An Fmri Study on Cortical Responses During Active Self-Touch and Passive

An Fmri Study on Cortical Responses During Active Self-Touch and Passive

An fMRI study on cortical responses during active self-touch and passive touch from others Rochelle Ackerley, Eusra Hassan, Andrew Curran, Johan Wessberg, Håkan Olausson, Francis Mcglone To cite this version: Rochelle Ackerley, Eusra Hassan, Andrew Curran, Johan Wessberg, Håkan Olausson, et al.. An fMRI study on cortical responses during active self-touch and passive touch from others. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, Frontiers, 2012, 6, 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00051. hal-01470593 HAL Id: hal-01470593 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01470593 Submitted on 17 Feb 2017 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 07 August 2012 BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00051 An fMRI study on cortical responses during active self-touch and passive touch from others Rochelle Ackerley 1*, Eusra Hassan 2, Andrew Curran 3, Johan Wessberg 1, Håkan Olausson 1 and Francis McGlone 4 1 Department of Physiology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden 2 Magnetic Resonance and Image Analysis Research Centre, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK 3 Alder Hey Children’s NHS Foundation Trust, Liverpool, UK 4 School of Natural Sciences and Psychology, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK Edited by: Active, self-touch and the passive touch from an external source engage comparable Robyn Grant, University of Sheffield, afferent mechanoreceptors on the touched skin site. However, touch directed to glabrous UK skin compared to hairy skin will activate different types of afferent mechanoreceptors. Reviewed by: Despite perceptual similarities between touch to different body sites, it is likely that Francoise Schenk, Université de Lausanne, Switzerland the touch information is processed differently. In the present study, we used functional Jonathan Cole, Poole Hospital, UK magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to elucidate the cortical differences in the neural Luke Boorman, Univeristy of signal of touch representations during active, self-touch and passive touch from another, Sheffield, UK to both glabrous (palm) and hairy (arm) skin, where a soft brush was used as the stimulus. *Correspondence: There were two active touch conditions, where the participant used the brush in their Rochelle Ackerley, Department of Physiology, University of right hand to stroke either their left palm or arm. There were two similar passive, touch Gothenburg, Box 432, Göteborg, conditions where the experimenter used an identical brush to stroke the same palm 405 30, Sweden. and arm areas on the participant. Touch on the left palm elicited a large, significant, e-mail: [email protected] positive blood-oxygenation level dependence (BOLD) signal in right sensorimotor areas. Less extensive activity was found for touch to the arm. Separate somatotopical palm and arm representations were found in Brodmann area (BA) 3 of the right primary somatosensory cortex (SI) and in both these areas, active stroking gave significantly higher signals than passive stroking. Active, self-touch elicited a positive BOLD signal in a network of sensorimotor cortical areas in the left hemisphere, compared to the resting baseline. In contrast, during passive touch, a significant negative BOLD signal was found in the left SI. Thus, each of the four conditions had a unique cortical signature despite similarities in afferent signaling or evoked perception. It is hypothesized that attentional mechanisms play a role in the modulation of the touch signal in the right SI, accounting for the differences found between active and passive touch. Keywords: glabrous, hairy, motor, sensorimotor, skin, somatosensory, stroking INTRODUCTION for touch to the arm, indicating that touch is processed over many The brain receives afferent information from the activation of cognitive levels. mechanoreceptors in the skin during interactions with the envi- The high discriminatory ability from human glabrous skin ronment. The present study focuses on the cortical representa- (e.g., the ventral surfaces of the hands and feet) is based on tions from active, self-touch and passive (other) touch to the inputs from four main classes of low-threshold mechanorecep- palm and arm. There are differences in the types of low-threshold tors, namely: rapidly-adapting types I (RAI; Meissner’s corpus- mechanoreceptors found in the glabrous skin of the palm com- cles) and II (RAII; Pacinian corpuscles), and slowly-adapting pared to the hairy skin on the arm (for an overview, see Macefield, types I (SAI; Merkel’s disks) and II (SAII; Ruffini’s endings). 