Approach to the Communication of Subsurface Hydrology and Hazards

Approach to the Communication of Subsurface Hydrology and Hazards

Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci., 20, 1737–1749, 2016 www.hydrol-earth-syst-sci.net/20/1737/2016/ doi:10.5194/hess-20-1737-2016 © Author(s) 2016. CC Attribution 3.0 License. A “mental models” approach to the communication of subsurface hydrology and hazards Hazel Gibson1, Iain S. Stewart1, Sabine Pahl2, and Alison Stokes1 1School of Geography, Earth and Environmental Sciences, Plymouth University, Plymouth, UK 2School of Psychology, Plymouth University, Plymouth, UK Correspondence to: Hazel Gibson ([email protected]) Received: 16 December 2015 – Published in Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. Discuss.: 18 January 2016 Revised: 12 April 2016 – Accepted: 15 April 2016 – Published: 4 May 2016 Abstract. Communicating information about geological and ing many hazard messages to fall into the largely now- hydrological hazards relies on appropriately worded commu- rejected “deficit model” of communication (Sturgis and Al- nications targeted at the needs of the audience. But what are lum, 2004). That model assumes people need to be educated these needs, and how does the geoscientist discern them? about those areas of knowledge in which they are seen to This paper adopts a psychological “mental models” approach be deficient, and it ignores their existing knowledge struc- to assess the public perception of the geological subsur- tures and wider concerns or values. Moreover, the respon- face, presenting the results of attitudinal studies and surveys sibility for tailoring the communication to the target audi- in three communities in the south-west of England. The find- ence is often placed on the public, requiring them to “ask the ings reveal important preconceptions and misconceptions re- right questions” (Rosenbaum and Culshaw, 2003). This em- garding the impact of hydrological systems and hazards on phasis on the public’s requirement to ask the right questions the geological subsurface, notably in terms of the persis- misses a bigger issue in communicating geological hazards, tent conceptualisation of underground rivers and the inferred namely the influence of intuitive judgments, such as heuris- relations between flooding and human activity. The study tics (Gilovich et al., 2002), in how people may interpret in- demonstrates how such mental models can provide geoscien- formation, especially unfamiliar scientific and technical data tists with empirical, detailed and generalised data of percep- (Kunreuther and Slovic, 1996). tions surrounding an issue, as well reveal unexpected outliers The value in examining perceptions specifically is increas- in perception that they may not have considered relevant, but ingly being recognised by many in the risk communication which nevertheless may locally influence communication. community, including in disaster risk reduction and commer- Using this approach, geoscientists can develop information cial geology fields. Barclay et al. (2008), for example, called messages that more directly engage local concerns and cre- for a more interdisciplinary “disaster reduction” approach to ate open engagement pathways based on dialogue, which in volcanic risk communication, which includes stakeholders in turn allow both geoscience “experts” and local “non-experts” policymaking and uses social and physical science to work to come together and understand each other more effectively. together to produce more appropriate and effective commu- nications based on the needs of the community. Meeting the particular needs of at-risk communities through collabora- tion between the physical and social sciences is now emerg- 1 Introduction ing as a fairly central component of modern risk science (Donovan et al., 2012; Frewer, 2004; Lave and Lave, 1991; Communicating information about geological and hydrolog- Mabon et al., 2014). ical hazards relies on appropriately worded communications The subjective nature of risk communication and under- (Liverman, 2010) targeted at the needs of the audience (Nis- standing among both experts and non-experts is now well bet, 2009). Those needs are often deemed to be what geo- established (Slovic et al., 2004), but it is easy for risk science professionals feel the public “need to know”, lead- Published by Copernicus Publications on behalf of the European Geosciences Union. 