How Social Feedback Processing in the Brain Shapes Collective Opinion

How Social Feedback Processing in the Brain Shapes Collective Opinion

How social feedback processing in the brain shapes collective opinion pro- cesses in the era of social media by Sven Banisch, Felix Gaisbauer and Eckehard Olbrich Max Planck Institute for Mathematics in the Sciences, Leipzig, Germany What are the mechanisms by which groups with certain social nature. While the potential for explaining collec- opinions gain public voice and force others holding a tive behaviour based on mechanisms identified in cog- different view into silence? And how does social me- nitive and social neuroscience is frequently emphasized dia play into this? Drawing on recent neuro-scientific (19, 20, 21), its integration with sociological theories of insights into the processing of social feedback, we de- collective opinion expression (15,6,7 ) is lacking. Social velop a theoretical model that allows to address these Feedback Theory (SFT) bridges this gap by formulating questions. The model captures phenomena described collective processes of opinion expression as a multi-agent by spiral of silence theory of public opinion, provides problem in which individual agents adapt according to a mechanism-based foundation for it, and allows in a reward- and value-based learning scheme identified in this way more general insight into how different group neuro-scientific research (22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27). SFT pro- structures relate to different regimes of collective opin- vides a coherent framework for modelling collective opin- ion expression. Even strong majorities can be forced ion processes that integrates recent neuro-scientific find- into silence if a minority acts as a cohesive whole. The ings, adaptive decision making (28) and political theory proposed framework of social feedback theory (SFT) of public opinion (15, 16). Such a theoretical framework highlights the need for sociological theorising to un- is essential for the analysis of societal-level implications of derstand the societal-level implications of findings in social neuroscience. social and cognitive neuroscience. Social neuroscience aims to identify neural mechanisms A better understanding of the collective processes underly- involved into the processing of social cues. to understand ing public opinion expression is crucial for a better under- their functional role in social cognition. fMRI studies have standing of modern society. Sociological models drawing shed light on the interaction and interconnectedness of on network science (1,2,3 ) and basic principles of hu- different brain regions and their functional role in social man interaction behaviour (4,5 ) have already provided cognition. While it has long been controversial if human useful insight into collective phenomena related to mass nature evolved a neural circuity specifically for handling mobilisation (6,7,8 ), societal-level change of behaviour social information or not (29, 30, 24, 20), it is now rela- (9, 10, 11) and beliefs (12). But for change to happen, for tively settled that a basic »reinforcement circuit« (23, 31) a movement to gain pace, the alternative must be voiced is strongly involved into value-based decisions and learn- by a sufficiently large group (13). And to be voiced, it ing from social feedback (32,33,34,35,36,37 ). Other brain must be perceived as something that can be said without processes interfere with this circuity (23,20,38 ) especially »fear of isolation« (14). when social situations and tasks involve higher cognitive Spiral of silence theory (15) is based on this old »law of functions such as trust (31), morality (39) or representa- opinion« (14, John Locke). It focusses on the collective tions of self and the other (40, 41). perception of what can be publicly voiced and hence im- Temporal difference reinforcement learning (TDRL) (42, pact public opinion. Noelle-Neumann assumes that hu- 43) has provided a useful computational account of the mans possess a »quasi-statistical organ« (16) to perceive brain mechanisms underlying social reward processing what can be said without being socially sanctioned. She and learning (24, 25, 26, 27). In TDRL, a new estimate frequently refers to the »social nature of man« (17) to ex- of the expected value Qt+1 associated to an action is a plain public opinion dynamics as a spiralling process in function of the current estimate Qt and the temporal dif- which silence may lead to more silence. In this paper we ference (TD) error δt between this estimate and the re- propose a mathematical model for this process based on ward that is actually obtained: Qt+1 = Qt + αδt. With reinforcement learning (RL) by social feedback (18). In a rate governed by α (referred to as learning rate) this repeated games played over a network, agents receive sig- scheme converges to a stable equilibrium in which the TD nals of approval or disapproval on expressing their opinion error δt approaches zero