Visual Search Under Scotopic Lighting Conditions ⇑ Vivian C

Visual Search Under Scotopic Lighting Conditions ⇑ Vivian C

Vision Research 113 (2015) 155–168 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Visual search under scotopic lighting conditions ⇑ Vivian C. Paulun a, Alexander C. Schütz a, Melchi M. Michel b, Wilson S. Geisler c, Karl R. Gegenfurtner a, a University of Giessen, Department of Experimental Psychology, Otto-Behaghel-Str. 10F, 35394 Gießen, Germany b Rutgers University, Department of Psychology, Piscataway, NJ 08854, United States c University of Texas at Austin, Center for Perceptual Systems and Department of Psychology, Austin, TX 78750, United States article info abstract Article history: When we search for visual targets in a cluttered background we systematically move our eyes around to Received 10 February 2015 bring different regions of the scene into foveal view. We explored how visual search behavior changes Received in revised form 3 May 2015 when the fovea is not functional, as is the case in scotopic vision. Scotopic contrast sensitivity is signif- Available online 16 May 2015 icantly lower overall, with a functional scotoma in the fovea. We found that in scotopic search, for a medium- and a low-spatial-frequency target, individuals made longer lasting fixations that were not Keywords: broadly distributed across the entire search display but tended to peak in the upper center, especially Visual search for the medium-frequency target. The distributions of fixation locations are qualitatively similar to those Scotopic vision of an ideal searcher that has human scotopic detectability across the visual field, and interestingly, these Scotopic sensitivity Eye movements predicted distributions are different from those predicted by an ideal searcher with human photopic detectability. We conclude that although there are some qualitative differences between human and ideal search behavior, humans make principled adjustments in their search behavior as ambient light level decreases. Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Stone, 2001; Findlay, 1997; Motter & Belky, 1998; Najemnik & Geisler, 2005, 2008, 2009; Wolfe, 2007; Wolfe & Gencarz, 1996). Looking for a specific object in the environment is an everyday Guiding the fovea to different locations in the visual field in task. Searching for a known face in a crowd, some pen on a clut- order to find a target object seems an optimal strategy given the tered desk, or – from a more evolutionary perspective – a fruit hid- drastic decrease in resolution toward the periphery. Moreover, den in foliage are only few examples. Visual search is considered to moving the eyes and thus the fovea seems the only way to effec- be one of the most fundamental perceptual tasks (Geisler, Perry, & tively use the given combination of a large field of view with only Najemnik, 2006; Wolfe, 2010). Humans have a large field of view, a small spot of high resolution. However, different natural condi- but visual acuity is not uniformly good across the retina. It shows a tions can have fundamental effects on how target visibility varies maximum within the central 2° of visual angle, and declines con- across the visual field. Due to the distribution of photoreceptors tinuously into the periphery (e.g. Pointer & Hess, 1989). In most in the retina, humans experience a central scotoma when exposed cases the visual scene under consideration is larger than the area to scotopic lighting conditions. Scotopic vision, often also referred that can be inspected during one single fixation. Thus, this area to as night vision, is defined as low-light vision that lies below cone of highest resolution, the fovea, has to be guided to multiple loca- threshold and is thus mediated only by rods, for example under tions in the scene during active visual search in order to find the starlight. Since the fovea solely contains cones, this region with desired object. Humans use saccades, high-speed eye movements, highest resolution under photopic conditions, is not operating to bring the projected images of possible target locations onto the under dim scotopic illumination. This can easily be experienced fovea. The underlying mechanisms (e.g. how fixation locations are when trying to fixate a dim star on the night sky — its image van- selected or how the specific moment of saccade execution is deter- ishes as soon as the fovea lands on it. mined by the visual system) and the efficiency of the search pro- Scotopic visual search must thus rely on peripheral information. cess are a matter of extensive research (e.g. Eckstein, Beutter, & By using gaze-contingent displays it has been shown that search performance deteriorates when vision in the periphery is limited (Geisler, Perry, & Najemnik, 2006; Loschky & McConkie, 2002). This suggests that peripheral information can be used effectively ⇑ Corresponding author. Fax: +49 (0) 641 99 26 119. E-mail address: [email protected] (K.R. Gegenfurtner). in photopic visual search tasks, presumably both to detect the