EXCLUSION and NONCONSCIOUS BEHAVIORAL MIMICRY: the ROLE of BELONGINGNESS THREAT DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment Of

EXCLUSION and NONCONSCIOUS BEHAVIORAL MIMICRY: the ROLE of BELONGINGNESS THREAT DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment Of

EXCLUSION AND NONCONSCIOUS BEHAVIORAL MIMICRY: THE ROLE OF BELONGINGNESS THREAT DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Jessica L. Lakin, B.A., M.A. * * * * * The Ohio State University 2003 Dissertation Committee: Approved by Professor Robert M. Arkin, Adviser Professor Tanya L. Chartrand Adviser Professor Marilynn B. Brewer Psychology Graduate Program ABSTRACT Human beings are social animals; the need to belong and be accepted is fundamental, and social exclusion can be devastating. It is therefore not surprising that people have developed behaviors, even automatic behaviors, which may help them to maintain relationships with important others. Research on nonconscious behavioral mimicry suggests that mimicking the nonverbal behaviors of others creates liking and rapport, and may therefore represent one way for an excluded person to affiliate with someone new. The current studies explored this hypothesis. In both experiments, participants played a simulated online ball-tossing game and were excluded by three computer-controlled players. They then completed an ostensibly unrelated task with a confederate who was not aware of the exclusion that occurred during the ball-tossing game. Experiment 1 demonstrated that people who were recently excluded from a social group mimicked the behaviors of a confederate more than people who were included in that group. Experiment 2 extended this finding by showing that, in addition to creating liking and rapport, mimicking the behaviors of others may also address threatened belongingness needs. Specifically, female participants were excluded by an all-female or all-male group and then interacted with a female or male confederate. When the female participants were excluded by an ingroup (i.e., females) and the confederate was also an ingroup member, they mimicked the behavior of that confederate more. This effect ii appeared to be mediated by belongingness threat. To the extent that individuals felt that they had been excluded from the group and that they belonged to that group, they mimicked the ingroup confederate more. The results of these experiments suggest that mimicking the behaviors of others may be a pro-social consequence of being excluded. In addition, people may be able to regulate their group identities by affiliating through mimicry, even though mimicry happens without intention, awareness, or conscious control. This suggests that nonconsciously mimicking the behaviors of others is functional and adaptive, and furthers the perspective that the unconscious is flexible and able to adapt to new situations. iii Dedicated to Nick iv ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would first like to thank my adviser, Dr. Robert Arkin, for his guidance, encouragement, and mentoring over the past five years. Without him, my graduate school years would certainly have been less fulfilling, less interesting, and less enjoyable. I would also like to thank the members of my dissertation committee, Dr. Tanya Chartrand and Dr. Marilynn Brewer. Tanya, you have served more as an advisor than a committee member, and I am enormously grateful that our time at Ohio State overlapped and we had the opportunity to work together. Marilynn, your excitement and interest in this line of research has been extremely motivating, and I am thankful for all of the discussions that we have had about this research and the guidance that you have offered along the way. I would be remiss if I did not also acknowledge the work of the many dedicated undergraduate research assistants who were instrumental in helping me to complete these (labor-intensive) studies: Joshua Gill, Aletheia Henry, Adam McClintock, Laurie Rathbun, Andy Samuelsson, Julie Swecker, and Megan Watts. I would also like to thank Desiree Rodriguez for the innumerable hours that she spent coding the videotaped mimicry data. Members of Arkin Lab, Chartrand Lab, and the Social Cognition Research Group deserve appreciation for having heard many presentations of the studies presented in this paper and not complaining (too much) about my repeated use of the same slides. v These presentations have been helpful in allowing me to discuss these studies and develop a better understanding of their implications. Stephanie Tobin and Valerie Jefferis deserve special thanks for allowing me to call them at weird hours and ask obscure statistical questions or talk (sometimes brag) about the results of these studies. On a more personal level, I would like to thank my friends, especially Jenny, Bobbi, Chris, Mike, and Pete, for their