Teasing Apart Crypsis and Aposematism – Evidence

Teasing Apart Crypsis and Aposematism – Evidence

Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2015, , –. With 5 figures. Teasing apart crypsis and aposematism – evidence that disruptive coloration reduces predation on a noxious toad MATTHEW T. MCELROY1,2,3* 1Department of Biology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA 2Burke Museum of Natural History and Culture, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA 3Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, Box 2072, Balboa, Ancon, Panama Received 8 June 2015; revised 9 July 2015; accepted for publication 12 July 2015 Both cryptic and aposematic colour patterns can reduce predation risk to prey. These distinct strategies may not be mutually exclusive, because the impact of prey coloration depends on a predator’s sensory system and cognition and on the environmental background. Determining whether prey signals are cryptic or aposematic is a prerequisite for understanding the ecological and evolutionary implications of predator–prey interactions. This study investigates whether coloration and pattern in an exceptionally polymorphic toad, Rhinella alata, from Barro Colorado Island, Panama reduces predation via background matching, disruptive coloration, and/or aposematic signaling. When clay model replicas of R. alata were placed on leaf litter, the model’s dorsal pattern – but not its colour – affected attack rates by birds. When models were placed on white paper, patterned and un-patterned replicas had similar attack rates by birds. These results indicate that dorsal patterns in R. alata are functionally cryptic and emphasize the potential effectiveness of disruptive coloration in a vertebrate taxon. © 2015 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2015, 00, 000–000. ADDITIONAL KEYWORDS: aposematic – background matching – Barro Colorado Island – Bufo typhonius – camouflague – cryptic coloration – predation – predator – prey – Rhinella margaritifera. INTRODUCTION ently conspicuous. For instance, prey may benefit from disruptive coloration in scenarios where they Coloration of cryptic organisms prevents or reduces are unable to background match (Schaefer & Stobbe, their exposure to predators by a variety of mecha- 2006; Stevens et al., 2006). Alternatively, bright col- nisms (Ruxton, Sherratt & Speed, 2004), but two our patterns may represent sexual signals for con- main strategies are background matching and dis- specifics but disruptive signals for predators (Toledo ruptive coloration. In background matching, dorsal & Haddad, 2009). Finally, conspicuous colour pat- markings that match the colour and pattern of sub- terns may represent aposematic signals for predators strates reduce detection of prey by predators, with colour vision, but disruptive coloration for col- whereas in disruptive coloration, contrasting pat- our blind predators (Brodie & Tumbarello, 1978; Bro- terns and false edges break up the outline of the die, 1993a; Brodie & Janzen, 1995; Wuster€ et al., prey’s body shape and hinder recognition by preda- 2004; Toledo & Haddad, 2009). Distinguishing tors (Stevens & Merilaita, 2009). In some instances, between background matching and disruptive col- disruptive coloration may affect recognition but not oration can be challenging, especially since both pro- detection, such that predators may be able to distin- cesses can occur synergistically to maximize crypsis guish prey from the background but are unable to (Stevens & Cuthill, 2006). However, background recognize prey for what they actually are (Stevens matching predicts that both colour and pattern are et al., 2006). Therefore, disruptive coloration may important for reducing predation, while disruptive provide cryptic protection even when prey are appar- coloration predicts that pattern but not necessarily colour is important. Although the concept of disrup- *E-mail: [email protected] tive coloration has been considered a widespread © 2015 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2015, , – 1 2 M. T. MCELROY anti-predator defence for over a century (Thayer, Bufo typhonius; Rhinella margaritifera). This species 1909; Cott, 1940; Ruxton et al., 2004), its effective- is a diurnal, sit-and-wait forager and has a remark- ness has been demonstrated experimentally only able polymorphism in colour and pattern that is gen- within the last decade (Cuthill et al., 2005; Stevens erally assumed to represent cryptic coloration & Cuthill, 2006; Fraser et al., 2007; Cuthill & (Fig. 1A). When attacked, Rhinella toads excrete Szekely, 2009). parotoid secretions containing steroids such as bufo- In contrast to crypsis, aposematism involves a con- genines and bufotoxins (Toledo & Jared, 1995; Jared spicuous appearance that advertises chemical or et al., 2009). These steroids can affect vertebrates structural