How Do Profoundly Deaf Children Learn to Read?∗

How Do Profoundly Deaf Children Learn to Read?∗

Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 16(4), 222–229 Copyright C 2001, The Division for Learning Disabilities of the Council for Exceptional Children ∗ How Do Profoundly Deaf Children Learn to Read? Susan Goldin-Meadow University of Chicago Rachel I. Mayberry McGill University Abstract. Reading requires two related, but separa- to read is important for at least two reasons. First, the ble, capabilities: (1) familiarity with a language, and more we understand the process by which deaf children (2) understanding the mapping between that language read, the more we can do to improve that process in the and the printed word (Chamberlain & Mayberry, 2000; deaf population. Second, understanding reading in deaf Hoover & Gough, 1990). Children who are profoundly children has the potential to inform us about reading in deaf are disadvantaged on both counts. Not surpris- all populations. ingly, then, reading is difficult for profoundly deaf chil- dren. But some deaf children do manage to read fluently. How? Are they simply the smartest of the crop, or do WHY MIGHT DEAF CHILDREN BE they have some strategy, or circumstance, that facili- DISADVANTAGED WHEN LEARNING tates linking the written code with language? A priori TO READ? one might guess that knowing American Sign Language (ASL) would interfere with learning to read English sim- Virtually all children learn to speak effortlessly. Yet not ply because ASL does not map in any systematic way all learn to read and reading is often difficult. Why? onto English. However, recent research has suggested To become readers, children must learn the mapping between the spoken language they already know and that individuals with good signing skills are not worse, printed words on a page. For English, as for most lan- and may even be better, readers than individuals with guages, that mapping is based on sound. Once children poor signing skills (Chamberlain & Mayberry, 2000). understand the underlying principles of the print-sound Thus, knowing a language (even if it is not the language mapping—once they “crack the code”—they can call captured in print) appears to facilitate learning to read. upon their knowledge of their spoken language to fa- Nonetheless, skill in signing does not guarantee skill cilitate the reading process. Profoundly deaf children in reading—reading must be taught. The next frontier are disadvantaged as potential readers on both of these for reading research in deaf education is to understand counts—they do not have easy access to the phonolog- how deaf readers map their knowledge of sign language ical code and many do not know any language well, let onto print, and how instruction can best be used to turn alone the language captured in print. signers into readers. Most profoundly deaf children read poorly. However, Reduced Access to the Phonological Code a small minority learn to read fluently. Understanding how profoundly deaf children learn, or fail to learn, Profoundly deaf children have inadequate access to the auditory basis for print-sound mapping. Roughly one in ∗ 1,000 children born in the United States is severely to Preparation of this paper was supported by grant RO1 DC00491 profoundly deaf (Ruben, 1972). A child with a severe from NIH to S. Goldin-Meadow, and by grant 171239 from the Nat- ural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada and (70 to 89 decibel) hearing loss is unable to hear even grant 410-98-0803 from the Social Sciences and Humanities Re- shouted conversation and thus cannot learn speech as a search Council of Canada to R. Mayberry. normally hearing child would. A child with a profound Requests for reprints should be sent to Susan Goldin-Meadow, (≥90 decibel) loss hears only occasional loud sounds University of Chicago, 5730 South Woodlawn Ave., Chicago, IL and these sounds may be perceived as vibrations rather 60637. than sound patterns. GOLDIN-MEADOW AND MAYBERRY: DEAF CHILDREN LEARNING TO READ 223 A deaf child’s limited hearing abilities can be aug- 1999; Newport & Meier, 1985). Unfortunately for the mented with hearing aids, and amplification via a hear- potential deaf reader, ASL is not English. ing aid does increase a child’s awareness of sound. How- Sign languages are autonomous languages, not based ever, the extent to which a hearing aid can help the child on the spoken languages of hearing cultures (Bellugi & learn a spoken language depends on many factors, in- Studdert-Kennedy, 1980; Emmorey, In press; Klima & cluding which speech frequencies the child is able to Bellugi, 1979; Lane & Grosjean, 1980). The structure hear with the hearing aid and the extent to which speech of ASL is distinct from the structure of English. Indeed, sounds remain distorted despite amplification (Moores, the structure of ASL is distinct even from the structure 1982; Seyfried & Kricos, 1989). The cochlear implant of British Sign Language—a fact that dramatically un- is a relatively new device designed to improve upon