Scaffolding Under the Microscope: Applying Selfregulation and Other­Regulation Perspectives to a Scaffolded Task

Scaffolding Under the Microscope: Applying Selfregulation and Other­Regulation Perspectives to a Scaffolded Task

Scaffolding under the microscope: applying selfregulation and other-regulation perspectives to a scaffolded task Article (Accepted Version) Leith, Georgia, Yuill, Nicola and Pike, Alison (2018) Scaffolding under the microscope: applying selfregulation and other-regulation perspectives to a scaffolded task. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 88 (2). pp. 174-191. ISSN 0007-0998 This version is available from Sussex Research Online: http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/69958/ This document is made available in accordance with publisher policies and may differ from the published version or from the version of record. If you wish to cite this item you are advised to consult the publisher’s version. Please see the URL above for details on accessing the published version. Copyright and reuse: Sussex Research Online is a digital repository of the research output of the University. Copyright and all moral rights to the version of the paper presented here belong to the individual author(s) and/or other copyright owners. To the extent reasonable and practicable, the material made available in SRO has been checked for eligibility before being made available. Copies of full text items generally can be reproduced, displayed or performed and given to third parties in any format or medium for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge, provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. http://sro.sussex.ac.uk SCAFFOLDING UNDER THE MICROSCOPE Scaffolding under the microscope: applying self-regulation and other-regulation perspectives to a scaffolded task Abstract Background Typical scaffolding coding schemes provide overall scores to compare across a sample. As such, insights into the scaffolding process can be obscured: the child’s contribution to the learning; the particular skills being taught and learned; and the overall changes in amount of scaffolding over the course of the task. Aims This paper applies a transition of regulation framework to scaffolding coding, using a self-regulation and other-regulation coding scheme, to explore how rich and detailed data on mother-child dyadic interactions fit alongside collapsed sample-level scores. Sample Data of seventy-eight mother-child dyads (M age = 9 years 10 months) from the Sisters and Brothers Study (SIBS: Pike et al., 2006) were used for this analysis. Methods Videos of the mother and child completing a block design puzzle task, involving multiple trials, at home were coded for their different self- and other-regulation skills at the end of every block design trial. Results These constructs were examined at a sample level, providing general findings about typical patterns of self-regulation and other-regulation. Seven exemplar families at different ends of the spectrum were then extracted for fine-grained examination, showing substantial trial- and behaviour-related differences between seemingly similarly scoring families. Conclusion This coding scheme demonstrated the value of exploring perspectives of a mother-child tutoring task aligned to the concept of other-regulation, and investigating features of the interaction that go undetected in existing scaffolding coding schemes. 1 SCAFFOLDING UNDER THE MICROSCOPE Introduction The metaphor of ‘scaffolding’ (Wood, Bruner & Ross, 1976), now entering its fifth decade, is used throughout developmental and cognitive psychology, applied to various topics (Granott, 2005), and operationalised in diverse ways for research purposes. Global maternal ‘scaffolding quality’ scores, compared across dyads, have shown their value in the wealth of existing research. However, these varied operationalisations of scaffolding introduce some challenges to the research area. Some measures show a drift from the fundamental principles of the theory of scaffolding. Furthermore, reducing down detailed behavioural observations for overall ‘scores’ may obscure interesting and important between- family differences. While reconfiguring the original scaffolding metaphor for empirical studies is a welcome scientific endeavour (Granott, 2005), there is value in looking back to the original ideas from which it emerged (e.g., Gauvain, 2005; Lajoie, 2005). This paper aims to return to the early principles, and bridge the detailed, process-oriented, dynamic analyses from the early scaffolding literature and the more quantitative, outcome-focused assessments of scaffolding quality more commonly used today. We describe an adapted coding scheme that tracks the three principles of scaffolding (transfer of responsibility, contingency and fading) which are not all always built into existing scaffolding coding schemes. Drawing on fine- grained coding practices, we investigated the tutoring and learning process during a videotaped task (see Carr & Pike, 2012), and aimed to apply an adapted coding scheme that describes specific characteristics of individual mother-child dyads, to examine what gets lost when collapsing these data for global scores to compare across families. 