An Item Response Theory and Factor Analytic Examination of Two Prominent Maximizing Tendency Scales

An Item Response Theory and Factor Analytic Examination of Two Prominent Maximizing Tendency Scales

Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 7, No. 5, September 2012, pp. 644–658 An item response theory and factor analytic examination of two prominent maximizing tendency scales Justin M. Weinhardt∗ Brendan J. Morse† Janna Chimeli∗ Jamie Fisher∗ Abstract The current study examines the construct validity of the Maximization Scale (MS; Schwartz et al., 2002) and the Maximization Tendency Scale (MTS; Diab et al., 2008) as well as the nomological net of the maximizing construct. We find that both scales of maximizing suffer psychometrically, especially in their proposed dimensionality. Using confirmatory factor analysis and item response theory (IRT) we identify and remove three problematic items from the MTS and six problematic items from the MS. Additionally, we find that the MS appears to be measuring difficulty and restlessness with the search for the best alternative, whereas the MTS is more focused on the search for the best option, regardless of choice difficulty. We then examined these revised scales in relation to other psychological constructs in the nomological net for maximizing and found that maximizers may not be unhappy but are generally distressed in the decision-making context. Finally, we suggest that future maximizng research use revised form of the MTS that seems to us to be most consistent with the original concept of maximizing/satisficing. Keywords: maximizing, psychometrics, scale-development. 1 Introduction and the development of alternative measures. Diab et al. (2008) developed a new maximization scale (Maximiz- The conceptualization and measurement of the maximiz- ing Tendency Scale; MTS) and found that the MTS did ing construct has received considerable attention in the not correlate with constructs such as depression, life sat- last ten years (Schwartz et al., 2002; Nenkov et al., 2008; isfaction, and neuroticism. However, their scale did cor- Diab, et al., 2008; Lai, 2010; Rim, et al., 2011). Tra- relate positively with regret. Nenkov et al. (2008) modi- ditional economic models of choice theorized that indi- fied the MS and created a 6-item scale that was found to viduals pursue a maximization goal in decision-making have better psychometric characteristics than the original contexts. However, in an evaluation of the existing 13-item scale. Lai (2010) also developed a new scale of data and economic models Simon (1956) stated, “Evi- maximizing and found that it correlated positively with dently, organisms adapt well enough to ‘satisfice’; they optimism and need for cognition, but the correlation be- do not, in general, ‘optimize’ .” Building on this re- tween her measure of maximizing and regret was incon- search, Schwartz et al. attempted to define the psycho- sistent across samples. Finally, Rim et al. (2011) exam- logical effects of maximizing for those who pursue max- ined both the MS and MTS using item response theory imizing goals. Specifically, they theorized that in envi- (IRT) and found that both scales had weakness in mea- ronments with a lot of choice, individuals with a maxi- suring the maximizing construct. They also found that mizing goal would likely be unhappy and regret their de- the MTS was not unidimensional as proposed by Diab et cisions. Schwartz et al. developed a 13-item measure of al. (2008). However, Rim et al. did not discuss remov- maximization (Maximization Scale; MS) and found that ing problematic items. The purpose of the current study scores from the MS correlated positively with depression, is to examine both the MS and MTS using exploratory perfectionism, and regret and correlated negatively with and confirmatory factor analysis, and then use polyto- happiness, life satisfaction, optimism, and self-esteem. mous IRT to resolve the problems found in the scales. In Since the development of this scale, there has been addition, we test whether these solutions can answer the considerable debate about the validity of the measure question of whether maximizers are happy or unhappy. Portions of this research were presented at the 2011 Association for Psychological Science annual convention and the 2011 Judgment 1.1 Construct validity of maximizing and Decision Making annual convention. ∗ Ohio University Schwartz and colleagues (2002) have changed the per- †Department of Psychology, 90 Burril Ave., 340 Hart Hall, Bridge- water State University, Bridgewater, MA, USA, 02325. Email: spective of the maximizing and satisficing constructs [email protected]. by departing from both economic models description of 644 Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 7, No. 5, September 2012 An examination of two maximizing tendency scales 645 maximizing choice strategy (von Neumann & Morgen- These less than optimal psychometric results have stein, 1944), and Simon’s (1955; 