defences of prey, leading to innate or such as snakes and mammals and may cause cardiac learned predator avoidance (Ruxton et al., 2004; Fra- abnormalities, localized paralysis, convulsions, and ser et al., 2007). Although bright and conspicuous even death (Sciani et al., 2013). Little information is coloration may promote the evolution of aposematic known about the predators of R. alata, but snakes signals (Saporito et al., 2007; Noonan & Comeault, likely represent a primary predator (e.g. Liophus 2009), it is not necessarily required. Aposematism sp.; Kollartis et al., 2013). White-nosed coatis (Nasua may occur if predators can associate dull but charac- narica) occasionally eat cane toads (R. marinus; teristic prey phenotypes with a prey’s defence (Sher- Gomppert, 1996) and are able to remove toxic secre- rat & Beatty, 2003; Wuster€ et al., 2004; Niskanen & tions from prey by rolling them in the dirt (Sazima Mappes, 2005). Although aposematism and crypsis & Caramaschi, 1986; Jared et al., 2009), and there- appear to represent opposite strategies for reducing fore may be able to consume R. alata despite its predation, they may not be mutually exclusive – pre- chemical defence. Rhinella alata is unpalatable to sumed cryptic patterns may function as aposematic bats (Page et al., 2012), and despite being an abun- signals (Wuster€ et al., 2004; Niskanen & Mappes, dant leaf litter anuran in Panama it is not eaten by 2005), and vice versa (Brodie & Tumbarello, 1978), birds (Poulin et al., 2001). It is unclear whether depending on the predator sensory system and on birds avoid attacking R. alata entirely, or whether the background (Stevens, 2007). For instance, bright attacks are abandoned following exposure to sec- patterning may represent an aposematic signal ondary chemical defences. In order to understand towards avian predators with colour vision, but func- the evolutionary ecology of predator–prey dynamics tion as cryptic coloration towards colour-blind preda- in this system it is necessary to determine whether tors. Furthermore, colour patterns may combine R. alata coloration functions as a cryptic signal, an crypsis and aposematism in a distance-dependent aposematic signal, or both. Here I manipulate the fashion, such that animals are cryptic from a dis- colour and pattern of clay model replicas of R. alata tance but aposematic at close proximities (Tullberg, and then determine whether and how different col- Merilaita & Wiklund, 2005). ours and patterns affect attack rates of visually The goal of this study is to determine whether oriented avian predators. Next, I tease apart three specific dorsal colours and/or patterns reduce preda- competing, but not mutually exclusive, hypotheses – tion on a leaf litter toad, Rhinella alata (formally that predation is reduced via: (1) background AB Population pattern frequency Clay model replicas Solid: 36% Striped: 48% Diamond: 16% Figure 1. A, Representative R. margaritifera phenotypes. Population frequencies grouped by pattern based on samples of 153 wild-caught individuals. B, Clay model replicas used in the study. © 2015 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2015, , – DISRUPTIVE COLORATION IN A TROPICAL TOAD 3 matching, (2) disruptive coloration, and/or (3) apose- types of clay and all toads. Achromatic colour con- matic signaling. trast is thought to be important for avian discrimina- tion of objects (Kelber, Vorobyev & Osorio, 2003). Colour contrasts are reports in ‘just-noticeable-differ- ences’ (JND). Two stimuli with JND < 1 are consid- MATERIAL AND METHODS ered indistinguishable, with stimuli becoming CLAY MODEL REPLICAS increasingly distinguishable as JND increases above Rhinella alata is represented by a variety of colour/ 1 (Siddiqi et al., 2004; Stoddard & Stevens, 2011). pattern phenotypes that are found on Barro Colorado Here I use the following interpretations for JND of Island (BCI), Panama. I made models from non-hard- two stimuli: indistinguishable when JND < 1; barely ening clay (Sargent Art) that represent the range of distinguishable when 1 ≤ JND < 2; and easily distin- toad colours (‘light’, ‘medium’, and ‘dark’ brown) and guishable when 2 ≤ JND. each of three dorsal patterns (‘solid’, ‘striped’, and ‘diamond’) (Fig. 1). Clay model replicas provide an attack record in the plasticine body and have been LEAF LITTER EXPERIMENT widely used in a variety of field studies to model In the first experiment, 36 clay models (four each of snakes (Brodie, 1993a), frogs (Saporito et al., 2007), nine morphs) were randomly ordered and placed on mice (Vignieri, Larson & Hoekstra, 2010), and but- the leaf litter 5 m apart along 45 transects. Tran- terflies (Ihalainen & Lindstedt, 2012). Clay models sects were placed perpendicular to

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    10 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us