2 SCAFFOLDING UNDER THE MICROSCOPE We shall first provide some overall context in which to position this work, by outlining the conceptual features of scaffolding and detailing how they may or may not fit with existing coding schemes. Transfer of responsibility While the feature transfer of responsibility is often referred to, it does not describe what is actually taking place for the child to take on more responsibility of the task. One insight into how enables this shift in ownership comes about was by Wertsch (1979), a contemporary of Wood’s, whose work neatly dovetails his own. Wertsch’s observations of mother-child tutoring interactions mapped Vygotskian theories of socio-cognitive learning (Vygotsky, 1978) onto actual interactions. He described the social learning process as a ‘transition of regulation’, emphasising that in tutored sessions children develop the self- regulation skills required for the task; by extension, adults demonstrate and model these self- regulation skills for the child (other-regulating). Scaffolding can then be understood as the strategies the mother uses to aid the transfer of regulation to the child over the course of the task. This perspective has since been used to describe the tutoring process more generally (e.g., Díaz, Neal, & Amaya-Williams, 1990; Lajoie, 2005). While maternal scaffolding could be conceptually understood as other-regulating behaviour, the actual measurement of scaffolding usually does not explicitly reflect other- regulation behaviours. Instead, overall support is often measured, either by the amount of support (e.g., Carr & Pike, 2012; Conner & Cross, 2003; Fernandes-Richards, 2006; Pratt, Kerig, Cowan, & Cowan, 1988; Wood, Wood, Ainsworth, & O’Malley, 1995) or the appropriateness of support (e.g., Englund, Luckner, Whaley, & Egeland, 2004; Hammond, Carpendale, Bibok & Liebermann-Finestone, 2012). Where support behaviours are coded separately, they tend to be classified by explicit actions (e.g. Wood, Bruner & Ross, 1976; 3 SCAFFOLDING UNDER THE MICROSCOPE Lindberg, Hyde & Hirsch, 2008) or by whether the support is cognitive or emotional (Pianta & Harbers, 1996), rather than by the specific regulation behaviour being modelled for the child to internalise. Given that Wertsch’s notion of regulation transition is bolstered by statistical associations between maternal scaffolding quality and both the child’s later self-regulation skills (e.g., Neitzel & Stright, 2003; Pino-Pasternak & Whitebread, 2010; Stright, Neitzel, Sears, & Hoke-Sinex, 2001), and indeed skill development within a task (Pino-Pasternak, Whitebread & Tolmie, 2010), it is surprising that other-regulation is rarely operationalised for tutoring research (two exceptions are Nader-Grosbois, Normandeau, Ricard-Cossette, & Quintal, 2008; and Hadwin, Wozney, & Pontin, 2005). The self-regulatory skills internalised by the child during a scaffolding interaction tend not to be examined (see Pino-Pasternak et al., 2010 for an exception), so few self- and other-regulation coding schemes exist. In contrast, in the literature on technology, the other-regulating role of the device is clearly considered in coding schemes (see Lajoie, 2005). Once such scheme is used in this paper and applied to scaffolding, to observe the emerging self-regulation of the child, along with the corresponding other-regulation of the mother, to operationalise the original concept of the transfer of responsibility, and plot how, in addition to whether, optimal scaffolding took place. Contingency Another key concept of scaffolding is that the mother’s support is contingent on the child’s own behaviour and actions, or when the child signals support is needed. While the tutor’s behaviour and skills are crucial to the process and product of an interaction, the contribution of the child is also a determining factor: the child’s own effort “assists the adult to assist” (Tharp & Gallimore, 1998, p. 101, original italics). Vygotsky (1978) described 4 SCAFFOLDING UNDER THE MICROSCOPE learning as an internalisation by children of behaviours externally modelled by the ‘expert’, which requires effort on behalf of the child; they are active participants in their own learning experience. A mother can scaffold in a highly contingent way, but unless the child is also able to clearly signal need for assistance and willing to internalise the lessons, it

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    39 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us