Using Network Models of Absolute Pitch to Compare Frequency-Range Discriminations Across Avian Species

Using Network Models of Absolute Pitch to Compare Frequency-Range Discriminations Across Avian Species

Behavioural Processes 84 (2010) 421–427 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Using network models of absolute pitch to compare frequency-range discriminations across avian species Ronald G. Weisman a,∗, Marisa Hoeschele b, Laurie L. Bloomfield c, Douglas Mewhort a, Christopher B. Sturdy b a Queen’s University, Canada b University of Alberta, Canada c Algoma University, Canada article info abstract Article history: The spectral frequency ranges of song notes are important for recognition in avian species tested in the Received 9 September 2009 field. Frequency-range discriminations in both the field and laboratory require absolute pitch (AP). AP Received in revised form 15 January 2010 is the ability to perceive pitches without an external referent. The authors provided a network model Accepted 18 January 2010 designed to account for differences in AP among avian species and evaluated it against discriminative performance in eight-frequency-range laboratory tests of AP for five species of songbirds and two species Keywords: of nonsongbirds. The model’s sensory component describes the neural substrate of avian auditory percep- Absolute pitch tion, and its associative component handles learning of the discrimination. Using only two free parameters Animals Birds to describe the selectivity and the sensitivity of each species’ auditory sensory filters, the model provided Quantitative network model highly accurate predictions of frequency-range discrimination in songbirds and in a parrot species, but Frequency-range discrimination performance and its prediction were less accurate in pigeons: the only species tested that does not learn Song and call recognition its vocalizations. Here for the first time, the authors present a model that predicted individual species’ per- formance in frequency-range discriminations and predicted differences in discrimination among avian species with high accuracy. © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction oscines. The flexibility and subtlety of oscines’ learned songs and calls are important determinants of oscines’ success as a suborder. Birds and mammals are rarely without a voice, and their In fact, oscines are highly successful; they constitute roughly half vocalizations are of demonstrable importance in communication of the approximately 9000 living species of birds. among members of the same species (termed conspecifics). Natu- ral selection has experimented with vocal communication learned by imitation in only a tiny number of distantly related orders 1.1. Auditory perception in song playback experiments and suborders of birds and mammals. Among birds, only true songbirds (oscines), humming birds (Apodiformes), and parrots Bird songs are most commonly studied in playback experi- (Psittaciformes) learn their vocalizations. Among mammals, only ments conducted in the field. In a playback study, territorial males whales and dolphins (Cetaceans), bats (Chiroptera), elephants (Ele- hear songs either recorded from conspecifics or synthesized to phantidae loxdonta), and of course humans (Homo sapiens) learn resemble conspecifics’ songs. The quantification of vigorous male their vocalizations. Comparisons in communication between vocal territorial responses (e.g., approaches to the loud speaker) mea- learning and nonlearning species, and comparisons among vocal sures the potency of the perceptual features or characteristics of learning species are the most powerful tools comparative scien- songs manipulated during playback experiments. Playback studies tists can apply to understand the evolution of communication (see measure the biological importance of a song’s perceptual features Jarvis, 2006, for a review). In this article, we have focused on the directly in nature, without any laboratory artifice. perceptual basis of vocal communication in birds, and especially in There are hundreds of published studies of song playback in dozens of oscine species (see McGregor, 1992; Slater, 2003). From these field experiments, it is now well known that songbirds col- ∗ lect information about several perceptual features of songs (e.g., Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Queen’s University, 99 Uni- number of notes, note duration, and the harmonic structure of versity Avenue, Kingston, Ontario, K7L3N6, Canada. Tel.: +1 613 540 4150; fax: +1 613 533 2499. notes) in order to identify conspecifics (e.g., Nelson, 1988). In study- E-mail address: [email protected] (R.G. Weisman). ing this vast literature, it became apparent to our research group 0376-6357/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2010.01.010 422 R.G. Weisman et al. / Behavioural Processes 84 (2010) 421–427 that one song feature, the spectral frequency ranges of song notes, (2006) have described our laboratory operant equipment and pro- has been cited repeatedly as important to both individual recogni- cedures for songbirds in detail. In brief, a bird hears an acoustic tion and species recognition. That is, in field playback experiments, stimulus (here always a sinewave tone) presented via a speaker frequency range predominates over other acoustic features (e.g., after the bird lands on a monitored perch in a sound isolation cham- the timing of notes) in determining the strength of the territorial ber. If the stimulus is an S+ (a go stimulus), then flying or hopping response (see Nelson, 1989a; Weary et al., 1986). The finding that from the perch to a feeder is reinforced with food; if the stimulus the frequency range of song notes (typically from the lowest to the is an S− (a no-go stimulus), then hopping to the feeder is not rein- highest peak frequency) is important to conspecific recognition has forced with food and is punished by a delay before the next trial. been replicated in at least 12 oscine species (e.g., Dabelsteen and Pigeons were tested in standard operant chambers fitted with peck- Pedersen, 1985; Emlen, 1972; Falls, 1963; Nelson, 1989b; Lohr et ing keys and grain feeders (see Friedrich et al., 2007). The pigeons al., 1994; Nowicki et al., 1989; Thompson, 1969; Weary et al., 1986; pecked the left key to hear a tone stimulus. If the stimulus was an Weisman and Ratcliffe, 1989; Wunderle, 1979). In a particularly S+, then pecking the right key was reinforced with food, if the stim- cogent example, Nelson (1989a) demonstrated that field spar- ulus was an S−, then pecking was punished with a delay. Notice that rows’ (Spizella pusilla) songs—pitch-shifted upward in frequency only pigeons (Columba livia) were trained to key peck. by two standard deviations—elicit no more territory defense than Frequency-range discriminations are laboratory analogues to songs recorded from birds of other species, whereas songs played identify the frequency ranges of successive notes in conspecific back within the normal frequency range elicited vigorous territory songs in playback experiments. We trained songbirds in the most defense. difficult of these discriminations, the eight-frequency-range dis- In describing the results of playback studies, field biologists crimination, using 40 tones, spaced 120-Hz apart, in the spectral like to talk about the acoustic properties of song, one of which region between 980 and 5660 Hz, and parsed into eight ranges is the ranges of spectral frequencies found in a species’ songs of five tones each (see Table 1). In the S+ (odd) version of the in nature. Be aware, however, that it is not what songbirds sing task, responses (hopping or flying to the feeder) to tones in the but what they hear that determines responsiveness to song. What odd ranges (1st, 3rd, 5th, and 7th) were reinforced (with food) songbirds hear are pitches in response to the frequencies pro- and responses to tones in the even ranges were not reinforced. duced in song. This is not a trivial point; the relationship between In the counterbalanced, S+ (even), version of the task, responses frequencies and pitches varies between avian and mammalian to tones in the even ranges (2nd, 4th, 6th, and 8th) were rein- species, and among avian species, as a function of differences in forced. Here we report on performance only in the S+ (odd) version auditory neurobiology. The critical perceptual ability that enables of the discrimination, because performance in the S+ (even) ver- songbirds to sort song notes into frequency ranges is called abso- sion is virtually the mirror image of performance in the S+ (odd) lute pitch (AP). AP is defined as the ability to reproduce or version. classify pitches with high accuracy without an external referent Over frequency-range experiments, testing a wide range of (Ward, 1998). Songbirds need no pitch pipe or other external species, important facts have emerged. The first finding is that standard to identify frequency ranges of the notes in conspecific birds (for example, zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata) succeed in songs. accurately tracking every shift between reinforcement and nonre- inforcement across eight frequency ranges, whereas humans and rats (and perhaps other mammals) seem only to track the shift 1.2. AP and frequency-range discriminations from the 1st to the 2nd range and from the 7th to the 8th range but otherwise fail in the eight-range discrimination. The second Although song playback research remains the best method for finding is that, although avian species always track reinforcement establishing the importance of a perceptual feature to song recog- across frequency ranges, the accuracy of the discrimination varies nition, the methodology has several limitations when the goal is to considerably across species (Weisman et al., 2006; Friedrich et al., study the perceptual abilities (here AP) that support use of pitch in 2007). songs and calls. For example, in playback experiments, birds habit- uate to extensive and repeated exposure to the same vocalizations and fail to respond at all to simplified acoustic stimuli that do not resemble conspecifics’ vocalizations. In studies of auditory percep- 2. A network model for classification by AP tion, one often wishes to study the responses of individuals across repeated presentations of an array of stimuli.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    7 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us