Nudging Evolutionary Mismatched Behaviors

Nudging Evolutionary Mismatched Behaviors

Evolutionary Mismatch 1 Nudging Evolutionary Mismatched Behaviors: Implications for Social Psychology and Public Policy Mark van Vugt Lianne P. De Vries Norman P. Li To appear in: J.P. Forgas, K. Fiedler, & W.D. Crano, Sydney Symposium of Social Psychology Draft: 26 April 2019 Word count: approximately 5000 (excl. references) Correspondence address: Mark van Vugt, Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Institute for Brain and Behavior Amsterdam, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, [email protected] Evolutionary Mismatch 2 Introduction Aliens visiting planet Earth would find Homo Sapiens a remarkable species. They would discover astonishing examples of human intelligence and ingenuity such as airplanes, skyscrapers, electricity, and the internet. At the same time, they would also witness our miserable failure in preventing poverty, violence, disease, and ecological disasters. Humans are both highly intelligent and highly stupid, the alien anthropologists would probably conclude. It is true, we humans are very good at some things but very bad at others. People are able to recognize the faces of persons they met many years ago, but they easily misremember the telephone numbers of their best friends. People are very patient in accumulating knowledge and training skills through a long period of intense education, but they are not patient enough to save money for their pension or resist the temptation of a Big Mac. And, although most people would be reluctant to steal from shopkeepers, these same individuals have no moral reservations to illegally download music and movies from the net. Why is it that humans are very good at some things but very bad at others? Why is it that we can learn some things much quicker and easier than others? What does this tell us about human psychology, and what can we learn from this for public policy? Here we argue that the human mind, with all its strengths and weaknesses, follows a logical, predictable pattern. If we are able to uncover these patterns it enables us to design better interventions that take into account the constraints of human nature. So far, public policy programs have been built on the model of Homo Economicus, the idea that humans guide their actions on the basis of a rational calculation of costs and benefits (Fox & Sitkin, 2015; Loewenstein, Bryce, Hagmann, & Rajpal, 2015). However, as we shall see, the behavior of Homo Sapiens is guided by a deeper evolutionary rationality that consists of a set of instincts, or evolved preferences that can depart dramatically from what appears to be rational (Johnson, Price & Van Vugt, 2014; Kenrick, Griskevicius, Neuberg & Schaller, 2010). Take an intervention program for Evolutionary Mismatch 3 increasing sign-up rates for post-mortem organ donations. Simply changing the default option from opt-in to opt-out doubles participation (from 42% to 82%) without any change in underlying beliefs or preferences (Johnson & Goldstein, 2003). Such “nudges” have been found to be fairly effective at changing people’s behaviors in part because they pay attention to the constraints of human psychology (Baumard, 2015). The core hypothesis put forward here is that the human mind evolved to solve particular challenges very efficiently. But these challenges were part of a relatively stable environment in which humans lived for many thousands of generations, the African Savannah out of which anatomically modern humans migrated some 60,000 years ago. Yet when environments change quickly – as they increasingly have been doing -- new threats and opportunities emerge to which our minds may not be perfectly calibrated. The result is evolutionary mismatch. In this contribution we will first investigate the concept of mismatch as a core tenet of evolutionary theory and evolutionary psychology and how mismatch affects our preferences and decisions. We also discuss how to find evidence for mismatched behaviors. We will then discuss the implications of the mismatch hypothesis for some core domains of social psychology, from close relationships to work, politics, and health and sustainability. Finally, we note the implications of mismatch for public policy and how to design interventions, such as nudging strategies, that are better aligned with the core psychology of Homo Sapiens. Evolutionary challenges and psychological mechanisms The mismatch hypothesis is one of the foundational principles of evolutionary psychology (Li, Van Vugt & Colarelli, 2018). Evolutionary psychology considers human cognition, emotion, and decision-making to be the products of psychological mechanisms that evolved to solve recurrent, adaptive challenges concerning survival and reproduction (Buss, 2015; Tooby & Cosmides, 1992). These psychological mechanisms are perfectly calibrated