http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2015.05.004 0042-6989/Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 156 V.C. Paulun et al. / Vision Research 113 (2015) 155–168 target and guide the eye movements. However, variations in visi- contrast of the target as well as the size of the visual field under bility within gaze-contingent displays are artificial, whereas the investigation. central scotoma in scotopic vision is natural. It is arguable whether both types are treated similarly by the visual system or whether for 2.1. General methods instance rather explicit but unfamiliar changes in the visibility map affect search behavior in a different manner than natural, previ- 2.1.1. Observers ously experienced but presumably more subtle changes. More Observers were four female students of the University of Gießen importantly, scotopic vision is different from peripheral photopic and one of the authors (VCP). On average they were 21.8 years old vision in that the latter is mediated by cones. As stated above, rods (SD = 1.3). All observers gave their informed consent to participate have a higher luminous sensitivity than cones and they are differ- and were naïve to the aims of the study (except for the author). In ently distributed on the retina. Cones peak in the fovea, where two conditions (low-sf and high-sf) only two observers partici- there are no rods, whereas the rods are more evenly distributed pated (one of them the author and one naïve subject). Visual acuity with a shallow peak at about 20° in the periphery (Curcio et al., of the observers was measured with Landolt rings prior to their 1990). Furthermore, both types of photoreceptors have different participation in the experiment. All five participants had a visual spectral, spatial and temporal characteristics. The spectral sensitiv- acuity of 1.4 and had normal night vision according to subjective ity of rods peaks at about 505 nm (Wyszecki & Stiles, 1982), the reports. For their participation the naïve subjects received either ones of cones peak at about 570 nm, 545 nm, and 445 nm, for money (eight euro per hour) or course credit. The experiments red, green and blue cones respectively (2° fundamentals; were carried out in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the Stockman & Sharpe, 2000). Due to the different pathways that World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and approved the rod and cone signals enter, rod vision is slower than cone by the local ethics committee LEK FB06 at Giessen University (pro- vision; it is delayed by about 67 ms (MacLeod, 1972), though the posal number 2009-0008). actual delay may depend on the adaptation state (Sharpe, Stockman, & MacLeod, 1989). Since rods show a larger neural con- 2.1.2. Apparatus and eye movement recordings vergence than cones the photoreceptors also differ in their spatial Eye positions were recorded with a head-mounted eye tracker sensitivity. On average, rods peak at 0.9 cpd and cones at 2.8 cpd (EyeLink II; SR Research, Osgoode, Ontario, Canada) at a sampling (D’Zmura & Lennie, 1986), but spatial sensitivity also depends on frequency of 500 Hz. Calibration was done prior to every block the eccentricity (Hess, Nordby, & Pointer, 1987). using a 9-fixation-point-procedure. Larger calibration targets were Taken together, having two types of photoreceptors with differ- used in the scotopic conditions to enable subjects to accurately fix- ent luminous sensitivity enables the visual system to work effec- ate its center even if it fades under this viewing condition, because tively under a huge range of illuminations, but it also entails they could use the endpoints as anchors, i.e. either a white cross many other substantial differences between photopic and scotopic (2° Â 2°) or black and white concentric circles (largest diameter vision. A foveated visual search strategy optimized for cone vision 2°). The eye tracker was calibrated so that the accuracy of calibra- will be suboptimal or even unsuccessful under scotopic illumina- tion was not worse than 0.35° in the photopic condition and 0.70° tion since the observer would make eye movements to bring prob- in the scotopic condition. When required, the calibration procedure able target locations onto the fovea but the foveal image of this could be repeated during a block, but this was rarely necessary. In position would fade as soon as the eyes land there. Here, we addition, drift correction was done prior to every trial. address the question whether the human visual system modifies Visual stimuli were presented on a calibrated CRT screen with a its search strategy to meet the properties of rod vision. resolution of 1280 Â 1024 pixels and a refresh rate of 100 Hz. A Adaptation of the search strategy to different lighting conditions correction of the monitor’s gamma nonlinearity was applied based would require the visual system to have representations of sepa- on prior photometric measures.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    14 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us