The Biology of Time Across Different Scales

The Biology of Time Across Different Scales

COMMENTARY The biology of time across different scales Dean V Buonomano Animals time events on scales that span from microseconds to days. In contrast to the technologies devised by humans to keep track of time, biology has developed vastly different mechanisms for timing across these different scales. For both individuals and society as a whole, the or the emergent behavior of large networks of avoid predators. One of the mechanisms under- ability to precisely track and tell time is critical neurons—lack the digital precision of modern lying sound localization relies on the ability to across scales spanning over 15 orders of magni- clocks. Second, the features required of a bio- discriminate very short temporal intervals3. In tude: from the nanosecond accuracy of atomic logical timer vary depending on whether its humans it takes sound approximately 600 µs clocks used for global positioning systems to function is to process speech, anticipate when a to travel the distance between the left ear and the tracking of our yearly trip around the sun. traffic light will change, or control the circadian the right ear. In barn owls this takes approxi- In-between these extremes we track the min- fluctuations in sleep-wake cycles. mately 160 µs, and they can detect differences of utes and hours that govern our daily activities. The mechanisms biology has exploited to tell approximately 10 µs (ref. 4). The ability to detect It is noteworthy that the same technology can time provide insights not only into the function be used to measure time across these different and importance of timing on different scales, scales; atomic clocks are used to time nanosec- but also into the relative limits and flexibility 10–6 ond delays in the arrival of signals from different of different strategies. Based on the presumed satellites and to make adjustments to the calen- underlying mechanisms and on didactic conven- dar year. Similarly, digital wristwatches are used ience, we can categorize timing into four dif- 10–5 to time hundredths of a second and the days of ferent timescales: microseconds, milliseconds, Sound localization the month. seconds and circadian rhythms (Fig. 1). –4 In nature, animals also keep track of time over 10 an equally impressive range of scales: from tens Microsecond timing 1 ms of microseconds, used for sound localization, The fastest scale of temporal processing occurs 10–3 to the anticipation of yearly seasonal changes, on the order of microseconds and allows ani- as well as the control of longer physiological mals to determine the interval it takes sound events such as puberty and menopause. It is in- to travel from one ear to the other. For many 0.01 Motion detection between these extremes that arguably the most vertebrates, localizing the source of a sound is sophisticated forms of timing occur. It is on the essential to survival. For example, barn owls scale of milliseconds and seconds that complex use sound localization to hunt rodents in the 0.1 Speech recognition forms of sensory and motor processing, which dark; rodents in turn use sound localization to Motor coordination include speech recognition and motor coordi- 1s 1 Music perception nation, take place1,2. The mechanisms by which Figure 1 Scales of biological timing. Humans Seconds animals tell time remain incompletely under- process temporal information over a wide range stood. Nevertheless, in contrast to human-made of timescales. On one extreme we detect the 10 timing devices, it is clear that the biological delay required for sound to travel from one ear 1 min solutions to telling time are fundamentally dif- to the other; these delays are on the order of Subjective time ferent across different timescales. The fact that tens to hundreds of microseconds. On the other 100 Time estimation there are numerous biological solutions to the extreme, we have daily physiological oscillations, such as our sleep-wake cycle. In-between these problem of telling time likely reflects two fac- extremes temporal processing occurs on the scale 103 tors. First, the biological components—be they of milliseconds and seconds. This intermediate 1h biochemical reactions occurring within a cell range is critical to sensory processing, motor coordination, and our subjective sense of time 104 that governs our daily activities. It is generally Appetite believed that timing in the range of tens to Dean V. Buonomano is in the Departments of 1d Sleep-wake cycles hundreds of milliseconds relies on mechanisms Neurobiology and Psychology, Brain Research 105 distinct from those responsible for timing seconds Institute, University of California, Los Angeles, and hours. However, where the temporal boundary Box 951761, Los Angeles, California 90095, USA. lies and how many neural mechanisms contribute 106 e-mail: [email protected] to timing within these