derscores the point that sign languages are not derivative the hearing aid. Unlike a hearing aid, which is remov- from spoken languages. Indeed, ASL is closer in struc- able, the cochlear implant is surgically placed inside ture to polysynthetic languages such as Navajo than to the portion of the inner ear that converts sound to neu- English (Newport & Meier, 1985). ral signals (the cochlea). The implant receives signals The bottom line for many deaf children born to deaf from an external device worn behind the ear and stimu- parents is that, although they are native (and fluent) lates electrodes in the cochlea; the electrodes stimulate users of a language (sign language), that language is the auditory nerve directly, bypassing the hair cells that not the language they are learning to read. implement the first stage of auditory neural process- ing in intact ears. Cochlear implants appear to improve hearing for adults who become deaf after having a spo- Deaf Children Born to Hearing Parents ken language. However, the data are much less clear for prelingually deaf children who must learn spoken language through the device (Owens & Kessler, 1989; Ninety percent of deaf children in the United States are Svirsky, Robbins, Kirk, Pisoni, & Miyamoto, 2000). born to hearing parents, who are not likely to know sign It is hardly surprising, then, that children born with language. As a result, these deaf children will not be severe to profound hearing losses often do not achieve exposed to sign language at birth. the kind of proficiency in spoken language that nor- Before 1960, the only educational option available to mally hearing children do. Even with intensive oral young deaf children in the classroom was oral instruc- instruction, deaf children’s acquisition of speech is tion without sign language. It was not until 1960, when markedly delayed when compared to the acquisition of Stokoe published the first linguistic analysis of ASL, speech by normally hearing children of hearing parents that educators began to realize that the manual modal- (Conrad, 1979; Mayberry, In press; Meadow, 1968; ity could support language. Although ASL was slowly Seyfried & Kricos, 1989). The bottom line for pro- earning recognition as a “real” language, the prevail- foundly deaf children is that they do not have access ing belief among teachers of the deaf was that learning to the same auditory base that normally hearing chil- to sign English ought to be better for learning to read dren do. English than learning to sign ASL. As a result, edu- cators, both deaf and hearing, invented a number of different sign systems (Signing Essential English, See- ing Essential English, Signing Exact English, Signed Reduced Access to the Language Captured English; Lou, 1988) which, as a group, are referred to in Print as Manually Coded English (MCE).1 All these systems are synthesized—they borrow signs from ASL and syn- Profoundly deaf children typically have imperfect tactic structure from English. The goal is for children knowledge of the language that is mapped by the print to learn the structure of English, not only through the system they are learning—English, for children learn- sound and lip-reading patterns of spoken English, but ing to read in the United States. Perhaps surprisingly, also through the manual patterns of signed English. To the language a deaf child typically learns differs for foster the development of speech and spoken English, children born to deaf vs. hearing parents. MCE is signed while simultaneously speaking English. Although perhaps an excellent idea in principle, MCE systems are difficult to process in practice. Teach- Deaf Children Born to Deaf Parents ers of the deaf find it hard to sign and speak at the same Speech is not the only route to language. Language can 1 be learned through the eye and hand rather than the ear The most current, although at the moment still rare, movement in and mouth, that is, children can learn a signed rather deaf education recognizes that knowing one language (ASL) makes it easier to learn another (English). Under this model, the goal of than a spoken language. Deaf children born to deaf par- the deaf school is to promote and, when necessary, teach ASL as ents are very likely to be exposed to a natural sign lan- deaf children’s first language, and then teach English (either through guage such as ASL from birth. These children learn print, sign, or sound) as their second—in other words, to foster ASL as their first language. They learn ASL easily, as bilingual deaf education (Singleton, Supalla, Litchfield, & Schley, easily as hearing children learn English (Lillo-Martin, 1998). 224 SPECIAL ISSUE time without distorting one of the two systems (Marmor However, some profoundly deaf individuals do learn & Petitto, 1979). Moreover, whereas some aspects of to read, and are as proficient at reading as their normally MCE are relatively easy to learn, others are not (Schick hearing peers.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    8 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us