1738 H. Gibson et al.: A “mental models” approach to the communication of subsurface hydrology and hazards communicators to focus on improving access to informa- lie the perception of risk in non-experts, notably concepts tion from the scientists’ perspective and overlook the impact such as “familiarity” and “dread”. A graphical representation of experience- and emotion-based preconceptions from the (Fig. 1) shows the relative perceptions of different threats, as non-expert perspective (Leiserowitz, 2006). Commonplace organised by their varying degrees of familiarity and dread. preconceptions will strongly influence the way that a non- The diagram shows that certain threats which may statisti- specialist will access and interpret the geoscience risk infor- cally be considered more risky – such as riding a bicycle mation provided to them (Liverman, 2010), and so it is vital – are perceived to be far less risky than a statistically safer that public perceptions of geological and hydrological haz- activity, such as flying in a commercial aeroplane (Slovic, ards be taken into consideration by communicators. An ex- 1987). Later work coined the term “affect heuristic” to de- ample of the importance of misconceptions is provided by scribe the important role of intuitive feelings in non-experts’ Shackley et al. (2004), who report a geoscience expert using risk assessments (Slovic, 2010; Slovic et al., 2004). the term “bubble” of CO2 (Shackley et al., 2004, p. 127) to The affect heuristic describes the way that an individual’s explain carbon capture and storage to a lay audience; the re- perception can colour their response to a piece of information sult was a participant gaining a misconception relating to the about a subject, by ascribing greater or lesser importance to storage of the carbon in the form of “a large bubble” of gas the risk than an expert would, based on a logical assessment. which could burst at any time. This misconception caused The affect heuristic can be described as a form of emotion, some participants great distress and increased their percep- defined as positive or negative feelings that are used to evalu- tion of the risk. ate an external stimulus (Slovic et al., 2007). The influence of It has long been known that, when the public receive in- heuristics such as the affect heuristic is so central to design- formation, they can interpret it – and therefore organise their ing effective risk communication that these need to be far reactions – in a variety of ways depending on their percep- more fully integrated into methods of assessing the public’s tion of both the science and the scientist (Fischhoff, 1995). perception of geological and hydrological issues (Mabon et Various inherent cultural and social assumptions control the al., 2014). way that this information is interpreted, not excluding the in- By taking into account the impact of a non-experts’ per- fluence of the individual’s previous educational background ception of risk, the field of risk communication shifts from a (Donovan, 2010; Mabon et al., 2014; Slovic et al., 2007). one-way form of communication towards more of a dialogue. Thus, without examining a population through social or psy- However, even within this more inclusive mode of commu- chological scientific inquiry, it is impossible to predict how nication, an outdated emphasis on the information and value they will respond to a particular science communication mes- judgments of the expert is still apparent (Sturgis and Allum, sage (Wynne, 1991). An example of the impact of the partic- 2004). By this account the “top-down” transfer of informa- ipant’s background on a risk communication message was tion provided by the expert must be translated by the emo- explored in a study by Keller et al. (2006). It was found that tional state of the non-expert (Slovic et al., 2004) and inte- a person’s background and experience, particularly of previ- grated into their own “lay knowledge” (Callon, 1999). While ous flooding events, had a significant impact on the severity experts may value local knowledge during individual com- of risk ascribed to a flood hazard communication. munications, often the contribution of the non-expert popu- A key challenge of communicating such messages, there- lation is dismissed as inappropriate by experts, who expect fore, is that in addition to the wider social or cultural im- decisions to be made on the basis of relevant technical in- pact on perception of scientific information, individuals ap- formation. An example of this was found by Johnson (2008) ply their own pre-existing ideas and concepts to any scien- in a study of watershed modelling and public participation, tific data that they are presented with (Mileti et al., 2004). which showed that an over-reliance on technical methods for In this context, psychology-based methods are vital, and one constructing the watershed model resulted in a disconnect be- such method is the “mental models” approach (Morgan et tween the public and the technical modellers, as the model al., 2002). This paper introduces the mental models method- was perceived to be inaccessible, despite early public enege- ology and presents empirical evidence for public perceptions ment. There is, however, a growing acknowledgment of the of the geological subsurface,

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    13 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us