The Impact of Predictive Cues and Visual Working Memory on Dynamic Oculomotor Selection Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Matthew D

The Impact of Predictive Cues and Visual Working Memory on Dynamic Oculomotor Selection Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Matthew D

Journal of Vision (2014) 14(3):27, 1–15 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/14/3/27 1 The impact of predictive cues and visual working memory on dynamic oculomotor selection Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Matthew D. Weaver Trento, Italy $ Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Davide Paoletti Trento, Italy $ Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, # Wieske van Zoest Trento, Italy $ Strategic use of advanced information about search any given time, can vary markedly between individuals. display properties can benefit covert attentional Most models of attentional control consider oculomo- selection. However, little work has investigated this tor and visual selection to be determined by the benefit on overt selection. The present study examined interplay between goal-directed processes, representing how cued information impacts oculomotor selection current observer selection goals, and stimulus-driven over time and the role played by individual differences in processes determined by the physical salience of visual working memory (VWM) capacity in utilizing such elements in the visual environment (e.g., Bundesen, cues. Participants searched for a specific orientation 1998; Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Egeth & Yantis, target in a saccade localization search task. Prior to each 1997; Folk, Remington, & Johnston, 1992; Itti & Koch, trial, additional information regarding secondary display 2000; Treisman & Sato, 1990; Wolfe, 1994, 2007; but features (color singleton identity) was either provided by see Awh, Belopolsky, & Theeuwes, 2012). By extension, a word cue or not. The cue increased accuracy performance from the earliest saccadic responses. VWM saccades (i.e., rapid eye movements) used for oculo- capacity was measured via a change-detection task and motor selection are generally considered to result from results showed that individuals’ VWM capacity scores the combination of stimulus-driven and goal-directed were associated with cue impact, whereby participants processes (e.g., Godijn & Theeuwes, 2002). Although it with higher capacity derived an increased cue is accepted that goal-directed strategies can use performance benefit. These findings suggest that advanced information about the properties of a search strategic use of cue information to select and reject display to directly influence covert attentional selection, salient singletons can develop very early following relatively little work has investigated this benefit on display presentation and is related to an individual’s overt saccadic selection. The aim of the present study VWM capacity. This research indicates that stimulus- was twofold: first, to explore how the impact of driven and goal-directed processes are not simply advanced knowledge provided by a preceding cue additive in oculomotor selection, but instead exhibit a develops over time to influence oculomotor selection distinct and dynamic profile of interaction. and second, to investigate the role that individual differences play in utilizing this cue to benefit selection performance. The first focus of the present study specifically Introduction concerned the impact of nonspatial (cf. spatial) information provided by endogenous (cf. exogenous) We are constantly inundated by sensory informa- cues, that is, those cues which require the information tion, which competes for selection from limited- to be used in a goal-directed manner to assist selection. capacity attentional resources. However, the ability to While previous research demonstrates that knowing efficiently and effectively select those subsets of what features to expect can improve visual search information, which are most behaviorally relevant at performance (Mu¨ller, Reimann, & Krummenacher, Citation: Weaver, M. D., Paoletti, D., & van Zoest, W. (2014). The impact of predictive cues and visual working memory on dynamic oculomotor selection. Journal of Vision, 14(3):27, 1–15. http://www.journalofvision.org/content/14/3/27, doi: 10.1167/ 14.3.27. doi: 10.1167/14.3.27 Received December 19, 2013; published March 24, 2014 ISSN 1534-7362 Ó 2014 ARVO Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 09/27/2021 Journal of Vision (2014) 14(3):27, 1–15 Weaver, Paoletti, & van Zoest 2 2003; Wolfe, Horowitz, Kenner, Hyle, & Vasan, 2004), cue itself; see also Theeuwes et al., 2006; Theeuwes & the developmental time-course by which this influence Van der Burg, 2007). The purpose of the present study operates to guide selection remains unclear. Given was to explore the dynamics of how and when sufficient time to apprehend the cue, some research nonspatial information provided by endogenous cues would indicate that advanced nonspatial