Medical Education and Distrust Modulate the Response of Insular-Cingulate 2 Network and Ventral Striatum in Pain Diagnosis

Medical Education and Distrust Modulate the Response of Insular-Cingulate 2 Network and Ventral Striatum in Pain Diagnosis

1 Medical education and distrust modulate the response of insular-cingulate 2 network and ventral striatum in pain diagnosis. 3 Giada Dirupoa,b,c, Sabrina Totaroa, Jeanne Richardc,d and Corrado Corradi-Dell’Acquaa,b 4 5 a Theory of Pain Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and 6 Educational Sciences (FPSE), University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland. 7 b Geneva Neuroscience Center, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland. 8 c Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland. 9 d Department of Psychology, Swiss Distance University Institute, Brig, Switzerland. 10 11 Corresponding author: Giada Dirupo, University of Geneva – Campus Biotech, Ch. des Mines 12 9, CH-1211, Geneva, Switzerland. Tel: +41223790966. E-mail: [email protected] 13 14 Keywords: Medical Students, Negative Feedback, Social Conformity, Medial Prefrontal 15 Cortex, Insula. 16 1 Abstract 2 Healthcare providers often underestimate patients’ pain, sometimes even when aware of 3 their reports. This could be the effect of experience reducing sensitivity to others pain, or 4 distrust towards patients’ self-evaluations. Across multiple experiments (375 participants), we 5 tested whether senior medical students differed from younger colleagues and lay controls in 6 the way they assess people’s pain and take into consideration their feedback. We found that 7 medical training affected the sensitivity to pain faces, an effect shown by the lower ratings 8 and highlighted by a decrease in neural response of the insula and cingulate cortex. Instead, 9 distrust towards the expressions’ authenticity affected the processing of feedbacks, by 10 decreasing activity in the ventral striatum whenever patients’ self-reports matched 11 participants’ evaluations, and by promoting strong reliance on the opinion of other doctors. 12 Overall, our study underscores the multiple processes which might influence the evaluation 13 of others’ pain at the early stages of medical career. 14 1 Introduction 2 Unrelieved pain is a major medical problem worldwide, resulting in human suffering and 3 economic costs. Unlike other medical conditions, which are diagnosed through reliable tests, 4 pain is difficult to quantify objectively, and it is mainly assessed by medical practitioners 5 (physicians, nurses, medical students, etc.) using indirect information or self-reports. As such, 6 healthcare providers systematically underestimate patients’ pain (Davoudi et al., 2008; 7 Duignan and Dunn, 2008; Kappesser et al., 2006; Puntillo et al., 2003; Teske et al., 1983), a 8 phenomenon which emerges as early as during university (Xie et al., 2018), becomes more 9 pronounced with long-lasting experience in the field (Choinière et al., 1990; Davoudi et al., 10 2008), and affects prevalently women (Greenwood et al., 2018) and ethnical minorities 11 (Ghoshal et al., 2020; Kaseweter et al., 2012; Todd et al., 2000). 12 Despite extensive research, the causes of pain underestimation are still unclear. A 13 popular theory suggests a major role of medical experience. Indeed, due to their daily 14 exposure to the severe conditions, often characterized by high levels of suffering, healthcare 15 providers could have progressively changed their frame of reference of what characterizes an 16 extreme pain (Bergh and Sjöström, 1999). Hence, they might be inclined to provide less 17 intense evaluations than those individuals (e.g., the average patient) who do not share the 18 same experience. Within this framework, neuroimaging literature repeatedly implicated brain 19 regions, such as the anterior insula (AI) and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) in 20 processing other people’s pain (Ding et al., 2019; Fan et al., 2011; Jauniaux et al., 2019; Kogler 21 et al., 2020; Lamm et al., 2011; Timmers et al., 2018). As this network is partly common to that 22 involved in first-hand pain (Corradi-Dell’Acqua et al., 2016, 2011; Kogler et al., 2020; Lamm et 23 al., 2011; Zhou et al., 2020), scholars interpreted this activations in terms of empathy, whereby 1 individuals simulate the observed state on oneself (Bernhardt and Singer, 2012; Lamm et al., 2 2019; Stietz et al., 2019). Critically, expert medical practitioners exhibited lower neural 3 response in these regions (Cheng et al., 2017, 2007), whereas they appear to over-recruit 4 prefrontal structures, often implicated in regulation and control (Cheng et al., 2007), possibly 5 reflecting enhanced ability at regulating their primary empathetic responses to the sight of 6 people potential sufferance. 