to Evolutionary Mismatch 4 the environment in which humans evolved, which is often referred to as the EEA, the environment of evolutionary adaptedness. The EEA for many of the more specialized human traits and their underlying mechanisms such as culture, cooperation, language, leadership, mate and food preferences lies on the African savannah where the human lineage evolved over a period of several million years. Around 6 million years ago the human lineage and that of our closest cousins – what are now bonobos and chimpanzees -- split into separate branches. Over the course of hominid evolution many key features of Homo evolved such as bipedal walking, retractable thumbs, and increased skull size. These physical changes can be easily determined from the fossil record (Kaplan, Hill, Lancaster & Hurtado, 2000). The genus Homo that marks the beginning of our modern Homo Sapiens lineage evolved around 2 million years ago with the withdrawal of forests in East-Africa and the emergence of the savannahs, vast open grasslands. Paleo-archaeological and anthropological evidence supports the idea that our human ancestors lived in relatively small groups, essentially extended families, that were nomadic and migrated with the available food resources (Foley, 1997). Increased reliance on large game animals for nutrition increased cooperation and enhanced division of labor between hunters (usually men) and gatherers (usually women) (Tooby & DeVore, 1987). Food sharing emerged as a collective insurance system against hunger and language emerged as a way to maintain social networks over increasingly large distances (Dunbar, 2003). Culture formed a buffer against perturbations in the environment which required novel social learning strategies based on imitation and teaching (Henrich, 2015). Prestigious individuals emerged as leaders to orchestrate coordinated group activities for group defense and collective movement (Van Vugt & Ahuja, 2010). The psychological mechanisms underlying these aspects of human behavior evolved over this long period, and were conserved when humans migrated out of Africa some 60,000 years ago. As a result of this transition, humans have begun living in ecologies and climatic conditions that are vastly Evolutionary Mismatch 5 different from the EEA. The reliance on intense agriculture that begun after the agricultural revolution, which happened some 10,000 years ago in several places of the world (less than 1% of human evolutionary time), caused further dramatic shifts in the way we live and connect to each other, resulting in a dietary and lifestyle changes to which our bodies and brains are not perfectly adapted (Lieberman, 2013). Similar seismic shifts in our physical and social organization have happened after the Industrial Revolution (some 250 years ago) when machines started to take over human physical work. Currently, more than half of the world’s population lives in large, densely populated cities surrounded by millions of (genetic) strangers in tall buildings with little or no access to nature. We are now in the middle of a digital revolution, blurring the distinction between the real world and the virtual world, with the potential to create increasingly more, novel mismatches (Giphart & van Vugt, 2018; Harari, 2014). The evolutionary mismatch hypothesis asserts that in evolutionarily novel environments the psychological mechanisms that evolved in a long and relatively stable period of human genetic evolution may not be appropriately functioning any more, producing behaviors that are suboptimal for the individual and perhaps for society (Li et al., 2018). If this is true this has far-reaching consequences for human psychology and public policy. Mismatch ideas have been applied before in biology, economics, health, and medicine (Gluckman & Hanson, 2004; Lieberman, 2013; Lloyd, Wilson & Sober, 2011; Nesse & Berridge, 1997). So far, however, there has been little interest in the implications for psychology and public policy, although some scholars in social psychology (Maner & Kenrick, 2010), organizational psychology (Van Vugt & Ronay, 2014), and health psychology (Curtis & Aunger, 2011) have recently started taking an interest in this concept. Mismatch is a by-product -- and inevitable consequence -- of the way evolution via natural selection works. Evolution produces psychological and behavioral mechanisms that Evolutionary Mismatch 6 have been retained via natural selection – and thus are species-typical, heritable, developmentally stable and efficient – because they solved a particular adaptive challenge better than alternative solutions. These mechanisms operate as heuristics, or decision rules that are (a) activated by specific environmental cues acting as inputs and (b) produce adaptive

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    28 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us