ranges is not known. Ris-Vicari Katie 594 VOLUME 3 NUMBER 10 OCTOBER 2007 NATURE CHEMICAL BIOLOGY COMMENTARY such short intervals is surprising in a system in 3 mm ms–1 (ref. 5); thus, a 300-µm distance strategy is that some neurons are able to detect which even the fastest events, such as an action can serve as a 100-µs delay. In the same man- the simultaneous occurrence of inputs from the potential, last a few hundred microseconds. The ner that the intersection of two cars that started left and right ears. This is termed ‘coincidence biological solution to detecting short intervals at fixed points heading toward each other pro- detection’ and is something neurons are inher- lies in using the time it takes action potentials vides a measure of their relative starting times, ently well designed to do. Although they are an to propagate down an axon as a physical delay the brain uses the intersection point between effective mechanism for detecting extremely line. Axons in the auditory brain stem conduct action potentials coming from the different ears short intervals, axonal delay lines evolved to action potentials at a speed of approximately to determine which started first. The key to this solve the highly specialized problem of sound Box 1 Interval selectivity in simple neural circuits Theoretical and experimental work suggests that neurons may be connections in parallel it is possible to tune the Ex unit to respond inherently capable of telling time on the scale of tens to hundreds selectively to either interval. Specifically, with relatively weak inputs of milliseconds as a result of the interaction between short-term to both the Ex and Inh cells (red traces), the Ex neuron can respond synaptic plasticity and circuit dynamics. In Figure 2a, a computer selectively to the 50-ms interval, thus functioning as an interval de- simulation shows how interval-selective neurons can be generated tector. Further increases in the excitatory synaptic weights can shift from a simple neural circuit (right) composed of an excitatory neuron the selectivity to 100 (green) or 200 (blue) ms, in the absence of (Ex) and an inhibitory neuron (Inh). There are five synapses: Input any changes in the temporal properties of the synapses or neurons. → Ex, Input → Inh, Inhfast → Ex, Inhslow → Ex and Inhslow → Inh. Figure 2b shows interval selectivity as a function of ‘synapse space’. The excitatory synapses exhibit paired-pulse facilitation (the second The strength of the Input → Ex and Input → Inh were parametrically excitatory postsynaptic potential is stronger than the first). The three varied over a range of weights. The results are represented as an sets of traces to the left (red, green and blue) represent the voltage RGB plot, which permits visualization of the selectivity to the three traces from the Ex and Inh cells for three different sets of synaptic intervals while varying two dimensions. As color-coded in Figure 2a, strengths (the sharp high-amplitude deflections represent action po- red represents regions of synapse space in which the Ex unit fires ex- tentials, whereas the longer low-amplitude events are subthreshold clusively to the second pulse of a 50-ms interpulse interval (IPI), but excitatory postsynaptic potentials). Each group consists of three sim- not to the 100- or 200-ms IPI; that is, red represents the Ex unit as ulations (overlaid traces) in response to a 50-, 100- and 200-ms in- a 50-ms interval detector. Similarly, green and dark blue areas repre- terval. By changing the strengths of the Input → Ex and Input → Inh sent regions of synapse space in which the Ex units respond only to the 100- or 200-ms interval, respectively. All other colors represent the summation of the primary colors (see color cube inset); thus, a white areas correspond to regions that respond to all the intervals Inp Ex but not to the first pulse. Black areas represent regions in which the Ex cell was not interval selective—not firing at all or firing only in re- Ex sponse to the first pulse. The three unfilled white squares show the Inh Ex areas of synapse space of the traces in Figure 2a. Inp Inh Inh Figure 2 Temporal processing on the millisecond timescale. (a) Computer Inh simulation showing how interval-selective neurons can be generated from a simple neural circuit. (b) Interval selectivity as a function of synapse Input space. Modified from ref. 21. b Ex 0.16 Inh 0.14 0.12 Ex 0.10 Synaptic weight Inp Inh (µs) Inh 0.08 50 0.4 0.45 0.6 100 0.5 0.55 200 ms Synaptic weight Inp Ex (mS) NATURE CHEMICAL BIOLOGY VOLUME 3 NUMBER 10 OCTOBER 2007 595 COMMENTARY localization. There is little evidence that similar a mechanisms are used to time longer intervals, or even that such a delay line strategy is sufficiently Gene flexible or powerful to underlie other forms transcription of timing.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    4 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us