Predicting Progress in Beginning Reading: Dynamic Assessment of Phonemic Awareness Janet E

Predicting Progress in Beginning Reading: Dynamic Assessment of Phonemic Awareness Janet E

Copyright 1992 by the American I Journal of Educational Psychology 1992, Vol. 84, No. 3, 353-363 Predicting Progress in Beginning Reading: Dynamic Assessment of Phonemic Awareness Janet E. Spector College of Education, University of Maine This study investigates the ability of a dynamic measure of phonemic awareness to predict progress in beginning reading. Thirty-eight kindergarteners who were nonreaders were assessed in the fall on receptive vocabulary, letter and word recognition, invented spelling, phoneme segmentation, phoneme deletion, and dynamic phoneme segmentation. They were retested near the end of the school year on reading, spelling, and phonemic awareness. The results of the multiple-regression analyses supported the hypothesis that dynamic assessment enhances the predictive utility of a phonemic awareness measure. Performance on dynamic phoneme segmen- tation was the best predictor of end-of-year reading scores and of growth in phonemic awareness. The study demonstrates the applicability of principles of dynamic assessment to the measurement of phonemic awareness and provides further evidence regarding the relationship between pho- nemic awareness and reading acquisition. This study investigates the ability of a dynamic measure of (e.g., Bruce, 1964; Rosner & Simon, 1971); and producing phonemic awareness to predict progress in beginning reading. invented spellings (e.g., Mann, Tobin, & Wilson, 1987; Morris The dynamic approach was compared with a more conven- & Perney, 1984; Read, 1971). tional static approach to assessing phonemic awareness. I The results of both correlational and experimental studies hypothesized that the dynamic measure would more accu- generally have indicated that students who enter reading rately predict progress in beginning reading than would a instruction unable to perform phonemic awareness tasks ex- static measure. The study was influenced by theory and perience less success in reading than students who score high research on two questions: (a) the relationship between pho- in phonemic awareness when instruction commences (e.g., nemic awareness and reading acquisition and (b) the effec- Bradley & Bryant, 1983; Calfee, Lindamood, & Lindamood, tiveness of dynamic versus static assessment. 1973; Juel, 1988; Liberman, Shankweiler, Fischer, & Carter, 1974; Perfetti, Beck, Bell, & Hughes, 1987; Share, Jorm, Maclean, & Matthews, 1984; Stanovich, Cunningham, & Phonemic Awareness and Reading Acquisition Cramer, 1984; Tunmer & Nesdale, 1985; Vellutino & Scan- One of the most consistent relationships to emerge from Ion, 1987). From a theoretical perspective, this finding is the past decade of research on reading is the relationship consistent with models of reading acquisition that emphasize between phonemic awareness and reading acquisition (see the critical role of insight into the alphabetic principle during reviews by Adams, 1990; Ehri, 1979; Golinkoff, 1978; Jorm the initial stages of learning how to read (e.g., Elkonin, 1973; & Share, 1983; Liberman & Shankweiler, 1985; Wagner & Gough & Hillinger, 1980; Liberman, 1973; Perfetti, 1985; Torgesen, 1987; Williams, 1984). Although there is some Rozin & Gleitman, 1977). That is, children must realize that variation across studies in the definition of phonemic aware- letters stand for sounds and when combined, yield words. ness, the term generally is used to denote the ability to perceive Previous research on phonemic awareness has identified spoken words as a sequence of sounds (Lewkowicz, 1980). tasks that appear to be reliable and valid predictors of reading Phonemic awareness has been measured by performance on progress. Yopp (1988), for example, identified two tasks from a wide range of tasks, including rhyming (e.g., Calfee, Chap- a battery of 10 phonemic awareness tests that together ac- man, & Venezky, 1972); isolating beginning, medial, and counted for 58% of the variance in scores on a learning test ending sounds (e.g., Williams, 1980); breaking words into designed to simulate the learning-to-read process. The capac- their component sounds (e.g., Fox & Routh, 1975; Goldstein, ity of phonemic awareness tests to predict aspects of reading 1976; Helfgott, 1976); saying words with target sounds deleted progress suggests that such tests could be used to identify children who would benefit from instructional intervention (Share etal., 1984). This article is based in part on a paper presented in April 1990 at A possible obstacle to practical application, however, is the the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Associa- unfamiliarity and complexity of many phonemic awareness tion in Boston. tasks. Whereas poor performance on a phoneme segmentation I am indebted to Pamela Schutz