2005). The mechanoreceptors on the glabrous skin allow high These afferents are in the Aβ conduction range of myelinated discriminatory abilities for touch, whereas the input from hairy mechanoreceptors and send information to the brain very quickly skin does not give such discrimination. Despite these differences, (conducting at 36–73 m/s; Kakuda, 1992)atahightemporalres- glabrous and hairy skin are both sensitive to touch; a recent study olution (Perge et al., 2012). This type of mechanoreceptive input has shown that psychophysical ratings of the intensity and the provides an excellent source of information during discrimina- pleasantness of touch were not different between the skin of the tive touch; the incoming, high-quality tactile information can palm and the arm (McGlone et al., 2012). However, the study be compared in areas of the brain such as SI. This capacity can also found that using a Touch Perception Task (from Guest et al., be demonstrated in ways such as using two-point discrimina- 2011), subjects used more sensory descriptors when evaluating tion tests, where the fingertip skin has high discriminatory ability touch to the palm, whereas they used more emotional descriptors (2–4 mm; Bickley and Szilagyi, 2007). Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org August 2012 | Volume 6 | Article 51 | 1 Ackerley et al. Cortical processing of touch Hairy skin is defined as the non-glabrous and non- Blakemore et al., 1998). This has been hypothesized to be for mucocutaneous skin that covers the majority of the body surface. sensory cancellation e.g., how you cannot tickle yourself because Hairy skin does not contain RAI mechanoafferents but instead, an internal forward model captures the relationship between the includes hair, field and C-tactile (CT) afferents. These afferents motor efference copy and the predicted sensory consequences of have myelinated axons (e.g., Aβ terminal hair and field units; the action (Weiskrantz et al., 1971; Blakemore et al., 1998, 2000). Vallbo et al., 1995), or are unmyelinated (CT; Vallbo et al., 1993). Sensory cancellation allows you to attend to any unexpected The density of myelinated afferents in hairy skin is much less parts of the input, while ignoring the expected sensory feedback. than in glabrous skin (Provitera et al., 2007). In contrast to Conversely, during passive touch, an expectation of touch may be glabrous skin, hairy skin has a much lower discriminatory ability present, but there is no motor efference copy to nullify or cancel (e.g., 30–40 mm in the two-point discrimination test; Bickley and the subsequent, sensory input. Szilagyi, 2007), but is nevertheless sensitive to touch; in fact, CT Previous neuroimaging studies have found that active touch afferents respond to <250 mg force (Vallbo et al., 1993; Wessberg produces sensations that are less intense, compared to that from et al., 2003; Cole et al., 2006). The touch information relayed to passive touch, in part due to sensory cancellation (Blakemore the brain from CT afferents is comparatively lower in temporal et al., 1998, 1999). In these studies, the BOLD signal in SI con- resolution due to the slower conduction velocity and more vari- tralateral to the touched surface was found to be significantly able firing discharge (Vallbo et al., 1993, 1999; Wessberg et al., lower for active touch. It is reasoned that incoming signals are 2003). attended to less if they match the expected parameters, so can Touch information is described classically as having a soma- be ignored, thus a decrease in the signal is observed. The find- totopical representation in the contralateral SI (Penfield and ing of decreased activity in SI for active compared to passive Rasmussen, 1950), where discriminative and integrative aspects of touch is controversial. Some studies have found a decreased touch are processed, such as form, texture, shape, and size (Hsiao, SI signal, for example, in animal in vivo recordings (Chapin 2008). Tactile input is also processed in other cortical areas and and Woodward, 1981; Jiang et al., 1991), electroencephalography the information flow can be split into dorsal and ventral streams (EEG; Abbruzzese et al., 1981; Tapia et al., 1987), magnetoen- (Romo et al., 2002). The dorsal stream sends information to cephalography (Hesse et al., 2010), and fMRI (Blakemore et al., Brodmann areas (BA) 5 and 7 and has been associated with pro- 1998). However, other animal in vivo electrophysiological exper- cessing during active touch,

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    10 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us