Decremental—Not Relative— Deprivation Motivates

Decremental—Not Relative— Deprivation Motivates

Title: Resisting Changes That Make Things Worse: Decremental—Not Relative— Deprivation Motivates Political Violence Authors: Henrikas Bartusevičius,1 Florian van Leeuwen2 Affiliations: 1Peace Research Institute Oslo, Hausmanns gate 3, 0186 Oslo, Norway. 2Department of Social Psychology, Tilburg University, Professor Cobbenhagenlaan 225, 5037 DB, Tilburg, Netherlands. Corresponding author: Henrikas Bartusevičius. Email: [email protected] Word Count: 10,339 (11,989 including references) Please note that this manuscript has not yet been peer reviewed Abstract. Despite extensive scholarly interest in the association between economic inequality and political violence, the micro-level mechanisms through which the former influences the latter remain not well understood. Drawing on pioneering theories of political violence, social psychological research on relative deprivation, and prospect theory, we examine individual-level processes underpinning the relationship between inequality and political violence. We present two arguments: despite being a key explanatory variable in existing research, lower economic status vis-à-vis other individuals (relative deprivation) is unlikely to motivate people to participate in political violence; by contrast, although virtually unexplored, a projected decrease in one’s own economic status (prospective decremental deprivation) is likely to motivate political violence. Multilevel analyses of nationally representative samples from many African countries provide evidence to support these claims. Based on this, we posit that focusing on changes in living conditions, rather than the status quo, is key to understanding political violence. 1 1 Introduction What motivates people to participate in political violence1? Over the last decade, scholars have shown a particular interest in grievance-based explanations of political violence (Cederman, Gleditsch, and Buhaug 2013; Østby 2013). A large portion of this research has analyzed grievances associated with economic inequality. In contrast to much prior work, more recent studies have demonstrated that economic inequality—when appropri- ately operationalized—relates to various forms of violence, including large-scale civil con- flict (Cederman, Gleditsch, and Buhaug 2013; Østby 2013). While state- or group-level analyses have extensively analyzed the empirical association between inequality and vi- olence, we know less about the individual-level processes through which one influences the other (see also Bartusevičius 2019; Dyrstad and Hillesund 2020; Hillesund 2015; Koos 2018; Miodownik and Nir 2016; Rustad 2016). Here, we examine the widely held claim that inequality motivates political violence. Ever since the publication of Why Men Rebel (Gurr 1970/2011), many scholars have assumed that economically disadvantaged individuals are predisposed to being mobilized for violent collective action. To date, this remains an undertheorized and empirically underexplored claim. Notably, extant empirical research has mainly focused on static inequalities. However, pioneering theoretical work considered changes in living condi- tions—not the status quo—as key to understanding violence (Davies 1962; Gurr 1970/2011). Indeed, in his seminal work, Gurr claimed that persistent relative deprivation should lead to psychological adjustment, not violence, stressing the need to account for dynamic forms of deprivation caused by changes in living conditions or people’s expec- tations about them (1970/2011, 46–56). This emphasis on dynamics is consistent with individual-level studies in other disciplines. Social psychological research indicates only a 1 We use “political violence” to refer to a broader construct of coalitional violence for a political cause, which subsumes various more specific types of violence, such as “civil conflict” or “non-state conflict”. We return to definitional/operational issues below. 1 weak association between individual relative deprivation and participation in collective actions (Smith et al. 2012), and behavioral economics demonstrates that risk-seeking is related to prospective losses rather than prospective gains (e.g., resources acquired from the advantaged) (Kahneman and Tversky 1984). Although psychologists have extensively analyzed relative deprivation, they have rarely linked it to participation in political violence, focusing instead on non-violent col- lective actions (van Zomeren et al. 2008). Furthermore, psychological research has pri- marily relied on samples from Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and developed (WEIRD) populations (Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan 2010). By contrast, while polit- ical scientists have extensively analyzed the empirical association between inequality and violence, both in WEIRD