Self-Centered Memories: the Reminiscence Bump and the Self

Self-Centered Memories: the Reminiscence Bump and the Self

Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (8), 1403-1414 doi:10.3758/MC.36.8.1403 Self-centered memories: The reminiscence bump and the self CLARE J. RATHBONE, CHRIS J. A. MOULIN, AND MARTIN A. CONWAY University of Leeds, Leeds, England The self–memory relationship is thought to be bidirectional, in such a way that memories provide context for the self, and equally, the self exercises control over retrieval (Conway, 2005). Autobiographical memories are not distributed equally across the life span; instead, memories peak between ages 10 and 30. This reminiscence bump has been suggested to support the emergence of a stable and enduring self. In the present study, the rela- tionship between memory accessibility and self was explored with a novel methodology that used generation of self images in the form of I am statements. Memories generated from I am cues clustered around the time of emergence for that particular self image. We argue that, when a new self-image is formed, it is associated with the encoding of memories that are relevant to that self and that remain highly accessible to the rememberer later in life. This study offers a new methodology for academics and clinicians interested in the relationship between memory and identity. In the field of memory research, there has been a recent niscence bump is of particular interest, because it is the upsurge of interest in narrative accounts of personality and period from which, in a free recall task, people produce identity. Such approaches (e.g., McAdams et al., 2006) the most memories. Crucially, the reminiscence bump is suggest that personality is supported by the construction based robustly on the age of the rememberer at the time of life stories based on autobiographical memory (AM). of encoding and not on the age of the memories, so that AM is memory for events that occur in one’s life (Conway artifactual, retrieval-based accounts of this finding can be & Rubin, 1993) and is closely linked to our life goals, rejected (Rubin et al., 1986). our emotions, and our personal meanings. Our very sense There are several plausible explanations for the high of identity depends on being able to recall personal his- level of memories from the reminiscence bump period. tory (self-defining memories; Moffitt & Singer, 1994); in One broadly cognitive theory is that this period is per- order to explore self and identity, it is necessary to under- meated by novel experiences and that it is this novelty, stand the encoding and retrieval of event-specific tempo- preserved in memory in some way, that ensures their en- ral knowledge (see, e.g., Skowronski et al., 2007). For a during memorability. In support of this theory, Cohen and review of the complex relationship between self and mem- Faulkner (1988) found that 93% of vivid life memories ory, see Conway (2005). One well-established finding of were of either first-time experiences or of unique events. AM research is that memories are not distributed equally Similarly, Pillemer, Goldsmith, Panter, and White (1988) across the life span, but instead parallel changes in the self observed high memorability of first-time experiences, and in goals over time. Undoubtedly, the self changes over with 41% of participants’ memories for their first year at the course of childhood and perhaps stabilizes into an en- college coming from their first month of coursework. during form only in late adolescence and early adulthood Somewhat counter to this proposal is the finding that, (Erikson, 1950; Schwartz, 2006; Waterman, 1999). of the experiences recalled from the period of the reminis- A prominent theory is that these periods of develop- cence bump, only a small proportion are of novel events. ment of the self and of personality are reflected in the Fitzgerald (1988) found little evidence that experiences life span retrieval curve, which is observed when adults recalled in the period of the reminiscence bump were (aged about 35 and older) recall autobiographical memo- novel events, the majority being events distinctive to the ries (e.g., Rubin, Wetzler, & Nebes, 1986). The life span participants’ life circumstances and personal interests. For retrieval curve consists of three components: the period of example, the memories tend to focus upon events con- childhood amnesia (from birth to approximately 5 years), cerning relationships, family, work, and education (El- the period of the reminiscence bump (10–30 years), and nick, Margrett, Fitzgerald, & Labouvie-Vief, 1999). Such the period of recency (from the present declining back to inconsistencies in research based on the novelty account the period of the reminiscence bump). For those studying have led to the development of alternative explanations, the relationship between the self and memory, the remi- such as the life script hypothesis. Berntsen and Rubin C. J. Rathbone, [email protected] 1403 Copyright 2008 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1404 RATHBONE, MOULIN, AND CONWAY (2002) proposed that AM is organized by culturally shared change, and resulting development of new identities, are life