Voluntary Temporary Abstinence from Alcohol During “Dry January” and Subsequent Alcohol Use

Voluntary Temporary Abstinence from Alcohol During “Dry January” and Subsequent Alcohol Use

Voluntary temporary abstinence from alcohol during ªDry Januaryº and subsequent alcohol use Article (Accepted Version) de Visser, Richard O, Robinson, Emily and Bond, Rod (2016) Voluntary temporary abstinence from alcohol during “Dry January” and subsequent alcohol use. Health Psychology, 35 (3). pp. 281-289. ISSN 0278-6133 This version is available from Sussex Research Online: http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/57508/ This document is made available in accordance with publisher policies and may differ from the published version or from the version of record. If you wish to cite this item you are advised to consult the publisher’s version. Please see the URL above for details on accessing the published version. Copyright and reuse: Sussex Research Online is a digital repository of the research output of the University. Copyright and all moral rights to the version of the paper presented here belong to the individual author(s) and/or other copyright owners. To the extent reasonable and practicable, the material made available in SRO has been checked for eligibility before being made available. Copies of full text items generally can be reproduced, displayed or performed and given to third parties in any format or medium for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge, provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. http://sro.sussex.ac.uk 1 de Visser, R.O., Robinson, E. & Bond, R. (in press). Voluntary temporary abstinence from alcohol during “Dry January” and subsequent alcohol use. Health Psychology note: This is the unformatted uncorrected manuscript accepted for publication. The final published version may vary. Abstract Objective: Temporary abstinence from alcohol may convey physiological benefits and enhance well-being. The aim of this study was to address a lack of information about: (1) correlates of successful completion of a planned period of abstinence, and (2) how success or failure in planned abstinence affects subsequent alcohol consumption. Methods: 857 British adults (249 men, 608 women) participating in the “Dry January” alcohol abstinence challenge completed a baseline questionnaire, a one-month follow-up questionnaire, and a 6-month follow-up questionnaire. Key variables assessed at baseline included measures of alcohol consumption and drink refusal self-efficacy (DRSE). Results: In bivariate analysis, success during Dry January was predicted by measures of more moderate alcohol consumption and greater social DRSE. Multivariate analyses revealed that success during Dry January was best predicted by a lower frequency of drunkenness in the month prior to Dry January. Structural Equation Modelling revealed that participation in Dry January was related to reductions in alcohol consumption and increases in DRSE among all respondents at 6-month follow-up, regardless of success, but these changes were more likely among people who successfully completed the challenge. Conclusions: The findings suggest that participation in abstinence challenges such as “Dry January” may be associated with changes toward healthier drinking and greater DRSE, and is unlikely to result in undesirable “rebound effects”: very few people reported increased alcohol consumption following a period of voluntary abstinence. 2 In recent years, various organizations in different countries have established campaigns in which people are challenged to give up alcohol for one month. Some are designed as sponsored fundraising events (e.g., au.dryjuly.com, nz.dryjuly.com). Others such as “Dry January” (www.dryjanuary.org.uk) are simply presented as a challenge to be undertaken in cultures in which alcohol consumption is a common feature of social life (Babor et al., 2010). Anecdotal evidence suggests that some people make use of such challenges to initiate reductions in alcohol consumption or to quit drinking altogether. The latter goal is a key motivation behind campaigns such as Stoptober (stoptober.smokfree.nhs.uk), in which smokers are challenged to stop smoking for one month (Brown et al., 2014), because smokers who can give up for one month are significantly more likely to quit (West & Stapleton, 2008). Whereas “Stoptober” is underpinned by a desire to encourage smoking cessation (Brown et al., 2014), temporary alcohol abstinence challenges do not aim for permanent abstinence. For example, the goal of the UK charity Alcohol Concern is to improve people’s lives by reducing the harm caused by alcohol, with a long term aim of changing the drinking culture (www.alcoholconcern.org.uk). Alcohol Concern first ran its abstinence challenge “Dry January” in 2013 to encourage people to think