and non-WEIRD countries, they have rarely tested individual- level theories against individual-level data (for a critique of this, see Dyrstad and Hil- lesund 2020; Hillesund 2015; Koos 2018; Miodownik and Nir 2016; Pettigrew 2015; 2016; Rustad 2016). Here, we present an analysis of large multinational survey data from African coun- tries. Consistent with the theoretical focus on temporal dynamics, we find no empirical support for the claim that perceived inequality vis-à-vis other individuals motivates vio- lence. By contrast, we find that a projected decrease in one’s own economic status moti- vates violence. We utilize behavioral-intentional measures to alleviate endogeneity con- cerns, and a large set of questions to assess alternative explanations. Drawing on a long tradition in applied economics (Altonji, Elder, and Taber 2005; Oster 2019), we also estimate the risk of omitted-variable bias, finding that unobserved confounders are un- likely to drive our results. Our findings also withstand various sensitivity tests, including those addressing reporting problems and using alternative data. Altogether, this article makes the following contributions. Theoretically, we renovate a five-decade-old theory of relative deprivation and political violence—routinely invoked in contemporary conflict research—with insights from recent psychological work and be- 2 havioral economics. Our study aimed both to test existing theory and to build new prop- ositions. Specifically, we aimed to assess the general argument by Gurr (1970/2011) that dynamic forms of deprivation, compared to static forms, are more important for violence. Subsequently, we specified and further developed this early theoretical work by integrat- ing insights from recent individual-level research in other disciplines, producing novel propositions that go beyond Gurr’s. Empirically, we show that perceptions of disadvantageous economic changes, but not individual-based inequalities, significantly relate to motivations to engage in violence. Macro-level studies have shown that inequalities between groups, compared to those between individuals, better predict civil conflict (e.g., Cederman, Gleditsch, and Buhaug 2013; Østby 2013); consequently, some scholars have called for a shift in focus from individual- to group-based grievances. Our micro-level findings suggest that group-based grievances are indeed associated with violent motivations, corroborating macro-level re- search. However, we find that certain forms of individual-based grievances also signifi- cantly relate to motivations to participate in violence. Instead of shifting focus away from individual-based variables, we suggest that macro-level research re-center on temporal economic dynamics, whether pertaining to individuals or groups. This suggestion has implications for conflict research writ large. It points to the central role of temporal dynamics, as contrasted to static features, in the processes leading to violence. More generally, our study contributes to an emerging effort to integrate psychological perspectives into the study of macro-level political phenomena (Kertzer and Tingley 2018), and the scrutiny of individual-level theories with individual-level data. Although commonly invoking theories of individual motivations, research on civil conflict has typ- ically focused on states or groups as units of analysis. Directly investigating individual motivations, we find that one key theoretical account underlying a substantial portion of conflict research needs to be revised, opening a range of new research avenues. 3 2 Assumptions in Existing Research Early studies of political violence argued that inequalities in income or land can motivate individuals to participate in violence challenging the status quo distribution of resources (Nagel 1974; Russet 1964; Sigelman and Simpson 1977). Recent studies added that ine- qualities overlapping with ethnic groups (horizontal inequalities) may also aid the mobi- lization of the disadvantaged for collective action (Buhaug, Cederman, and Gleditsch 2014; Cederman, Weidman, and Gleditsch 2011; Gurr 2000; Østby 2008; 2013; Stewart 2008). Ethnic groups share a common social identity and horizontal inequalities can strengthen the salience of these identities (Gurr 2000, 66–69; Østby 2008, 147). Salient identities, in turn, can facilitate the mobilization of solitary individuals for collective action (Gurr 2000, 75). While emphasizing mobilization, studies of horizontal inequalities have not objected the claim that inequalities motivate violence. That is, this research has accentuated con- ditions that facilitate violence (e.g., inequality aligned with particular identity groups), while still assuming that inequalities, whether individual or group, motivate violence.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    87 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us