 3 and 21 from the Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics Study who were divided into 5 age groups (3–6, 7–9, 10–13, 14–17, and 18–21). Scores were adjusted for sex, level of parental education, and family income. Results: Although the correlations between the 2 executive function measures were moderate and consistent across age groups, their correlations with the other 5 cognitive measures were highest in the youngest age group and decreased across the older age groups. Exploratory factor analysis revealed that all NTCB measures loaded onto a single factor for the 3- to 6-year-olds. Across the older age groups, the executive function and processing speed measures loaded onto one factor, and the vocabulary knowledge, oral reading, and working memory measures loaded onto a second factor. Conclusions: These results indicate that younger children’s performance on the NTCB is more intercorrelated and less differentiated, while performance on the NTCB executive function measures becomes more differentiated from performance on the other measures with develop- ment. These results support the hypothesis that executive functions become increasingly differentiated from other cognitive functions with development as the functional specialization of neural systems progresses throughout childhood and young adulthood. General Scientific Summary The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery is a brief computerized method for evaluating neuropsycho- logical functions in children, adolescents, and adults using the same set of tests. Characterization of performance on these measures across development will help us understand typical cognitive and brain development in children and apply these results to studies of school performance and children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Keywords: computerized assessment, cognitive development, executive functions This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. Natacha Akshoomoff, Department of Psychiatry and Center for Human Data used in preparation of this article were obtained from the Pediatric This article is intended solely for the personal use ofDevelopment, the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. University of California, San Diego; Timothy T. Brown, Imaging, Neurocognition and Genetics Study (PING) database (http://ping Center for Human Development and Department of Neurosciences and .chd.ucsd.edu). As such, the investigators within PING (T. L. Jernigan, Center for Multimodal Imaging and Genetics, University of California, San A. M. Dale, L. Chang, Natacha Akshoomoff, C. McCabe, E. Newman, Diego; Roger Bakeman, Department of Psychology, Georgia State Uni- T. M. Ernst, P. Van Zijl, J. M. Kuperman, S. S. Murray, C. S. Bloss, N. J. versity; Donald J. Hagler Jr., Department of Radiology and Center for Schork, W. Thompson, H. Bartsch, D. G. Amaral, E. R. Sowell, W. E. Multimodal Imaging and Genetics, University of California, San Diego; Kaufmann, P. Van Zijl, S. Mostofsky, B. J. Casey, B. Rosen, T. Kenet, Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics Study. J. A. Frazier, D. N. Kennedy, and J. R. Gruen) contributed to the design The authors gratefully thank the children, adolescents, young adults, and and implementation of PING and/or provided data but did not participate parents who participated in this study. This work was funded by a grant from in analysis or writing of this report. PING data are disseminated by the the National Institute on Drug Abuse awarded to Natacha Akshoomoff and PING Coordinating Center at the Center for Human Development, Uni- Timothy T. Brown (R01DA038958). Chase Reuter provided data support. versity of California, San Diego. Data collection and sharing for this project were funded by the National Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Natacha Institute on Drug Abuse and the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute Akshoomoff, Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine and Center for of Child Health and Human Development (Grants RC2DA029475 and Human Development, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman R01HD061414). Drive, LA Jolla, CA 92093-0115. E-mail: [email protected] 777 778 AKSHOOMOFF ET AL. The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NTCB) was designed to Here we examined associations among NTCB variables within tap key functions (executive function, attention, episodic memory, five age groups in the PING sample. We defined the age groups to working memory, language, and processing speed) across the life reflect commonly understood developmental periods (early child- span (ages 3–85 years; Gershon et al., 2010). For pediatric studies, hood, middle childhood, puberty, adolescence, and young adult- this approach has the advantage of a brief, computerized assess- hood) by grouping participants ages 3–6, 7–9, 10–13, 14–17, and ment using the same set of measures with young children, older 18–21 together. We were particularly interested in how perfor- children, and adolescents. mance on the two primary measures of executive functioning, the In a previous report, we described the age-related changes in Dimensional Change Card Sort Test (a measure of cognitive performance on the NTCB from a large normative sample of 1,020 flexibility) and the Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test individuals ranging in age from 3 to 21 years (Akshoomoff et al., (a measure of inhibitory control in the context of selective visual 2014). These data were collected as part of the Pediatric Imaging, attention), was related to performance on the other five cognitive Neurocognition, and Genetics (PING) study (Jernigan et al., 2016). measures. In the original validation study, the correlations between As expected, age accounted for a large portion of the variability in these executive function measures and the NTCB receptive vocab- scores. Overall results were very similar to those reported for a ulary measure (Picture Vocabulary Test) were lower in the 8- to smaller sample of children and adolescents in the validation study 15-year-olds compared with the 3- to 6-year-olds (Zelazo et al., (Weintraub et al., 2013). 2013). We predicted that, while cognitive flexibility (the Dimen- Performance on some executive function measures improves sional Change Card Sort) would correlate with inhibitory control rapidly with age in young children while performance on “higher (the Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test) in all age order” or more complex tasks does not peak until adolescence or groups examined, each would correlate with other tests in younger early adulthood, particularly those requiring impulse control or the groups but correlate increasingly less in older age groups, indicat- “cognitive control system” (Casey, Giedd, & Thomas, 2000). ing greater differentiation. We also predicted that exploratory However, few published behavioral studies to date have charac- factor analyses would show a single factor for younger age groups, terized the developmental time course of the same set of executive indicating that younger children’s performance across the NTCB function tasks across the full age range over which they are measures was relatively undifferentiated, but that similar factor believed to mature—namely, from preschool ages into young analyses for older age groups would show two or more factors, adulthood (Best & Miller, 2010). Certain elements of task perfor- indicating greater differentiation of cognitive abilities with age. mance may reflect a broader array of fundamental skills in younger children. Executive functions are characterized by both unity and Method diversity (Teuber, 1972). Adult studies have shown both a task- domain general performance factor that cannot be explained by Participants general cognitive abilities (unity) as well as task-domain specific factors (diversity; Miyake & Friedman, 2012; Miyake et al., 2000). The 1,020 participants in this study (486 females and 534 males Studies of executive functions in younger children support a single aged 3–21 years) are the same as those recruited for an earlier unitary factor (Nelson et al., 2016; Wiebe, Espy, & Charak, 2008; study reported in Akshoomoff et al. (2014). All participants were Wiebe et al., 2011) with emergence of diversity beginning in recruited through local postings and outreach activities conducted middle childhood (Huizinga, Dolan, & van der Molen, 2006). in the greater metropolitan areas of Baltimore, Boston, Honolulu, Executive functions may become increasingly differentiated Los Angeles, New Haven, New York, Sacramento, and San Diego. from other cognitive functions with development as the

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    7 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us