scripts. A life script is defined as a form of seman- linked with heightened memory retrieval. tic knowledge, outlining the typical, culturally expected In addition, reminiscence bumps have been found for events that occur at given times in the life span (Berntsen age of encountering favorite films (Sehulster, 1996), & Rubin, 2002, 2004; Rubin & Berntsen, 2003). Across songs (Holbrook & Schindler, 1989), and books (Larsen, a range of studies, Berntsen and Rubin found that people 1996). Janssen, Chessa, and Murre (2007) directly com- do not show reminiscence bumps for sad and traumatic pared distributions of first encounters with favorite books, memories, but only for positive, important life events (see films, and records and found reminiscence bumps for all also Collins, Pillemer, Ivcevic, & Gooze, 2007). Berntsen three. Results were proposed to partially reflect increased and Rubin (2002) suggested that sad or traumatic events rehearsal of favorite items. Furthermore, the authors sug- are not expected culturally and contradict rudimentary be- gested that their findings could indicate privileged encod- liefs about the self (Janoff-Bulman, 1988); thus, the life ing of items during adolescence, because this could be script account explains the preference for a life narrative of when memories most linked with self formation are laid positive memories, linked with normative “happy” events, down. A further avenue of support for viewing the self as such as marriage and childbirth. Although this account central to the reminiscence bump comes from the study of views normative life scripts as the central organizational temporal references in dreams. Grenier et al. (2005) found factor in autobiographical recall, an alternative account that dreams demonstrated a peaking of events temporally of the bump posits the self as the key component. Rather associated with adolescence and young adulthood, in ef- than placing the emphasis on an internal cultural script, fect producing a reminiscence bump. Furthermore, con- this view suggests that the high accessibility of memories tent analysis of dreams temporally linked with the bump from the reminiscence bump period may be related to their revealed a prominence of themes associated with identity enduring relation to the self. and life goals (Cappeliez, 2008) that were regarded as re- Erikson (1950) proposed that one’s adult identity flecting the importance of reminiscence bump memories emerges during late adolescence and early adulthood. for the construction of a meaningful self. Possibly, many memories from this period are of “self de- The self is not envisaged as a unitary structure; rather, it fining” experiences (Singer & Salovey, 1993) and have a is the combination of many self schemata, including cog- powerful effect in binding the self to a specific reality. This nitive representations developed from specific autobio- alternative view suggests that a self account (Conway & graphical events, as well as general representations, such Haque, 1999) of the reminiscence bump might explain as “I am a mother” (Markus, 1977). Among these many why most memories are generated from this period. Thus, types of self-schemata are the selves that are activated at the reminiscence bump relates to a time in an individual’s a given moment, depending on certain contextual cues life that is critical for the formation and maintenance of present. Context has been shown to have a marked effect a stable self (e.g., Erikson, 1950; Rubin, Rahaal, & Poon, on the types of self-concepts generated (e.g., a female in an 1998). In support of this idea, Conway and Haque showed all-male room is more likely to use the frame of gender in similar accessibility of autobiographical memories from a self description; see McGuire & Padawer-Singer, 1976). a period outside the reminiscence bump, a product of the Markus and Kunda (1986) defined this active identity as “privileged encoding” of experiences that are particularly the working self-concept. More recently, this term has been relevant to the self. A group of older Bangladeshi partici- developed by Conway and Pleydell-Pearce (2000) as the pants showed a second reminiscence bump between the working self, a control structure that combines episodic ages of 35 and 55, coinciding with a period of national memories with autobiographical knowledge in AM for- conflict in Bangladesh. These data suggest that the forma- mation. The working self consists of self concepts, goal tion of a collection of new self images (as a result of cir- hierarchies, and other forms of self knowledge and may cumstances that involve major changes to personal iden- function to exercise inhibitory control over the knowledge tity) can result in the heightened retrieval of memories base (Conway, Singer, & Tagini, 2004). The working self from that time, further demonstrating the relation between and its link to AM change over childhood, but, it has been high accessibility of memories and AM’s enduring link suggested, stabilizes in late adolescence and early adult- with the self (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000).

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    12 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us