7 Medical experience and emotion regulation might not be sufficient for explaining the 8 entirety of the phenomenon. In clinical settings, acknowledging high levels of pain often leads 9 to the prescription of strong analgesics, which however have contraindications for patients’ 10 health (Buckeridge et al., 2010; Butler et al., 2016; Makris et al., 2014). Concerns for such side- 11 effects (e.g., opiophobia) contribute to inadequate pain treatment in medical settings 12 (Bennett and Carr, 2002; Bertrand et al., 2021; Corradi-Dell’Acqua et al., 2019), as healthcare 13 providers prioritize those cases in which pain is unequivocally established. In this perspective, 14 one study showed that doctors and nurses tended to underestimate pain in larger extent 15 when presented with cues that patients might have lied or exaggerate their ratings (Kappesser 16 et al., 2006). In a similar vein, recent qualitative investigations revealed that emergency nurses 17 tend to privilege their own personal judgments rather than patients pain-ratings 18 (Johannessen, 2019; Vuille et al., 2018). Furthermore, when their clinical assessment is 19 inconsistent with the self-reports (e.g., patients denounce high suffering while few minutes 20 before they were chatting/laughing), nurses try to convince patients to reappraise their pain, 21 or at least mark their own point of view in the clinical chart when transferring the case to a 22 colleague (Vuille et al., 2018). Thus, healthcare providers appear to scrutinize the reliability of 23 patients’ pain and, in some case, even to mistrust their reports and feedbacks, at the 24 advantage of the opinion of other medical practitioners. However, to the best of our 1 knowledge, no study investigated systematically the role played by patients’ feedbacks in pain 2 assessments, and whether this influence changes with respect to that of other physicians. 3 Psychology and neuroscience literature provided us with several tools to investigate 4 the role of social influence in individuals’ perceptual decisions and subjective experiences 5 (Cialdini and Goldstein, 2004; Klucharev et al., 2009; Schnuerch and Gibbons, 2014; Wu et al., 6 2016). In a seminal paradigm, participants evaluated the attractiveness of women and, 7 subsequently, saw a feedback representative of peers’ mean ratings (Klucharev et al., 2009). 8 Critically, when unexpectedly requested to repeat the task, participants reappraised each face 9 consistently with the feedback received. Furthermore, a large discrepancy with others’ 10 evaluations increased the neural activity in the dorsal-posterior portion of the medial 11 prefrontal cortex (dpMPFC), often implicated in prediction error, whereas agreement with 12 these feedbacks recruited the reward system in the ventral striatum (VS) (Klucharev et al., 13 2009, see also Wu et al., 2016 as meta-analysis). These effects have supported the idea that, 14 even in subjective decisions with no correct/incorrect answers, participants considered the 15 opinion of peers as an indirect proxy of their proficiency in the task (Klucharev et al., 2009). 16 The present research aims at testing the degree with which healthcare providers take 17 into account feedbacks about their pain assessment, and whether they weight in different 18 extent the opinion of the patient from that of other physicians. Across two-experiments, we 19 implemented a modified version of the social influence paradigm described above (Klucharev 20 et al., 2009; Wu et al., 2016). Here individuals appraised the pain of facial expression video- 21 clips, and subsequently were confronted with two feedbacks (see Figure 1): the self-report of 22 the person in pain (the Target of each video) and the average opinion of 20 medical 23 practitioners (hereafter, MPs). The two feedbacks occurred simultaneously, and each of them 1 could appear either at a lower, equal or higher position with respect to participants’ initial 2 rating (see methods). After 30 minutes from the end of this first session, participants were 3 unexpectedly asked to evaluate again the same facial expressions, thus providing the 4 opportunity to assess if their initial evaluations changed and, in that case, which feedback 5 (Target vs. MPs) best explained the reappraisal. In the context of pain assessments, the rating 6 of the patient should represent the most reliable source of information to which compare 7 one’s performance. Hence, individuals’ behavioral and neural responses should ideally be 8 influenced in privileged fashion by the Target‘s feedback, as opposed to that of MPs who are 9 not experiencing the pain directly. However, healthcare providers might mistrust information 10 arising from the person in pain, and privilege the one of physicians, towards which they have 11 developed positive dispositions. Hence, when confronted with feedbacks about their pain 12 assessment, they might value in greater extent the opinion of other doctors at the expense of 13 that of the patients. 14 15 Figure 1. Schematic representation of trial structure in the first session. For demonstrative 16 purposes, one author of the study (GD) is depicted in the figure expressing pain in similar way 17 to the video-clips used as stimuli (Lamm et al., 2007). 1 Results 2 Population Validation 3 In the present study, we recruited as a population of interest medical students from different 4 years of medical faculty. This population was chosen due to its feasibility in terms of 5 recruitment, and the straightforward quantification of individual experience (corresponding 6 to the year of enrollment at university, regardless of future specializations, department, 7 hierarchy, etc.).

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    56 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us