about the way they drink and to talk about alcohol: this reflects theorizing around “social contagion”, and a hope that healthy changes in beliefs and behavior among a sub-group of people will spread through the population (Christakis & Fowler, 2013; Einstein & Epstein, 1980). Alcohol Concern also allows people to opt in to fundraising via sponsorship of their attempt to complete Dry January. One small-scale study of the effects of a month of abstinence found marked reductions in liver fat and blood glucose, moderate reductions in blood cholesterol, and marked increases in self-reported sleep quality, concentration, and work performance (Coghlan, 2014). These benefits are impressive, but it has been suggested that they may be lost if people subsequently return to previous levels of drinking or experience “rebound effects” whereby their alcohol use increases following a period of abstinence (New Scientist, 2014). Correlates and consequences of temporary abstinence There is a need for more information about the correlates and consequences of participation in alcohol abstinence challenges. Little is known about how many people successfully complete the Dry January challenge, or about characteristics that distinguish those who succeed from those who fail. Furthermore, there has been no evaluation of the longer-term effects of voluntarily undertaking a period of abstinence from alcohol. It is important to address these knowledge gaps to determine the potential utility of abstinence 3 challenges within health psychology. Characteristics of drinkers are likely to explain success in abstinence challenges and/or subsequent alcohol consumption. Drink Refusal Self-Efficacy (DRSE: Young et al., 1991) is an individual’s self-perceived capacity to refuse alcohol in three domains: social settings when others are drinking, for emotion regulation, and opportunistic drinking. Greater DRSE correlates with less harmful alcohol consumption (Atwell, Abraham & Duka, 2011; de Visser et al., 2014; Gilles, Turk & Fresco, 2006; Oei & Jardim 2007). One might, therefore, expect people with greater DRSE to be more likely to complete abstinence challenges. One might also expect those who have completed a month of abstinence in the past to be more likely to complete a new abstinence challenge because they have demonstrated their DRSE. There is a lack of evidence about how patterns of alcohol use affect success in an abstinence challenge. However, evidence from two related domains suggests that moderate drinkers may be more likely than heavier drinkers to complete an abstinence challenge. One study of pregnant women found that lighter drinkers were less likely to intend to drink or to actually drink during pregnancy (Zammit, Skouteris, Wetheim, Paxton & Milgrom, 2008). Studies of alcohol use among university students indicate that more moderate alcohol consumption is related to a lower likelihood of intended and actual heavy episodic drinking (Norman & Conner, 2006; Norman, Conner & Stride, 2012). Furthermore, people who more strongly endorse fun and sociability as reasons for binge drinking are more likely to subsequently engage in binge drinking (Norman et al., 2012). This finding supports the earlier speculation that people with lower social DRSE may be less likely to complete an abstinence challenge. It is also important to note that habitual patterns of alcohol intake exert a strong influence on the subsequent alcohol intake (Norman, 2011). In addition to considering characteristics of individuals, it is important to consider social contextual influences on behavior change. Social support can help people to adhere to health behavior change (Bauld, Bell, McCullough, Richardson & Greaves, 2010; Olander et al., 2013). Social support can be conceptualized in general terms and/or as a measure of direct support from specific individuals. In the context of Dry January, it could be conceptualized as encouragement from important individuals such as the event organizers and/or support and encouragement from companions in “buddy systems”, in which participants pair up to offer mutual support. Buddy systems can increase the likelihood of successful health behavior change across a range of behaviors (Jepson, Harris, Platt & Tannahill, 2010; West & Stapleton, 2008), and there is evidence that supportive friends or partners can help people to 4 reduce their alcohol intake (Barber & Crisp, 1995; McCrady, 2004). However, it is not known whether undertaking an alcohol abstinence challenge with another person affects success rates. People who engage in fundraising through Dry January may have a greater resolve to complete the challenge after having made a public commitment to it: evidence from social psychological research

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    21 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us