The Impact of Learning on Sexual Selection and Speciation

The Impact of Learning on Sexual Selection and Speciation

Review The impact of learning on sexual selection and speciation 1 2 3 4 Machteld N. Verzijden , Carel ten Cate , Maria R. Servedio , Genevieve M. Kozak , 5 1 Jenny W. Boughman and Erik I. Svensson 1 Department of Biology, Lund University, SE-22362, Lund, Sweden 2 Institute of Biology, Leiden University, PO Box 9505, 2300 RA Leiden, the Netherlands 3 Department of Biology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA 4 University of Illinois, Champaign, IL 61820, USA 5 Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824, USA Learning is widespread in nature, occurring in most In this review, we focus on the effects of learning on animal taxa and in several different ecological contexts mate choice and its consequences for sexual selection and and, thus, might play a key role in evolutionary process- speciation. Mate choice typically requires preferences of es. Here, we review the accumulating empirical evidence one sex for certain traits in the other; therefore, we exam- for the involvement of learning in mate choice and the ine both mate preference learning and learning of mating consequences for sexual selection and reproductive iso- traits. lation. We distinguish two broad categories: learned The study of the role of learning in the development of mate preferences and learned traits under mate selec- mate choice has a long track record that has provided tion (such as bird song). We point out that the context of insight into how widespread and diverse this phenomenon learning, namely how and when learning takes place, is. A summary of sexual imprinting and other forms of often makes a crucial difference to the predicted evolu- preference learning (including examples of each) is provid- tionary outcome. Factors causing biases in learning and ed in Box 1. Unlike learned preferences, the learning of when one should expect the evolution of learning itself traits under sexual selection has mainly been shown in the are also explored. song of oscine passerines (reviewed recently in several papers e.g., [3–5]). However, a few examples are emerging Learning as a driving force in evolutionary change from other behaviors and taxa, such as courtship displays It is now increasingly appreciated that several non-genetic and human behavior (e.g., [6–9]). Learned behavior is mechanisms can influence evolutionary change (e.g., [1]), challenging to demonstrate, because controlled rearing and among these are learned behaviors. Learning, which experiments are often necessary to be able to convincingly has been studied in depth in many species, is a common demonstrate learning. Therefore, we expect more exam- process involved in the development of behavior. Although ples of learned preferences and traits to accumulate as this research field has long been separated from the study researchers look for this evidence more specifically. of genetic evolution, connections between these disciplines have recently received attention [2]. In particular, the The relationship between learned mate choice and number of studies connecting the development of behavior phenotypic plasticity with studies of speciation and sexual selection has in- Learning can be viewed as a form of phenotypic plasticity. creased rapidly. Concurrently, theoretical developments Thus, key concepts from research in plasticity could be are uncovering the effects of learned preferences and traits used in learning research to facilitate integration between on the dynamics of evolution in these contexts. the proximate research tradition on learning and evolu- We discern two broad categories of learned behavior tionary research terminology. The concept of reaction directly affecting mate choice: learned behavior that affects norms is central to quantitative genetics and phenotypic preferences for mates, and learned traits that are the direct plasticity [10] and can be applied to mate choice. Specifi- targets of mate choice. Therefore, we define learning here cally, we suggest that, in the case of mate choice, reaction in the context of mate choice as a process that affects the norms become analogous to preference functions, which development of preferences and behavior involved in mate have been used traditionally to model preferences for male choice or mate attraction, through exposure to stimuli that secondary sexual traits [11]. Mate preference functions constitute the specific phenotypes or behavior of potential quantify the mating response as a continuous function of mates, conspecifics, or individuals of other species. Mere variation in a quantitative male trait (i.e., a length of a exposure to (in the case of imprinting), or interaction with tail), and can differ in linear slopes, curvature, or intercept (in the case of social or reinforced learning), these stimuli [11] (Figure 2a). For instance, increasing the amount of the can change responses to the same or similar stimuli on cue could result in a stronger mate response and increased future encounters. propensity to mate (resulting in directional sexual selec- tion). In Figure 2, we present the simplest scenario: when Corresponding author: Verzijden, M.N. ([email protected]) preference functions differ only in slope so that curvature 0169-5347/$ – see front matter ß 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2012.05.007 Trends in Ecology and Evolution, September 2012, Vol. 27, No. 9 511 Review Trends in Ecology and Evolution September 2012, Vol. 27, No. 9 result of learning, variation in preference functions be- Box 1. Evidence for learned mate preferences tween individuals can be introduced in later life stages Males and females of many species learn their mate preferences, even when such variation did not exist in earlier ones. and learning can take place throughout life. When learning occurs at an early stage of life, affecting pair formation at a much later stage in life, this is called sexual imprinting [65]. This requires a period of Theoretical predictions of the evolutionary social interactions with, usually, closely related individuals (the consequences of learning during mate choice imprinting set [13]), such as parents and siblings. Phenotypic traits, The existence of preferences and traits that can be acquired such as visual or olfactory ones, are learned from these models, or modified by learning can have consequences for both the resulting in the learner being able to discriminate its own species action of sexual selection and the evolution of pre-mating and sex (Figure 1g–h, main text). Prime examples of sexual imprinting come from birds, with over 100 species from 15 different isolation during speciation. Although these consequences orders demonstrating this behavior [32], even in brood parasitic have been investigated to some extent by mathematical species [66]. Recent studies have shown that other groups of models, such studies are relatively few in number. Learning vertebrates with parental care also show sexual imprinting, includ- is a multifaceted phenomenon in part because different ing some mammals (sheep and goats [67]) and fish (cichlids and sticklebacks [68–70]; Figure 1e, main text). Several species of cases vary in the segment of the population that individuals poeciliid fish demonstrate learning while juveniles through expo- learn from (the generalizable concept of the ‘imprinting set’ sure to phenotypes in their population (oblique or horizontal [13]) or how this learning occurs, and in part because, as imprinting sets), which affects both within- and between-species discussed above, both preferences and traits can be learned. mate preferences [59,71]. Both factors can have profound effects on the predicted Mate preferences can also be learned when individuals are independent and mature. In many cases, individuals learn through evolutionary consequences of learning. Although some tan- experience with potential mates. For instance, female Syrian talizing, but tentative, conclusions can be drawn from cur- hamsters [72], male guppies [73], male fruit flies of at least three rent theoretical studies, this area begs more thorough species [51,74], and Calopteryx damselfly females (Figure 1a,b, main investigation. text) [53] learn to discriminate against heterospecific mates after courtship interactions. In other species of damselflies with female- limited polymorphism, males learn to prefer the morph with which Sexual selection they had a successful mating experience [63,64]. For the case of sexual selection per se, mathematical In some cases, individuals need experience with variation in models that have investigated the consequences of learn- phenotypes before expressing a preference. For example, female ing have arrived at few generalities. Mating preferences field crickets and female treehoppers become choosier with acquired by parental imprinting, for example, have been experience, depending on the quality of the male with which they interacted [75–78]. Likewise, female wolf spiders from polymorphic found to stabilize sexual selection onto a specific trait populations prefer ornamented males, but only after experience phenotype in some situations [14,15], but can cause run- with both ornamented and unornamented males [52,79]. away sexual selection under other conditions, including Individuals can also learn from the experience of another when preferences are also affected by a directional bias individual with mates, and copy the mate choice of others [80], either by choosing that same individual (e.g., [18]), or generalizing ([14,16], see also [17]).

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    9 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us