1956) view that all de- motivated researchers to re-evaluate the scale measur- cision makers would satisfice in order to adapt to their ing the maximizing/satisficing construct. A particular environment. Schwartz et al.’s revised perspective is re-evaluation of the MS and its psychometric proper- that both maximizing and satisficing represent choice- ties came from Diab and colleagues (Diab et al., 2008). behavior tendencies performed by decision makers de- More specifically, Diab et al. have indicated that there pending on their standing on the maximizing construct. are psychometric and conceptual irregularities with the In addition, Schwartz et al. (2002) focused on the de- MS. First, they indicated that the MS falls short of com- gree to which maximizing is associated with regretting monly accepted psychometric standards. Second, they decisions. They proposed that satisficers and maximizers suggested that there was not a clear connection between differ in their sensitivity to regret because of differences the theory of maximizing and satisficing and the MS. As in investment and goals in the decision making process. reported by Diab et al. (2008), even though the theoret- For maximizers, the potential for regret can increase as a ical basis for the original maximizing scale is Simon’s consequence of two factors. The first is the potential for (1955) definition of maximizing representing the opti- failing to find the best option after spending much time mization goal, many of the items that compose the MS and effort in searching for the very best alternative. The seem to diverge from this definition. For instance, items second is the potential for failing to choose the very best such as having “difficulty writing letters to friends” and option in spite of the amount of available choice in the “preference for ranking things like movies” do not seem market place. Therefore internalizing the failure as re- to fit conceptually with an optimizing goal definition. flecting the decision makers’ inability to optimally make Third, Diab and colleagues argued that the Schwartz et a decision would yield great dissatisfaction. On the other al. (2002) conclusion that the tendency to maximize was hand, satisficers have the goal of finding a good enough correlated with being less happy was a reflection of how alternative that has crossed the decision maker threshold, the construct was measured, and not a reflection of the consequently, the time and effort spent by satisficers dur- construct itself. ing the choice process is much more modest. Thus, sat- Diab et al. (2008) addressed the above criticisms by isficers are likely to experience less dissatisfaction, not developing the MTS. This scale was expected to better only because their investment is modest, but also because represent and measure the constructs of maximizing and their goal does not elicit unrealistic expectations. satisficing. The MTS has three items from the original Schwartz and colleagues performed a series of corre- MS and six new items that tap into the definition of maxi- lational studies to provide evidence for the differentiation mization as an “optimization goal”. Furthermore, Diab et of these two groups (maximizers and satisficers), not only al. examined the correlation between MTS and measures with reference to the choice tendency, but also in relation of indecisiveness, avoidance, regret, neuroticism, and life to a variety of other psychological constructs. The other (dis)satisfaction. Results showed clear differences be- dimensions in the nomological net of maximizing were tween the original MS and the new MTS. First, they subjective happiness, which assesses dispositional happi- found that the MTS demonstrated substantially greater ness (Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999); depression (Beck internal consistency reliability than the MS (MS α = .58; & Beck, 1972); life orientation, which assesses dispo- and MTS α = .80). As predicted, the MTS was largely un- sitional optimism (Scheier & Carver, 1985); satisfaction related to maladaptive personality and decision-making with life (Diener, et al., 1985); dispositional neuroticism constructs. More specifically, MTS did not correlate with (John, et al., 1991); self-oriented perfectionism (Hewitt indecisiveness, avoidance, neuroticism, and life (dis) sat- & Flett, 1990; 1991); and self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965). isfaction, except regret. Although, the correlation be- Findings for the validity of the MS, based on the re- tween tendency to maximize and regret was lower for the lationships between maximizing and the aforementioned MTS (r = .27) than observed for the original MS (r = .45). constructs in the nomological net showed that maximiz- In sum, Diab and colleagues presented a different version ers experience less satisfaction, happiness, optimism, and of maximizing

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    15 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us