Psychological Assessment Copyright 1993 by the American Psychological Association, Inc 1993, Vol. 5, No. 3, 336-342 1040-3590/93/S3.00 The Thought Disorder Index: A Reliability Study Michael J. Coleman, J. Tyler Carpenter, Christine Waternaux, Deborah L. Levy, Martha E. Shenton, Jonathan Perry, David Medoff, Hazel Wong, Dara Monoach, Patricia Meyer, Carita O'Brian, Camille Valentino, Delbert Robinson, Melissa Smith, David Makowski, and Philip S. Holzman This article reports interrater reliability among 4 teams of raters who independently evaluated thought disorder in 20 Rorschach protocols using the Thought Disorder Index (TDI). Intraclass correlation coefficients were calculated to assess the degree of association among the 4 teams for total thought disorder scores, severity levels, and qualitative thought disorder factors. Highly ac- ceptable agreement was obtained. Spearman rank order correlation coefficients for these same variables were significant for all possible pairings of teams. A repeated-measures analysis of vari- ance indicated that the absolute amount of thought disorder tagged by each team differed even though the teams' relative rankings of thought disorder among subjects was very similar. Such scoring differences reflect individual differences in threshold for detecting deviant thinking. Thought disorder has long been regarded as a hallmark of guage, and Communication (TLC; Andreasen, 1979,1986); the schizophrenia. Kraepelin (1896/1919) described the disor- thought disorder assessment scale devised by Harrow and col- dered thought processes of schizophrenia as "derailments." leagues (Harrow & Quinlan, 1985; Marengo, Harrow, Lanin- Bleuler (1911/1950, 1924) wrote that the central feature of Kettering, & Wilson, 1986); and the Thought Disorder Index schizophrenic thought was "loosening of associations," which (TDI; Johnston & Holzman, 1979; Solovay et al., 1986). The included associations by contiguity, bizarre and clang associa- psychometric properties of these scales, including their in- tions, as well as condensations of ideas and incoherent dis- terrater reliability, are a necessary requirement for evaluating course. Subsequent investigations have confirmed the presence these scales. This article reports a study of interrater reliability of these disorders of thinking in schizophrenic conditions but assessments for the TDI. have also identified thought disorder in patients with affective The TDI is a multidimensional system for tagging, classify- psychoses, organic brain diseases, and borderline personality ing, and measuring disturbances in thinking. It provides both a disorder (Andreasen & Powers, 1974; Cameron, 1944; Carlson qualitative and quantitative inventory of cognitive slippage. The & Goodwin, 1973; Clayton, Pitts, & Winokur, 1965; Edell, TDI emerged from the work of Rapaport, Gill, and Schafer 1987; Gershon, Benson, & Frazier, 1974; Goldstein, 1944; (1945/1968), who developed a set of scoring categories to iden- Harrow & Quinlan, 1977; Johnston & Holzman, 1979; O'Con- tify thinking disturbances in psychotic and neurotic adults. nell, Cooper, Perry, & Hoke, 1989; Shenton, Solovay, & Holz- These scoring categories were later refined by Watkins and man, 1987; Solovay, Shenton, & Holzman, 1987). Stauffacher (1952), who fashioned them into a quantitative The study of thought disturbances in psychotic patients has scoring system, the "Delta Index." The Delta Index yielded se- spawned the development of objective scales for the measure- verity-weighted scores that were summed and divided by the ment of thought disorder. Several such scales are in common number of Rorschach responses to yield a quantitative index of use, including the Scale for the Assessment of Thought, Lan- thought disorder. Johnston and Holzman (1979) further revised this system, eliminating unreliable scoring categories and add- ing other categories to the system. Subsequent refinements have also been made (Solovay et al., 1986). Michael J. Coleman, Harvard Medical School/McLean Hospital; J. Tyler Carpenter, Harvard University/McLean Hospital; Christine The TDI has been used in several different studies to (a) Waternaux and Deborah L. Levy, Harvard Medical School/McLean discriminate among schizophrenic, manic, and schizoaffective Hospital; Martha E. Shenton, Harvard Medical School/Brockton Vet- patients on the basis of qualitative differences in thought dis- erans Affairs Hospital; Jonathan Perry, Harvard University/McLean order (Holzman, Solovay, & Shenton, 1985; Shenton, Solovay, & Hospital; David Medoff and Hazel Wong, McLean Hospital; Dara Holzman, 1987; Solovay, Shenton, & Holzman, 1987); (b) detect Monoach and Patricia Meyer, Harvard University/McLean Hospital; thought disorder in patients with borderline and schizotypal Carita O'Brian, McLean Hospital; Camille Valentino, Delbert Robin- personality