An Adaptation for Altruism? the Social Causes, Social Effects, and Social Evolution of Gratitude Michael E

An Adaptation for Altruism? the Social Causes, Social Effects, and Social Evolution of Gratitude Michael E

CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE An Adaptation for Altruism? The Social Causes, Social Effects, and Social Evolution of Gratitude Michael E. McCullough, Marcia B. Kimeldorf, and Adam D. Cohen University of Miami ABSTRACT—People feel grateful when they have benefited Robert Trivers, have apprehended the importance of gratitude from someone’s costly, intentional, voluntary effort on for creating and sustaining positive social relations (Bartlett & their behalf. Experiencing gratitude motivates beneficia- DeSteno, 2006; Harpham, 2004; McCullough, Kilpatrick, ries to repay their benefactors and to extend generosity Emmons, & Larson, 2001). Oddly, though, psychological science to third parties. Expressions of gratitude also reinforce largely neglected gratitude until the 21st century. Fortunately, benefactors for their generosity. These social features recent research has explored gratitude’s distinct social causes distinguish gratitude from related emotions such as hap- and effects. These studies may help to shed light on gratitude’s piness and feelings of indebtedness. Evolutionary theories evolutionary history. propose that gratitude is an adaptation for reciprocal al- truism (the sequential exchange of costly benefits between nonrelatives) and, perhaps, upstream reciprocity (a pay- GRATITUDE AS A PROSOCIAL EMOTION it-forward style distribution of an unearned benefit to a third party after one has received a benefit from another Gratitude is a pleasant emotion, but it is different from simple benefactor). Gratitude therefore may have played a happiness because gratitude is typically preceded by the per- unique role in human social evolution. ception that one has benefited from another person’s generosity. Because gratitude is predicated upon receiving a benefit from KEYWORDS—gratitude; emotion; evolution; morality; pro- another social agent, McCullough et al. (2001) proposed that social behavior; reciprocity; altruism gratitude possesses three psychological features that are rele- vant to processing and responding to prosocial behavior: It is a Emotions are discrete, time-limited, affective responses to sig- (a) benefit detector and both a (b) reinforcer and (c) motivator of nificant environmental changes. Once activated, emotions are prosocial behavior. thought to coordinate thought, physiology, and behavior so that people can respond to reality in self-protective and self-en- hancing ways (Ekman, 1992). Discovering what a particular Gratitude as a Benefit Detector emotion does—the environmental inputs that activate it and its First, McCullough and colleagues proposed that gratitude is an effects on behavior, for instance—is therefore regarded by many affective readout that alerts people that they have benefited from emotion researchers as a royal road to discovering what it is another person’s prosocial behavior. Gratitude is responsive for—that is, why humans evolved to experience that emotion. to four types of information about the benefit-giving situation: (a) Gratitude—a positive emotion that typically flows from the the benefit’s costliness to the benefactor, (b) its value to the perception that one has benefited from the costly, intentional, beneficiary, (c) the intentionality with which it was rendered, and voluntary action of another person—is an interesting test bed for (d) the extent to which it was given even without relational ob- considering this functional approach to emotion. Western social ligations to help (for example, parents’ obligations to help their theorists from Seneca and Cicero to Adam Smith and David children). Recent experiments support this characterization of Hume, to modern social scientists such as Robert Frank and gratitude as a benefit detector. For example, Tsang (2006a) found that, consistent with the proposition that gratitude increases proportionally with the benefactor’s intentionality, participants Address correspondence to Michael E. McCullough, Department of Psychology, University of Miami, P.O. Box 248285, Coral Gables, FL experienced more gratitude toward benefactors who helped them 33124-0751; e-mail: [email protected]. out of benevolent rather than self-serving motives. Volume 17—Number 4 Copyright r 2008 Association for Psychological Science 281 Gratitude Gratitude as a Reinforcer of Prosocial Behavior Gratitude may motivate prosocial behavior by influencing McCullough et al. (2001) also proposed that gratitude reinforces psychological states that support generosity and cooperation. prosocial behavior because expressions of gratitude (for exam- For example, Dunn and Schweitzer (2005, Study 3) found that ple, saying ‘‘thanks’’) increase the likelihood that benefactors participants who described a time in the past