The Role of Attachment and Self-Regulation in The

The Role of Attachment and Self-Regulation in The

Forum on Public Policy The Role of Attachment and Self-Regulation in the Etiology of Residential Treatment of Childhood Sexual Behavior Problems and the Development of Public Policy on Early Childhood Literacy Barbara E. Simpson, Department of Psychology, Gustavus Adolphus College Abstract As the title suggests, it will be suggest in this paper that attachment is a useful concept and that attachment disorders need to be addressed for infants and children to develop into responsible, caring, and mature adults for whom the everyday ability to learn from experience and adapt appropriately to an ever changing world. There will be no attempt to argue that sex offense and early literacy are tied in any way except through the concept of attachment disorder, because children, adolescents, and adults offend for so many different reasons that there is no one underlying pathology let alone one centered on attachment. However, it will be argued there is something about those adolescent sex offenders who remain attachment disordered at ages 12 to 18 that can inform the discussion of the role of attachment and attachment deficits in early literacy and that differentiate the disordered from the normal capacity to develop language and to learn how to read and write. The Nature of the Problem In this paper, attachment will be defined as the bonding of an adult caregiver and an infant, to the end that the infant develops trust in a caregiver and then progressively explores and masters the environment into which it has been born. Those who first articulated the concept of attachment, John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth (Ainsworth & Bowlby, 1991), focused on its relationship to species adaptation. They argued that species survival depended on an infant being cared for and taught to make sense out of an otherwise unintelligible world and that the infant’s doing so was predicated on its ability to form an attachment bond with what they called a preferred attachment figure. More specifically, they and others (Eisenberg, Sadovsky, & Spinrad, 2005; Posner & Rothbart, 2009; Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2005) argued that attachment to a preferred caregiver was the mechanism, by which an infant, and later the child came to learn. To this point, each argued that any substantial disturbance of a child’s attachment to a primary caregiver would damage both an infant’s and a child’s potential for social bonding and the development of a sense of self and for its earning how to learn. As a consequence, children, who were not securely attached to a primary caregiver, would fail to develop an adequate set of constructs with which to interact with things outside self and become progressively alienated from reality, fail to develop culturally relevant norms, values, and expectations, tax sorely those who would try to teach them, and, in extreme cases, even die. As the literature on attachment has developed (Posner & Rothbart, 2009; Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2005), attachment, whether secure or disorganized, has become a powerful lens through which to understand development broadly enough to speak to the matter of two areas of human concern as widely divergent as the early acquisition of literacy and the development of a subset of the most seriously disturbed of young adolescents, the attachment disordered adolescent male sex offender. 1 Forum on Public Policy Although most adolescent sex offenders are normal, healthy teenagers, apart from the fact that they have made at least one very bad sexual choice, those who get sent to and treated in residential centers like the Hoffmann Center in St Peter Minnesota are attachment disordered and greatly at risk. Those who have come to and been treated at this center are, progressively, somewhere between four and nine years behind same aged peers in social development and educationally one to four grade levels below grade standard. Those who have come, in recent years, have been identified at admission to the program to have been toddlers and pre-school children who had demonstrated pervasive learning disorders. It is these young men to whom the literature of this paper is addressed. The Literature Of Attachment And Of Attachment Disorders Attachment Although Bowlby and Ainsworth had focused their early work on the evolutionary advantage attachment provided, they soon began to differentiate the adaptive value of different forms of attachment Identifying a successful, e.g., a secure attachment as a capacity for selective relationship that emerged in the first year of life, laid the cognitive and emotional foundation on which a child built a sense of self and came to construe meaning more broadly defined (Kinniburgh, Blaustein, Spinazzola, & Van der Kolk, 2005). This literature of attachment suggested that, for the first several months of life, an infant will focus attention on anyone who smiles or provides comfort, but that, at approximately seven to nine months, will begin to identify a caregiver, or caregiver dyad, as a preferred attachment figure (Ainsworth, Bell & Stanton, 1974). At this point, the infant will begin to become shy around unfamiliar adults (stranger wariness) and protest separations (demonstrate separation protest) from the preferred attachment figure (Ainsworth, Blehar, Walters, & Wall, 1978). Once this shyness is established, an infant is to have become attached. Attachment is focused, it is directed toward establishing trust and becomes the base from which the infant begins to explore its environs and make sense out of his or her experience. That is, a very personal relationship becomes the ground out of which the infant makes meaning. By definition, then, attachment exchanges are, reciprocal. Marvin et al., (Marvin, Cooper, Hoffman, & Powell, 2002), using Bowlby’s and Ainsworth’s categories of attachment, identified the behaviors under which a secure attachment was developed. The first was that the preferred attachment figure had to be securely enough established to understand both self and other and to respond appropriately to the needs articulated by the infant. The second was that the preferred attachment figure had to decode and appropriately attend to needs expressed by the infant. These needs are both physical, related to bodily functioning, and social, infant and social, encompassing the infant’s early attempts to babble, smile, demonstrate delight and otherwise engage in social interaction. The third was that the preferred attachment figure had to correctly mirror back to the infant the various messages that the infant sends as the infant sends them. While what the preferred caregiver focuses on is important, it is the mirroring back to the infant the reciprocal of the infant’s experience that forges attachment (Kochanska, Aksan, Prisco, 2 Forum on Public Policy & Adams, 2008). In this process of mirroring, the infant codes experience and develops concepts for understanding what has happened, and by extension, what will happen in the future (Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, & Juffer, 2003) according to the formula “the preferred caregiver responds ‘X’: therefore I am ‘Y’. Repeated pairings of the infant’s experience and the caregiver’s response, the infant to develop an internal representation (Claussen & Crittenden, 2000; Ham, & Tronick, 2009), that allows it to act (Carver, 2005), self-regulate and develop emotional balance (Eisenberg, Sadovsky & Spinrad, 2005), and form a secure attachment (Eisenberg, 2010). A secure attachment, thus, develops social competence and provides the circumstance necessary for a growing child to develop the capacity to understand emotion, self-reflect, and self-regulate. Reality, from this perspective, is socially construed. To be sure, the physical world has its own logic and rhythm, but infants, children, and later adults establish understanding by having their perceptions validated in interaction with others. When people make mistakes in construing reality, they do so at least partly because there has been some disjunct, for them, between their seeing, doing, and responding and someone else’s verifying their experience with them. To this point, both recent and early literature in the development of literacy (Sokol, Muller, Carpendale, & Young, 2010), has suggested that being appropriately situated in social context, e.g., being located in social and relational space as well as in physical space is important for effective learning. When one is not appropriately situated in social space, one can say that one has an attachment disorder. Attachment Disorders An insecure attachment leads the infant, and later, the emerging child to make mistakes in construing reality. Marvin et al. (Marvin, Cooper, Hoffmann, & Powell, 2002) demonstrated that Ainsworth’s three patterns of failed attachment could be translated into a cognitive behavioral formulation. In the first pattern, the insecure, avoidant-dismissing one, both the attachment figure and the child subordinated the more intimate attachment, e.g., care-giving interactions through a defensive focus on exploration. In the second, the insecure, ambivalent- preoccupied type, both the attachment figure and the child restricted the child’s independent exploration, focusing instead on (often ambivalent) attachment care-giving interactions to the end that the child becomes too dependent on the caregiver. In the third, the insecure, disorganized or insecure other pattern, the caregiver’s heightened fear of and/or anger toward the child’s attachment behavior leads to disorganization and/or abdication of the executive, care-

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    14 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us