for enabling data collection; the task might indicate low phonemic awareness, it might also principal, teachers, and students of Pendleton Street School for their reflect the child's lack of understanding of task requirements participation in the study; and Ted Coladarci and the anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments and suggestions. or difficulty in meeting ancillary task demands. For example, Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to some researchers have asked children to count the number of Janet E. Spector, College of Education, Shibles Hall, University of sounds in a spoken word and then to use a pencil or dowel to Maine, Orono, Maine 04469-0155. make a corresponding number of taps (e.g., Liberman et al., 353 354 JANET E. SPECTOR 1974). Poor performance might indicate difficulty in seg- Traditional tests assess only two states: unaided success and menting the spoken word, but it might also reflect difficulty failure. That is, the child either answers a question correctly, in counting the sounds, attending to task instructions, or without prompts or cues from the examiner, or the child is maintaining the one-to-one correspondence between sounds considered to fail the item. From a Vygotskian perspective, and taps. Thus, a limitation of conventional tests of phonemic however, the child may be somewhere in between these two awareness is that they yield too many false negatives, that is, states: unable to perform the task independently but able to students who are unable to perform the experimental task but achieve success with minimal assistance. Vygotsky (1935/ who actually possess (or could easily acquire) the ability that 1978) considered this in-between state to be the zone of the task is designed to measure. Dynamic assessment is an proximal development: "the distance between the actual de- approach that might be useful in eliminating these false velopmental level as determined by independent problem- negatives. solving and the level of potential development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collabo- Dynamic Assessment ration with more capable peers" (p. 85). Consider, for example, two children who earn the same low Dynamic assessment is a general term used to describe a score on a traditional phoneme segmentation test that requires variety of evaluation approaches that emphasize the processes, students to articulate each of the sounds that they hear in a in addition to the products, of assessment (see reviews by word. With minimal instruction, the first child experiences Campione, 1989; Lidz, 1981, 1987). These approaches in- significant growth in performance, whereas the second child clude learning potential assessment (e.g., Budoff, 1987a, shows little improvement. Although the two children received 1987b; Feuerstein, 1979); testing-the-limits procedures (Carl- the same score initially, different degrees of future success son & Wiedl, 1978, 1979); mediated assessment (e.g., Brans- might be predicted for the two children in tasks that presume ford, Delclos, Vye, Burns, & Hasselbring, 1987; Burns, 1985); phonemic awareness. and assisted learning and transfer (e.g., Campione, Brown, In the present study, I developed a dynamic measure that Ferrara, Jones, & Steinberg, 1985). assesses the ability of kindergarten children to perform a To obtain information about responsiveness to instruction, phonemic awareness task when given supportive prompts and dynamic approaches require the interaction between tester cues. On the basis of previous research on dynamic assessment and student. When a student has difficulty solving a problem (e.g., Budoff, 1987a, 1987b; Campione et al., 1985; Carlson or answering a question, the tester attempts to move the & Wiedl, 1979; Embretson, 1987; Ferrara, Brown, & Cam- student from failure to success by modifying the format, pione, 1986), I hypothesized that the dynamic measure would providing additional examples or trials, modeling an appro- more accurately predict future progress in reading than would priate strategy for success, or offering increasingly more direct comparable static measures. Also considered was the ability cues or prompts. The intensity of the dynamic intervention of the dynamic measure to forecast growth in phonemic varies across approaches, ranging from brief, standardized awareness. I expected the dynamic measure to be a better prompts (e.g., testing the limits) to complex, individualized predictor of phonemic awareness at the end of the kindergar- probes (e.g., learning potential assessment). ten year than the traditional static measures of phonemic Advocates of dynamic assessment characterize traditional awareness. tests as static: A student's failure to solve a problem or answer a question is viewed merely as an indicator of where

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    11 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us