friendship and support. You can finally quit asking me when I will be done with school, and I will finally stop complaining about being a 27-year-old student. To my parents and brother, who have provided me with endless love and support my entire life: you deserve at least some recognition for getting me to this point. And finally, to Nick, my soon-to-be husband: I could not be more grateful for Mr. Clark’s U.S. History class. Your love, support, and dedication are invaluable. I simply can’t imagine my life without you in it, and I can’t wait to spend the rest of it with you. vi VITA June 28, 1976 Born – Merrillville, Indiana 1998 B.A. Psychology, Butler University 1998-1999 University Fellow The Ohio State University 1999-2000 Pre-Doctoral Fellow National Institute of Mental Health 2000 M.A. Psychology, The Ohio State University 2000-2002 Graduate Teaching and Research Associate The Ohio State University 2002-2003 University Fellow The Ohio State University PUBLICATIONS Arkin, R. M., & Lakin, J. L. (2001). The Taj Mahal of selves. Psychological Inquiry, 12(4), 203-205. Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, 334-339. Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The Chameleon Effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 145-162. vii FIELDS OF STUDY Major Field: Psychology Minor Fields: Social Psychology Quantitative Psychology viii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Abstract ii Dedication iv Acknowledgments v Vita vii List of Figures xii Chapters: 1. Introduction 1 1.1 Evidence for the Importance of Belonging 2 1.1.1 Need to Belong 2 1.1.2 Evolutionary Psychology 4 1.1.3 Social Exclusion 5 1.1.4 Ostracism 8 1.1.5 Summary 10 1.2 Nonconscious Behavioral Mimicry 11 1.2.1 Evidence for the Occurrence of Behavioral Mimicry 11 1.2.2 Bi-directional Relationship between Mimicry and Rapport 14 1.3 Link between Exclusion and Behavioral Mimicry 17 2. Experiment 1: Mimicry and Exclusion 20 2.1 Hypotheses 20 2.2 Method 20 2.2.1 Participants 20 2.2.2 Procedure 21 ix 2.2.3 Debriefing 25 2.3 Results 26 2.3.1 Manipulation Checks 26 2.3.1.1 Affect 26 2.3.1.2 Enjoyment of Cyberball 27 2.3.1.3 Evaluation of Other Cyberball Players 27 2.3.1.4 Summary 28 2.3.2 Interjudge Reliability 28 2.3.3 Mimicry Measure 29 2.3.4 Relationship between Exclusion, Negative Affect, and Mimicry 29 2.3.5 Photo Description Task and Partner Ratings 30 2.3.6 Confederates’ Ratings 31 2.4 Discussion 31 3. Experiment 2: Mimicry and Threatened Belongingness Needs 36 3.1 Hypotheses 37 3.1.1 Exclusion by an All-Female Group 37 3.1.2 Exclusion by an All-Male Group 38 3.2 Method 39 3.2.1 Participants 39 3.2.2 Procedure 39 3.2.3 Debriefing 41 3.3 Results 43 3.3.1 Manipulation Checks 43 3.3.1.1 Sex Identification of Cyberball Partners and Photo Description Task Partner 43 3.3.1.2 Affect 43 3.3.1.3 Enjoyment of Cyberball and Evaluation of Other Cyberball Players 44 3.3.1.4 Summary 45 3.3.2 State Self-Esteem 46 3.3.3 Ostracism Model Needs 46 3.3.3.1 Belongingness 47 3.3.3.2 Self-Esteem 47 3.3.3.3 Meaningful Existence 48 3.3.3.4 Control 48 3.3.4 Interjudge Reliability 48 3.3.5 Mimicry Measure 49 3.3.6 Positive Affect, State Self-Esteem, Ostracism Model Needs, and Mimicry 50 3.3.7 Attributions for Exclusion 52 3.3.7.1 Female Exclusion Condition 52 x 3.3.7.2 Male Exclusion Condition 52 3.3.8 Photo Description Task and Partner Ratings 52 3.3.9 Confederates’ Ratings 53 3.4 Discussion 55 4. General Discussion 60 4.1 Summary of Effects 60 4.2 Implications for Exclusion Literatures 61 4.3 Implications for Nonconscious Behavioral Mimicry Literature 63 4.4 Future Directions 67 4.5 Conclusion 70 Appendix A: Experiment 1 Materials 71 Appendix B: Experiment 1 Figure 81 Appendix C: Experiment 2 Materials 83 Appendix D: Experiment 2 Figure 98 List of References 100 Endnotes 110 xi LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1 Covariate-adjusted behavioral mimicry means as a function of condition, Experiment 1 82 2 Covariate-adjusted behavioral mimicry means as a function of condition and sex of confederate, Experiment 2 99 xii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Human beings are social animals (e.g., Aronson, 1999; Caporael, 2001b; Ehrlich, 2000; Wright, 1994). From dawn to dusk (and sometimes from dusk to dawn), our lives are filled with social interactions. We talk to significant others and children as we get ready for work, discuss the events of the previous evening with our co-workers, eat lunch with colleagues before meeting with others during the afternoon, and spend the evening cementing bonds with friends and family members. The number of social interactions that most individuals have on a daily basis – ranging from the superficial to the profound – is extraordinary.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    124 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us