Journal of Forensic Psychology Research and Practice ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/wfpp21 Coerced-Reactive Confessions: The Case of Thomas Quick Ulf Stridbeck To cite this article: Ulf Stridbeck (2020) Coerced-Reactive Confessions: The Case of Thomas Quick, Journal of Forensic Psychology Research and Practice, 20:4, 305-322, DOI: 10.1080/24732850.2020.1732758 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/24732850.2020.1732758 © 2020 The Author(s). Published with license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC. Published online: 28 Feb 2020. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 1332 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=wfpp21 JOURNAL OF FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCH AND PRACTICE 2020, VOL. 20, NO. 4, 305–322 https://doi.org/10.1080/24732850.2020.1732758 Coerced-Reactive Confessions: The Case of Thomas Quick Ulf Stridbeck, LL.D. Department of Public Law, Faculty of Law, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway ABSTRACT KEYWORDS Thomas Quick of Sweden confessed to over thirty murders False confessions; coerced- between 1993 and 2000, and was convicted of eight of reactive confessions; them over the course of six trials in Sweden. All the con- miscarriage of justice; victions relied on his blurred confessions. He has since – injustice; legal safeguards between 2011 and 2013 – been exonerated for all of them. This case report will provide a detailed account of a real life case where the psychiatric patient has kept notes and documents from his period in the psychiatric hospital, uniquely demonstrating the gradual and subtle develop- ment of false confessions. In addition to Saul Kassin and Lawrence Wrightsman´s three major types of false confes- sions, (voluntary, coerced-compliant, coerced-internalized) Quick´s confessions are an example of a fourth type of false confessions – coerced-reactive confessions, a subcategory of coerced confessions in that way, as they are not police induced. The confessor has reacted to exter- nal pressures outside of the police interrogation setting. Joseph McCann ends his paper with the call for more research on how various sources of coercion impact on the interrogation and confession process and this paper attempts to provide a contribution to that end. However, the Quick story is not only a story of injustice, but also a psychiatric care scandal. As the therapists, more or less unwittingly brought Quick to false confessions it illustrates how confirmation bias in psychiatric health care can poison the subsequent police investigation and result in wrongful convictions. The Quick case teaches us that coerced- reactive false confessions in a therapy room can lead to miscarriage of justice in the courtroom. There are three major types of false confessions (voluntary, coerced-compliant, coerced-internalized, see Kassin & Wrightsman, 1985). In his 1998 paper, McCann proposed coerced-reactive as a fourth subtype of false confessions. Coerced-reactive confessions are false confessions coerced in noncustodial situa- tions. In general, coerced confessions tend to get much attention both in research (Gudjonsson, 2003; Kassin & Wrightsman, 1985;Leo&Ofshe,2001)andin exoneration cases (Appleby, Hasel, & Kassin, 2013;Garrett,2011; www.innoncen ceproject.com). Voluntary false confessions do not tend to get the same attention. CONTACT Ulf Stridbeck [email protected] Department of Public Law, Faculty of Law, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway © 2020 The Author(s). Published with license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way. 306 U. STRIDBECK Documented false confessions leading to convictions are rare in the Nordic countries (Stridbeck, 2015) and tend to be coerced false confessions.1 Voluntary false confessions leading to wrongful convictions are even rarer. However, some people confess to the murder of famous people – herostratic reputation. For example, according to the Swedish police, 130 people have confessed to the murder of the Swedish Prime minister Olof Palme in 1986 (Stridbeck, 2015). None of them were charged, let alone convicted. Studies of real life false confessors have identified special populations that might be especially prone to give false confessions (Follette, Leo, & Davis, 2017). Juveniles, persons with low IQ and mentally ill persons, such as Quick, are overrepresented among false confessors. Overview Thomas Quick, of Sweden, was born as Sture Bergwall in 1950. He confessed to over thirty murders, which he claimed to have committed in Sweden, Norway and Finland between the years 1993 and 2000. He was convicted of eight of them over the course of six trials. The judgments applied to three murders in Norway: two young women and a little girl. In addition, five murders in Sweden: two young men, a tourist couple from the Netherlands, and a boy. The