Understanding and Sharing Intentions: the Origins of Cultural Cognition

Understanding and Sharing Intentions: the Origins of Cultural Cognition

BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2005) 28, 675–735 Printed in the United States of America Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition Michael Tomasello, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne, and Henrike Moll Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] http://www.eva.mpg.de/psycho/ Abstract: We propose that the crucial difference between human cognition and that of other species is the ability to participate with others in collaborative activities with shared goals and intentions: shared intentionality. Participation in such activities requires not only especially powerful forms of intention reading and cultural learning, but also a unique motivation to share psychological states with oth- ers and unique forms of cognitive representation for doing so. The result of participating in these activities is species-unique forms of cultural cognition and evolution, enabling everything from the creation and use of linguistic symbols to the construction of social norms and individual beliefs to the establishment of social institutions. In support of this proposal we argue and present evidence that great apes (and some children with autism) understand the basics of intentional action, but they still do not participate in activities involving joint intentions and attention (shared intentionality). Human children’s skills of shared intentionality develop gradually during the first 14 months of life as two ontogenetic pathways intertwine: (1) the general ape line of understanding others as animate, goal-directed, and intentional agents; and (2) a species-unique motivation to share emotions, experience, and activities with other persons. The develop- mental outcome is children’s ability to construct dialogic cognitive representations, which enable them to participate in earnest in the collectivity that is human cognition. Keywords: collaboration; cooperation; cultural learning; culture; evolutionary psychology; intentions; shared intentionality; social cog- nition; social learning; theory of mind; joint attention Human beings are the world’s experts at mind reading. As complexity over historical time. Children become more skill- compared with other species, humans are much more skill- ful at collaborating and interacting with others culturally ful at discerning what others are perceiving, intending, de- throughout early childhood, but their first nascent attempts siring, knowing, and believing. Although the pinnacle of begin, once again, at around the first birthday. mind reading is understanding beliefs – as beliefs are in- Tomasello et al. (1993) argued and presented evidence disputably mental and normative – the foundational skill is that these two dimensions of human expertise – reading in- understanding intentions. Understanding intentions is tentions and interacting with others culturally – are inti- foundational because it provides the interpretive matrix for mately related. Specifically, the way humans understand deciding precisely what it is that someone is doing in the the intentional actions and perceptions of others creates first place. Thus, the exact same physical movement may be species-unique forms of cultural learning and engagement, seen as giving an object, sharing it, loaning it, moving it, get- which then lead to species-unique processes of cultural ting rid of it, returning it, trading it, selling it, and on and cognition and evolution. For example, it is only if a young on – depending on the goals and intentions of the actor. child understands other persons as intentional agents that And whereas understanding beliefs does not emerge until she can acquire and use linguistic symbols – because the around age 4 in human ontogeny, understanding intentions learning and use of symbols requires an understanding that begins to emerge at around a child’s first birthday. the partner can voluntarily direct actions and attention to Human beings are also the world’s experts at culture. Hu- outside entities. Indeed, material and symbolic artifacts of mans do not just interact with conspecifics socially, as do all kinds, including even complex social institutions, are in many animal species, but they also engage with them in com- an important sense intentionally constituted (Bloom 1996; plex collaborative activities such as making a tool together, Searle 1995; Tomasello 1999a). preparing a meal together, building a shelter together, play- Recently, however, some new empirical findings have ing a cooperative game, collaborating scientifically, and on emerged which suggest that understanding intentions can- and on. These collective activities and practices are often not be the whole story of cultural cognition. Briefly, the structured by shared symbolic artifacts, such as linguistic main finding is that some nonhuman primates understand symbols and social institutions, facilitating their “transmis- more about intentional action and perceptions than was sion” across generations in ways that ratchet them up in previously believed (and this is also true, to some degree, of Downloaded© 2005 from https://www.cambridge.org/coreCambridge University Press. Emory 0140-525X/05 University, on 30 $12.50 Oct 2019 at 19:20:00, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms675 . https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05470120 Tomasello et al.: Understanding and sharing intentions children with autism). But they do not thereby engage so- and social engagements that make possible uniquely human cially and culturally with others in the ways that human activities such as the creation and use of linguistic and children do. Therefore, understanding the intentional ac- mathematical symbols, the creation and use of artifacts and tions and perceptions of others is not by itself sufficient to technologies that accumulate modifications over genera- produce humanlike social and cultural activities. Some- tions in cultural evolution, and the creation of social prac- thing additional is required. tices and institutions such as marriage and government that Our hypothesis for this “something additional” is shared depend on collective beliefs – in short, what we will call intentionality. We propose that human beings, and only hu- skills of cultural cognition. man beings, are biologically adapted for participating in col- laborative activities involving shared goals and socially co- ordinated action plans (joint intentions). Interactions of 1. Intentional action this type require not only an understanding of the goals, in- tentions, and perceptions of other persons, but also, in ad- If we want to know how people understand intentional ac- dition, a motivation to share these things in interaction with tion, we must first have a model of exactly what intentional others – and perhaps special forms of dialogic cognitive action is. Here we propose a simple model based on con- representation for doing so. The motivations and skills for trol-systems principles – in which goal, action, and percep- participating in this kind of “we” intentionality are woven tual monitoring are all seen as components in the larger into the earliest stages of human ontogeny and underlie adaptive system that serves to regulate the organism’s be- young children’s developing ability to participate in the col- havioral interactions with the environment. lectivity that is human cognition. As discovered by cyberneticians such as Weiner (1948) In this article, we explicate and elaborate this account of and Ashby (1956), machines that act on their own “intel- how humans come to (1) understand intentional action and ligently” all have the same basic organization involving the (2) participate in activities involving shared intentionality. same three components: (1) a reference value or goal to- Our focus is on how these two skills interweave during nor- ward which the system acts, (2) the ability to act in order mal human ontogeny, but we also review recent empirical to change the environment, and (3) the ability to perceive findings with great apes and children with autism, provid- the environment so as to know when the state of the envi- ing the skeleton of an evolutionary account in the process. ronment matches the reference value. The prototypical We employ a “control systems” approach (from cybernetic exemplar, of course, is the thermostat which – all by itself theory) to characterize the structure of intentional action without human intervention – can regulate the tempera- and a “shared intentionality” approach (from the philoso- ture of a room. It does this by (1) having a reference value phy of action) to characterize the types of cognitive skills set by a human (e.g., 25 degrees), (2) being able to turn on or off an air heater or cooler, and (3) being able to sense the room temperature (e.g., with a thermometer) and Michael Tomasello is Co-Director of the Max Planck compare it to the reference value to determine whether Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Ger- heating, cooling, or no action is required. This circular or- many. Research interests focus on processes of social ganization – goal determines action, which changes per- cognition, social learning, and communication/langu- ception (feedback), which (when compared to goal) again age in human children and great apes. determines action – makes the thermostat a self-regulat- Malinda Carpenter is a Staff Scientist in the De- ing device.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    61 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us