when they felt will behave prosocially again in the future. Reviewing previous grateful toward someone (thereby creating grateful emotion in studies, McCullough and colleagues concluded that benefactors the present) subsequently reported higher levels of trust toward a who are thanked for their efforts are willing to give more and third party than did participants who were asked to describe work harder on behalf of others when future opportunities arise a time they felt angry, guilty, or proud. Similarly, Jackson, than are benefactors who have not been thanked. Writing ‘‘thank Lewandowski, Fleury, and Chin (2001) found that having par- you’’ on a restaurant bill even raises servers’ tips. ticipants recall a real-life gratitude-inducing experience caused Why would saying ‘‘thanks’’ reinforce prosocial behavior— them to attribute another person’s good fortune to stable causes and even tipping? Perhaps because the beneficiary’s expression that were under the fortunate person’s control. In other words, of thanks acknowledges to the benefactor that he or she has gratitude causes one to give people credit for their accom- noticed a kindness and, thus, might be prone to reciprocate when plishments. Trust and a readiness to give people credit for their a future opportunity to do so arises. In other words, expressing accomplishments are important lubricants for positive social gratitude may make beneficiaries seem like safe targets for fu- interaction, so psychological effects such as these may explain ture investments. how gratitude promotes prosocial behavior. DISTINGUISHING GRATITUDE FROM OTHER EMOTIONS Gratitude as a Motivator of Prosocial Behavior This leads to gratitude’s third prosocial characteristic: It moti- In light of its prosocial characteristics, functional emotion vates people to behave prosocially after receiving benefits. In theorists would likely speculate that gratitude was shaped by 2001, when McCullough and colleagues reviewed the literature, evolutionary pressures for generosity and helping. However, if gratitude’s efficacy as a motivator of future prosocial behavior gratitude’s prosocial characteristics were not unique to gratitude was the most speculative of their proposals about gratitude’s (for example, if gratitude were indistinguishable in these re- effects. More recent research has confirmed their speculation. spects from emotions such as happiness and indebtedness), then McCullough, Emmons, and Tsang (2002) found that people who the proposition that gratitude was designed specifically to fa- scored higher on self- and peer-report measures of the ‘‘grateful cilitate social exchange would be weakened. Fortunately for the disposition’’ also scored higher on measures of prosocial be- functionalist approach, research shows that gratitude is quite haviors during the previous month (and overall) than did people distinct from several kindred emotions in its phenomenology, who scored lower on the gratitude measures. In addition, Bartlett causes, and effects. and DeSteno (2006) discovered that participants made to feel grateful toward a benefactor exerted more effort to help the Gratitude Versus Other Positive Emotions benefactor on an unrelated task (i.e., completing a boring and Psychologists have long held that positive emotions such as cognitively taxing survey) than did nongrateful participants. happiness and amusement can promote prosocial behavior. They were also more likely to help a stranger (that is, someone What may distinguish gratitude from other positive emotions in who had not helped them) than were nongrateful participants. this respect is that gratitude stimulates helping even when it This latter finding shows that gratitude’s effects on prosocial is costly to the helper. Bartlett and DeSteno (2006) found that behavior were not simply caused by reminding people of the participants in an experimentally induced state of gratitude norm of reciprocity (the norm that dictates that one should return voluntarily spent more time completing a boring survey as a help for help received). favor to their benefactor than did participants in an amused Likewise, Tsang (2006b) found that people who received a emotional state (see also Tsang, 2006b). Additionally, as noted benefit due to the intentional effort of a partner not only were above, Jackson et al. (2001) showed that gratitude causes people more grateful than were people who received the benefit by to give other people who encounter good fortune more credit for chance but also were more likely to behave generously toward their good fortune than does simple happiness. their partner in response. Finally, Emmons and McCullough (2003) found that participants who wrote daily for 2 weeks about things for which they were grateful reported offering more Gratitude Versus Obligation and Indebtedness emotional support and

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    5 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us