such that expectations and ac- to peers. They evolve an expectation about the social re- tual reception of rewards are aligned (43). The usefulness arXiv:2003.08154v1 [physics.soc-ph] 18 Mar 2020 ward obtained when expressing their opinion and remain of TDRL in computational neuroscience derives from the silent if they expect punishment (negative reward). RL finding that the activity of dopaminergic neurons in the naturally captures the assumed »quasi-statistical« opinion midbrain regions is quantitatively related to the »reward- perception. Our paper develops a mechanism-based ap- prediction error« (20) between the experienced reward proach (5) that enables a more general application of the and its expected value (22, 44, 45), that is, to δt. Social assumptions underlying spiral of silence theory and allows neuroscience has provided ample evidence that such a ba- to relate structural variations across different groups to dif- sic reward processing circuit is also highly involved into ferent regimes of collective opinion expression. peer influence processes (34, 36), social conformity (33) Moreover, we show that these modelling choices are well- and approval (35). grounded in recent neuro-scientific insights into human Given that the processing of and learning by social feed- back is so deeply routed in the human brain, it is of utter- inter-group connectivity. The probability of in-group in- most importance to better understand the collective con- fluence is q11 for group 1, and q22 for group 2. Interac- sequences of these processes. Especially in social media, tion probability across groups is denoted by q12; q21 respec- a tremendous number of quick feedback decisions is made tively. We define the structural strength of group 1 and 2 day by day by billions of users. »Like buttons« and quanti- (denoted as γ and δ) as tative markers of collective endorsement can be associated (N − 1) q (N − 1) q with low cognitive costs which suggests that a dominant γ = 1 11 and δ = 2 22 : (4) role is played by the fast value processing mechanisms ac- N2 q12 N1 q21 counted for by TDRL. Recent studies have provided some The structural strength of a group is determined by the evidence for that (36,37). While it is reasonable to assume relative size of the group and the relative in-group connec- that the social reward circuit has evolved to facilitate cohe- tivity or cohesion (48, 49). As γ and δ determine the prob- sion and cooperation in small groups (20) with intensive ability of in-group versus out-group interaction (γ=(γ + 1) pairbonding (30), this reasoning may not apply for soci- versus 1=(γ+1) for group 1), they also govern the expected eties of increased complexity (46, 47). In complex social rewards for opinion expression for individuals in the two networks, human ability to coordinate with in-groups may groups come at the expense of an increasing alienation to out- γ 1 groups and therefore drive polarization dynamics (18). (r1) = p1 − p2 − c; (5) E γ + 1 γ + 1 Here we show that SFT of opinion expression provides a neuro-biologically grounded explanation of collective pro- δ 1 E(r2) = p2 − p1 − c; (6) cesses involved into »spirals of silence« (15) and related δ + 1 δ + 1 phenomena of collective opinion expression. where the probabilities for opinion expression p1; p2 are We consider the situation that two groups with different given by (3). The Q-values are updated by Eq. (2) sub- standpoints on a controversial issue have evolved and en- stituting the agent index i by the respective group index gage in public discourse. Individuals within both opinion and the reward with the expected rewards derived above. groups can decide to express (E) their standpoint or to be As visible in Eq. (2), in TD learning the change of Q- silent (S). They receive supportive feedback from their re- values from one time step to the other is given by the TD spective in-group and negative feedback from agents in the error times the learning rate α. In the continuous time out-group when expressing their opinion. Individual inter- limit (50, 51, 52), we describe the model dynamics by a action is hence formulated as repeated opinion expression system of two differential equations games with a reward system that captures approval and _ disapproval by peers: Q1 = E(r1) − Q1 Q_ = (r ) − Q : (7) 8 −c silent neighbor 2 E 2 2 t < ri = −c + 1 agreement (1) As the right hand side is zero when the Q-value estimate : −c − 1 disagreement is equal to the expected reward, the fixed points of (7) are possible equilibria of the associated collective game.1 The parameter c corresponds to a fixed cost of expression. We apply this model to a minority-majority setting in Having received a social feedback reward during an inter- which one third of the population supports opinion 1 and action, agents update the expected value Q (A) of their i the other two thirds hold the majority view opinion 2.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    7 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us