6 3 the Cognitive Neuroscience of Language Acquisition

6 3 the Cognitive Neuroscience of Language Acquisition

6 3 The Cognitive Neuroscience of Language Acquisition KARIN STROMSWOLD ABSTRACT This chapter reviews findings from several re- haps as a function of the quality of instruction), and search areas-normal language acquisition, learnability theory, there may be no critical period for acquisition. developmental and acquired language disorders, and language Even if the ability to learn language is the result of in- acquisition after the critical period-indicating that the ability nate mechanisms and. predispositions, another question to acquire language is the result of innate brain mechanisms. It is possible that infants' brains are predisposed to perceive cate- remains: Are these mechanisms specific to language and gorically such stimuli as phonemes, words, syntactic catego- language acquisition (e.g., Chomsky, 1981, 1986; Pinker, ries, and phrases, and this predisposition allows children to 1994) or are they also involved in tasks and abilities that acquire language rapidly and with few errors. are not linguistic (e.g.,. Karmiloff Smith, 1991; Elman et Because the ability to learn a language is a uniquely hu- al., 1996)? If the ability to acquire language is the result man ability, language acquisition is an important topic of innate mechanisms used solely for language and lan- in cognitive neuroscience. Perhaps the most fundamen- guage acquisition, language may be functionally and an- tal question about language and language acquisition is atomically autonomous or modular from other abilities, the extent to which the ability to learn language is the re- in which case developmental and acquired lesions may sult of innate mechanisms or predispositions (henceforth specifically impair or spare the ability to learn language. referred to as innate abilities). Innate abilities often share Conversely, if general-purpose mechanisms are in- certain characteristics. An innate ability is usually volved in language acquisition, we would not expect to present in all normal individuals. Its acquisition tends to find evidence of the functional or anatomical modularity be uniform and automatic, with all normal individuals of language or language acquisition. going through the same stages at the same ages, without specific instruction. There may be a critical period for Language development successful acquisition. The ability is likely to be func- tionally and anatomically autonomous or modular. Fi- LINGUISTICS AND THE UNIVERSAL FEATURES OF LAN- nally, the trait may be heritable. GUAGE Superficially, learning to talk differs from Although these characteristics are by no means defini- learning to walk in that children are capable of learning tive, they can be used to evaluate traits that may be in- many different languages, but just one basic walk. If chil- nate. Consider, for example, the ability to walk and the dren really are predisposed to learn all human lan- ability to knit. The ability to walk exhibits most of the guages, then all languages must be fundamentally the hallmarks of innate abilities, and is presumably innate; same. In fact, linguists have discovered that, although but the ability to knit exhibits few of these hallmarks, some languages seem to differ radically from other lan- and is presumably not innate. If children's brains are in- guages (e.g., Turkish and English), in essential ways all nately predisposed to learn language, then given ade- human languages are remarkably similar to one another quate ex to language, all children with normal (Chomsky, 1981,1986; Croft, 1990). brains should, without instruction, learn language in a Generative linguists usually assume that language in- relatively uniform way, just as normal vision develops volves rules and operations that have no counterparts in given adequate exposure to visual stimuli (Hubel and nonlinguistic domains and that the ability to use and Wiesel, 1970). But if the ability to learn language is not acquire language is part of our innate endowment.. For innate, instruction may be necessary, the course of ac- example, within principles-and-parameters (P&P) gener- quisition may vary greatly from person to person (per- ative theory (Chomsky, 1981, 1986), all languages are said to share a common set of grammatical principles. Differences among languages result from the different KAREN STROMSwOLD Department of Psychology and Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, parametric values chosen for those principles. According N.J. to P&P theory, at some level, children are born knowing 909 the principles that are universal to all languages (Univer- 1985). Representative utterances during this period in- Where Humpty Dumpty go 1 and sal Grammar); thus, to learn a particular language, all clude Sarah want cookie, they must do is learn the vocabulary and