Oldowan Toolmaking and Hominin Brain Evolution: Theory and Research Using Positron Emission Tomography (Pet)

Oldowan Toolmaking and Hominin Brain Evolution: Theory and Research Using Positron Emission Tomography (Pet)

stone age institute publication series Series Editors Kathy Schick and Nicholas Toth Stone Age Institute Gosport, Indiana and Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana Number 1. THE OLDOWAN: Case Studies into the Earliest Stone Age Nicholas Toth and Kathy Schick, editors Number 2. BREATHING LIFE INTO FOSSILS: Taphonomic Studies in Honor of C.K. (Bob) Brain Travis Rayne Pickering, Kathy Schick, and Nicholas Toth, editors Number 3. THE CUTTING EDGE: New Approaches to the Archaeology of Human Origins Kathy Schick, and Nicholas Toth, editors Number 4. THE HUMAN BRAIN EVOLVING: Paleoneurological Studies in Honor of Ralph L. Holloway Douglas Broadfield, Michael Yuan, Kathy Schick and Nicholas Toth, editors STONE AGE INSTITUTE PUBLICATION SERIES NUMBER 1 THE OLDOWAN: Case Studies Into the Earliest Stone Age Edited by Nicholas Toth and Kathy Schick Stone Age Institute Press · www.stoneageinstitute.org 1392 W. Dittemore Road · Gosport, IN 47433 COVER PHOTOS Front, clockwise from upper left: 1) Excavation at Ain Hanech, Algeria (courtesy of Mohamed Sahnouni). 2) Kanzi, a bonobo (‘pygmy chimpanzee’) fl akes a chopper-core by hard-hammer percussion (courtesy Great Ape Trust). 3) Experimental Oldowan fl aking (Kathy Schick and Nicholas Toth). 4) Scanning electron micrograph of prehistoric cut-marks from a stone tool on a mammal limb shaft fragment (Kathy Schick and Nicholas Toth). 5) Kinesiological data from Oldowan fl aking (courtesy of Jesus Dapena). 6) Positron emission tomography of brain activity during Oldowan fl aking (courtesy of Dietrich Stout). 7) Experimental processing of elephant carcass with Oldowan fl akes (the animal died of natural causes). (Kathy Schick and Nicholas Toth). 8) Reconstructed cranium of Australopithecus garhi. (A. garhi, BOU-VP-12/130, Bouri, cranial parts, cranium recon- struction; original housed in National Museum of Ethiopia, Addis Ababa. ©1999 David L. Brill). 9) A 2.6 million-year-old trachyte bifacial chopper from site EG 10, Gona, Ethiopia (courtesy of Sileshi Semaw). Back: Photographs of the Stone Age Institute. Aerial photograph courtesy of Bill Oliver. Published by the Stone Age Institute. ISBN-10: 0-9792-2760-7 ISBN-13: 978-0-9792-2760-8 Copyright © 2006, Stone Age Institute Press. All rights reserved under International and Pan-American Copyright Conventions. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, without permission in writing from the publisher. CHAPTER 9 OLDOWAN TOOLMAKING AND HOMININ BRAIN EVOLUTION: THEORY AND RESEARCH USING POSITRON EMISSION TOMOGRAPHY (PET) BY DIETRICH STOUT ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION Attempts to understand the paleopsychological and What role might early stone tools have played in neuro-evolutionary significance of early stone tools the evolution of the human mind? This is an old ques- have long suffered from a scarcity of hard evidence tion, but one of enduring interest. In the past, regarding the actual neural substrates of stone toolmak- researchers have generally approached the issue by ing skill. The Positron Emission Tomography (PET) attempting to define the cognitive demands of stone tool pilot study of Stout et al. (2000), together with prelimi- manufacture. This has been done in the relatively casu- nary results from ongoing follow-up research, are al or “common-sense” language of archaeologists (e.g. beginning to readdress this problem by providing a new Belfer-Cohen & Goren-Inbar, 1994; Chase, 1991; avenue of experimental inquiry for human origins Gowlett, 1984; Isaac, 1986; Karlin & Julien, 1994; researchers. In these studies, PET was used to identify Tobias, 1979) as well as through more explicit reference the regions of the brain that display increased activity to psychological theory (Mithen, 1996; Parker & during simple Oldowan-style (Mode I) flake produc- Gibson, 1979; Robson Brown, 1993; Wynn, 1989). tion. Although results are preliminary pending further Some workers have even attempted to identify the neu- analysis, robust evidence of activation in the primary roanatomical foundations for tool-behavior, usually in sensorimotor cortices surrounding the central sulcus, in order to demonstrate some direct co-evolutionary con- the visual cortices of the occipital lobe, and in the cere- nection with language abilities (Calvin, 1993; bellum has already been observed. These activations Greenfield, 1991; Wilkins & Wakefield, 1995). reveal the relatively intense visuomotor demands of Despite the quality and quantity of consideration stone knapping and highlight those regions of the brain devoted to the issue, the link between tools and cogni- that would have been the most likely targets of selection tion in human evolution