disorders (Edell, 1987); (c) predict psychotic symp- son, Melissa Smith, and David Makowski, Hillside Hospital (adivision toms in patients with personality and affective disorders of Long Island Jewish Medical Center); and Philip S. Holzman, Har- (O'Connell, Cooper, Perry, & Hoke, 1989); (d) measure neuro- vard University, Harvard Medical School, and McLean Hospital. leptic-induced reduction of thought disorder in schizophrenic This study was supported by PHS grants MH 31154 and MH 31350. patients (Hurt, Holzman, & Davis, 1983; Spohn et al., 1986); (e) We gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Clara Lajonchere and Jared Cruz of the Psychology Laboratory at McLean Hospital. identify unique patterns of thought disorder in patients with Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mi- unilateral right hemisphere cortical damage (Daniels et al., chael J. Coleman, Psychology Laboratory, McLean Hospital, 115 Mill 1988); (f) detect the often subtle cognitive slippage that occurs Street, Belmont, Massachusetts 02178. in the unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenic, manic, 336 TDI RELIABILITY 337 and schizoaffective patients (Shenton, Solovay, Holzman, Cole- Table 1 man, & Gale, 1989); and (g) characterize the amounts and Thought Disorder Index Scoring Categories by Level of Severity kinds of thought disorder shown by children who were hospital- .25 level ized for psychotic and behavioral symptoms, children whose mothers had been hospitalized for psychotic symptoms, and 1. Inappropriate distance a. Increase of distance normal children (Arboleda & Holzman, 1985). The TDI has b. Excessive qualification been shown to be unrelated to race and socioeconomic status c. Concreteness (Haimo & Holzman, 1979; Johnston & Holzman, 1979). d. Overspecificity In principle, any verbal sample can be assessed using the e. Syncretistic response TDI, although it is most commonly applied to responses to 2. Flippant Rorschach cards (Rorschach, 1921/1942). The Rorschach Test 3. Vagueness is advantageous for two reasons. First, it seems to elicit more 4. Peculiar verbalization instances of thought disorder than such nonprojective tests as 5. Word-finding difficulty the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Johnston & Holzman, 6. Clang 1979; Spohn et al., 1986). Second, TDI scores based on the 7. Perseveration Rorschach Test are uncorrelated with IQ (Johnston & Holz- 8. Incongruous combination man, 1979), whereas those based on the WAIS are significantly a. Composite responses but modestly and negatively correlated with IQ (Gold & Hurt, b. Arbitrary form-color 1990; Johnston & Holzman, 1979). c. Inappropriate activity Previously, Johnston and Holzman (1979) reported interrater .50 level reliability among two raters for total TDI scores. Pearson r ranged from .82 to .93. Solovay, Shenton, and Holzman (1987), 8. d. Internal-external response also using two judges in the same laboratory, reported in- 9. Relationship verbalization terrater reliability for total TDI scores of .89, individual scoring 10. Idiosyncratic symbolism categories of .81, severity levels of .79, and various factor scores a. Color symbolism b. Image symbolism ranging from .84 to .89. We report here a systematic study of 11. Queer response interrater reliability on all features of TDI scoring, using four a. Queer expression rating teams at three separate sites, who independently scored b. Queer word usage the same Rorschach responses using the TDI. c. Queer imagery 12. Confusion 13. Looseness Method 14. Fabulized combination Instrument 15. Playful confabulation 16. Fragmentation The TDI distinguishes 23 qualitative categories of thought distur- bance, which are weighted along a continuum of severity (.25, .50, .75, .75 level and 1.0), with the .25 level representing very mild forms and the 1.00 17. Fluidity level reflecting the most severe forms of thought disorder. The TDI categories and severity levels are listed in Table 1. The inventory in 18. Absurd response Table 1 makes no claim to completeness; it does, however, comprise 19. Confabulation most types of thought disorder encountered—with greater or lesser a. Details in one area generalized to a larger area frequency—in the examination of psychotic patients. The TDI manual b. Extreme elaboration contains more complete descriptions of the categories as well as the 20. Autistic logic psychometric characteristics of the validation studies (Johnston & Holzman, 1979; Solovay et al., 1986). 1.00 level The total thought disorder score is computed as the sum of the fre- 21. Contamination quency of each instance of thought disorder, multiplied by its category 22. Incoherence weight (e.g., .25, .50, .75, or 1.0), divided by
Details
-
File Typepdf
-
Upload Time-
-
Content LanguagesEnglish
-
Upload UserAnonymous/Not logged-in
-
File Pages7 Page
-
File Size-