Swedish boy and the girl in Norway are not documented murder cases since they are disappearances without any corpses. Despite that, his confession was so compel- ling that he was convicted of murder here as well. Moreover, there is a great variation in modus operandi: different victims and different approaches do not indicate that a serial killer has been active. All the cases relied solely on his confessions. There was no forensic evidence or witnesses in any of them. Despite the absence of corroborating evidence, he was convicted. Between 2011 and 2013, he was exonerated for all of them. His criminal career started in 1970 when he was sentenced to a mental institution for personality disorders after he had tried to molest four young boys at a children’s clinic where he had been working for a short period. He was released in 1973, but continued to receive treatment. The following year, he stabbed a man and was suspected of an attempted murder. However, since he was receiving psychiatric care he was never charged, but sent back to the clinic. He was finally released in 1977. For a period of fourteen years, he was not visible in any criminal records. Then, in 1991, he and an accomplice were convicted of theft and aggravated robbery with a hostage. For these crimes, Quick was not sent to prison but to a psychiatric institution. The treatment he received was medicine and therapy for the anxiety attacks he suffered. The 1See the Norwegian Karmoy case (Gudjonsson, 2003) and the Norwegian Moen cases (http://justicedenied.org/ issue/issue_40/moen_i-40.pdf). JOURNAL OF FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCH AND PRACTICE 307 methodology they used was their very own “specialty”–British object relation therapy – where recovered memory therapy is essential. The therapy sessions were important for both the therapists at the hospital and for Quick himself. They had mutual benefit of each other. The hospital needed to market their own therapy model and Quick got someone to talk with. Further, he got more or less unrestricted amounts of narcotic classified drugs. Moreover, he “became somebody”. There are similarities with the Henry Lee Lucas case in the United States (Gudjonsson, 2003). Lucas confessed to hundreds of unsolved murders. In an interview with Gisli Gudjonsson Lucas estimated that he had made over 3000 false confessions to murder. It was during his imprisonment he began confessing to one murder after the other. His confessions were so farfetched, and often contradictory, that they were dismissed as hoaxes by the media. It is unclear how many murders he had committed. Anyway, he was convicted of murdering eleven people. Later, Lucas withdrew many of his confessions; he claimed the attention he received for his numerous statements gave him privileges that normal prisoners were denied. His reasoning for the confessions was “that prior to his arrest in 1983 he was `nobody`; that is, he had no friends and nobody listened to him or took interest in him. Once he began to make false confessions all that changed and he has thoroughly enjoyed his `celebrity` (or notoriety) status and had now many friends” (Gudjonsson, 2003, p. 557). Many of his false confessions were built on knowledge learned from case files. As with Lucas, Quick was totally uninterested in the long-term consequences of his confessions. He had nothing to lose by confessing falsely. Both Lucas and Quick were willing to say anything that might grant them short-term advan- tages. Some immediate benefits outweighed the long-term costs of the isolated and boring hospital life. Quick was already incarcerated, he got almost unrest- ricted amounts of medication, he became an interesting person who was visited by curious journalists, and both the psychotherapists and the investigators took him around to different alleged crime scenes in Sweden and Norway. He confessed to get something, not to escape from harsh conditions. Once Quick began to make false confessions, he became important. He was rewarded with benefits similar to those that had been granted to Lucas. The Quick case is not about false memories, like the Ingram case (Ofshe, 1992), or erroneous memories, but false stories with fake facts, with the purpose of satisfying the psychotherapists. In his book he writes, “Iwantedtogivethem something” (Bergwall, 2016, p. 39). In the diary the 316 day he writes that when he looked into the therapist’s eyes, he knew he “must satisfy her desires. … It was the words that were her desires, the words that were spelled trauma, childhood and restoration. I gave them to her.” (Bergwall, 2016,p.61). The Quick case illustrates how a mix of psychotherapy, repressed memory theory, and free prescription of drugs induced a person to confess murders of 308 U.
Details
-
File Typepdf
-
Upload Time-
-
Content LanguagesEnglish
-
Upload UserAnonymous/Not logged-in
-
File Pages19 Page
-
File Size-