information impact oculomotor selection during visual search. In should have an immediate or early influence on doing so, we sought to extend existing research by attentional selection (Adams & Chambers, 2012; Bravo examining cue impact across the latency distribution of & Nakayama, 1992; Hillstrom, 2000; Lamy, Carmel, oculomotor responses, using a mixed design that Egeth, & Leber, 2006; Lamy & Yashar, 2008; Leonard allowed measurement of intertrial priming effects. & Egeth, 2008; Mu¨ller & Krummenacher, 2006; Mu¨ller Because eye movements determine the quality of early et al., 2003; Treisman, 1988; Wolfe, Butcher, Lee, & visual information processing, they represent a form of Hyle, 2003; Zhang & Luck, 2009), while other findings behavior more implicit and sensitive than manual would suggest an influence only later, following initial button-press responses, effectively reflecting a real-time stimulus-driven selection processes (Cave & Pashler, measure of the overt allocation of visual attention. 1995; Hochstein & Ahissar, 2002; Kim & Cave, 2001; Recent research on oculomotor selection in visual search Nothdurft, 2002; Theeuwes, Reimann, & Mortier, has highlighted the importance of examining when a 2006; Theeuwes & Van der Burg, 2007, 2011; Tsal & response is made relative to stimulus onset. Early Lavie, 1988; van Zoest, Donk, & Theeuwes, 2004). saccades following a display presentation are more likely In addition, more recently, it has been increasingly to be made to the most physically salient stimuli argued that many findings supporting a role for early regardless of task-relevance (Godijn & Theeuwes, 2002; goal-directed guidance by nonspatial information can Ludwig & Gilchrist, 2002; Mulckhuyse, van Zoest, & also be explained by passive bottom-up intertrial Theeuwes, 2008). As saccade latency increases, so, too, priming (see Awh et al., 2012; Lamy & Kristjansson, does the likelihood that the saccade is directed to the 2013; Theeuwes, 2013, for recent reviews). For example, task-relevant item (Donk & van Zoest, 2008; van Zoest & repeating versus switching a target dimension or feature Donk, 2005, 2008; van Zoest et al., 2004; see also Hickey, value across trials can also lead to significant improve- van Zoest, & Theeuwes, 2010, for complementary results ment in terms of speed and accuracy of visual selection, with covert attentional shifts). Using this approach, van a phenomenon termed priming of pop-out (PoP) Zoest and Donk (2008) interestingly demonstrated a (Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1994). In paradigms where benefit of task instructions on the earliest 20% of advanced knowledge of target properties is manipulated saccadic responses in a search task requiring discrimi- by holding properties constant (‘‘blocked’’) versus nation of targets from unique distractors based on color. varying them (‘‘mixed’’) across trials, any evidence that While these data suggest a potentially early role for goal- attentional settings for a specific target feature can guide directed guidance based on advanced nonspatial infor- attention then becomes confounded with intertrial mation, such conclusions are precluded by use of a repetition effects (e.g., Belopolsky, Schreij, & Theeuwes, blocked (rather than mixed) design which could not rule 2010; Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1994; Pinto, Olivers, & out a bottom-up intertrial priming account of the Theeuwes, 2005). One way to disentangle these effects is findings. By examining saccadic accuracy as a function of by using cues to provide advance knowledge on a trial- saccade latency, the present study will add a further layer by-trial basis. Leonard and Egeth (2008) used a mixed of understanding to the dynamic nature of when and design to compare word cues that either provided 100% how cue information influences selection, providing valid information about an upcoming target color (‘‘red’’ greater scope to reconcile existing findings. or ‘‘green’’) or were noninformative (‘‘either’’) in a The second motivation of the present study was to singleton search, while concurrently measuring impact investigate how individual differences in visual working of repeating versus switching target colors on successive memory (VWM) capacity influences overt selection trials. While similar search performance benefits from performance. VWM is broadly considered to be a both advanced feature cuing and target repetition were capacity-limited cognitive resource involved in the observed, the results indicated these were separable and active ‘‘on-line’’ maintenance of information to per- independent effects. However, along with other research form current tasks (Cowan, 2001). Within the context (Mu¨ller & Krummenacher, 2006; Mu¨ller et al., 2003), of visual search, VWM has been implicated as a crucial these findings contrast with those of Theeuwes and Van mechanism that defines and maintains active represen- der

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    15 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us