parametric set- Adam write pencil. Children's early speech is often de- tings of that language. Similarly, within another genera- scribed as "telegraphic" (Brown, 1973) because it resem- tive linguistic theory-optimality theory (OT)-the same bles the way adults speak when words are at a premium, universal constraints operate in all languages, and lan- as in a telegram. Gradually, omissions become rarer un- guages differ merely in the ranking of these constraints til children are between three and four years old, at (Prince and Smolensky, 1993). According to OT, chil- which point the vast majority of English-speaking chil- A dren are born "knowing" the universal constraints; thus, dren's utterances are completely grammatical (Strom- to learn a particular language, all children must do is, swold, 1990a,b, 1994b). Children who are acquiring learn the vocabulary and ranking of constraints for that languages like Turkish, which have rich, regular, and language (Tesar and Smolensky, 1996). Linguists work- perceptually salient morphological systems, generally ing within the functionalist tradition (e.g., Foley and Van begin to use functional category morphemes at a Valin, 1984) are more likely to assume that language younger age than children acquiring morphologically shares properties with nonlinguistic abilities, and that op- poor languages (Aksu-Koc and Slobin, 1985; Berman, erations used in language acquisition are used in other, 1986; Peters, 1995). For example, in striking contrast to nonlinguistic domains (e.g., Bates and MacWhinney, the telegraphic speech of English-speaking children, 1982; Budwig, 1995; Van Valin, 1991). Turkish-speaking children often begin to produce mor- phologically complex words before they begin to use UNIFORMITY IN LANGUAGE ACQUISITION Within a multiword utterances (Aksu-Koc and Slobin, 1985) 2 given language, the course of language acquisition is re- Within a given language, children master the syntax markably uniform (Brown, 1973).' Most children say (grammar) of their language in a surprisingly similar their first referential words at 9 to 15 months (Morley, manner (Brown, 1973). For example, children acquire the 1965; Benedict, 1979; Fenson et al., 1994; Huttenlocher 14 grammatical morphemes of English in essentially the and Smiley, 1987), and for the next 6-8 months, chil- same order (Brown, 1973; deVilliers and deVilliers, dren typically acquire single words fairly slowly until 1973). Similarly, all 15 of the children I studied acquired they have acquired approximately 50 words. For most the 20-odd English auxiliary verbs in essentially the same children acquiring English, the majority of their first 50 order (Stromswold, 1990a). The order in which these 15 words are labels for objects (e.g., cookie, mother, father, children acquired complex constructions-questions, neg- bottle) with a few action verbs (eat, come, go), social terms ative constructions, passives, datives, exceptional-case- (good-bye, hello), and prepositions (up, down) rounding out marking constructions, embedded sentences, preposi- the list (Nelson, 1973; Bates et al., 1994; Benedict, 1979). tion-stranding constructions, causative constructions, Once children have acquired 50 words, their vocabular- small clause constructions, verb-particle constructions, ies often increase rapidly (e.g., Reznick and Goldfield, and relative clauses constructions-was. also extremely 1992; Benedict, 1979; Mervis and Bertrand, 1995), ex- regular '(Stromswold, 1988, 1989a,b, 1990a,b, 1992, panding by 22 to 37 words per month (Benedict, 1979; 1994b, 1995; Stromswold and Snyder, 1995; Snyder and Goldfield and Reznick, 1990). Stromswold, 1997). Finally, to a remarkable degree, At around 18 to 24 months, children learning mor- within and across languages, children make certain types phologically impoverished languages such as. English of mistakes and not others. begin combining words to form two-word utterances such as want cookie, play checkers, and big drum (Brown, ACQUISITION OF SYNTACTIC CATEGORIES In order to 1973). During this two-word stage, the vast majority of acquire their language, children must not only learn the children's utterances are legitimate portions of sentences meanings of words like cat and eat, they must also learn in the language they are learning. Thus, in English-a that words like cat are nouns and words like eat are language that has restricted word order-children will verbs. That is, they must learn the categorical member- say want cookie but not cookie want (Brown, 1973) and he ship of words. This is critical because whether a syntac- bigbut not big he (Bloom, 1990). Children acquiring such tic or morphological ri& applies to a particular word morphologically impoverished languages gradually be- depends

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    24 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us