remains tentative and contro- on knapping skill. Somewhat less definitive evidence of versial. Part of the reason is a lack of direct evidence superior parietal activation further suggests that higher- regarding the relationship between tool-behavior and level visual association and spatial cognition may also brain function. What is needed is concrete evidence be involved. Available evidence does not indicate the regarding the actual neurophysiological underpinnings recruitment of prefrontal planning and problem solving of stone toolmaking skill. The technology of Positron regions, nor show any clear overlap between toolmak- Emission Tomography (PET), initially suggested as a ing and language processing networks. Results from the tool for the study of stone tools and cognition by Toth & PET research, although preliminary, are already rele- Schick (1993), and applied for the first time in research vant to numerous hypotheses concerning the cognitive (Stout et al., 2000) discussed below, provides the oppor- and evolutionary implications of early stone tools. tunity to collect just this kind of evidence. 268 The Oldowan: Case Studies Into the Earliest Stone Age HOMININ “PALEOPSYCHOLOGY” Among human origins researchers, discussion tends to center on such concepts as the “imposition of Information about the workings of the brain and arbitrary form” (Holloway, 1969) and the use of mental mind can come from two main sources: the study of (Clark, 1996) or procedural (Gowlett, 1984) “tem- neuroanatomy/neurophysiology and the observation of plates” in tool production. These criteria are used to behavior. This is as true in human evolutionary studies informally compare and evaluate the cognitive com- as it is in neuroscience and psychology, although the plexity of industrial complexes, as in the “opportunis- available data and degree of experimental control in the tic” Oldowan (Isaac, 1981) or the “more complicated former are obviously much more limited. Direct but and patterned” Acheulean (Schick & Toth, 1993). greatly limited evidence of protohuman neuroanatomy Although both informative and useful, the use of such is provided by endocasts of hominin cranial fossils “intuitive criteria” (Robson Brown, 1993) to evaluate (Falk, 1980; Holloway, 1995; Tobias, 1991) while more lithic technologies obviously leaves many more specif- detailed but indirect evidence comes from comparative ic questions unanswered. In order to achieve a more studies of modern primate brains (Gannon et al., 1998; complete appreciation of the cognitive implications of Preuss et al., 1999; Semendeferi & Damasio, 2000). stone tools, researchers have tended to borrow from one Observation of modern non-human primate behavior or another branch of psychological theory. also provides an important comparative perspective Developmental psychology has generally been the most (McGrew, 1992; Savage-Rumbaugh & Lewin, 1994; popular, including the constructivist developmental Rumbaugh et al., 1996; Tomasello & Call, 1997). With stages of Piaget and Inhelder (e.g. 1969) and elabora- respect to the “observation” of pre-modern behavior, it tions of the nativist modularity first proposed by Fodor is the reconstructive work of Paleolithic archaeologists (1983). that provides the best source of data. The use of these behavioral data to explore pre-modern mental charac- Piaget teristics and capacities might, for lack of a better term, An early and influential application of Piagetian be called hominin paleopsychology. Stone artifacts are psychology to Paleolithic archaeology is that of Parker one major source of information in this challenging and Gibson (1979). These authors argue, not only that undertaking. Piaget’s developmental stages may be used to evaluate Holloway (1981a) refers to prehistoric stone tools the cognitive sophistication of early toolmakers, but that as “fossilized behavior”. Although far from ideal, “certain projective and Euclidean preconcepts…arose durable stone artifacts do represent one of our best indi- as adaptations for stone-tool manufacture” (p. 375). cators of prehistoric behavior and cognition. This is due This conclusion is presented as part of a broader, reca- in part to practical issues of preservation and recovery, pitulationist, model of cognitive evolution that sees sen- but also to the nature of chipped stone technology itself. sorimotor, symbolic, intuitive and linguistic capacities At a theoretical level, tool-behavior rivals language as a as primary and secondary adaptations to intelligent tool hallmark of human cognition (Preston 1998). In fact, use. The scope of the model is not such that much atten- Schlanger (1994: 143) argues that “even if… we could tion is paid to specific tool types or industries; rather the actually observe a Palaeolithic band in vivo, it would be

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    42 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us