Individual Vigilance Profiles in Flocks of House Sparrows (Passer Domesticus)

Individual Vigilance Profiles in Flocks of House Sparrows (Passer Domesticus)

Canadian Journal of Zoology Individual vigilance profiles in flocks of house sparrows (Passer domesticus) Journal: Canadian Journal of Zoology Manuscript ID cjz-2017-0301.R2 Manuscript Type: Article Date Submitted by the Author: 13-Feb-2018 Complete List of Authors: Boujja-Miljour, Hakima; University of Montreal, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine Leighton, Patrick A.; University of Montréal, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine Beauchamp,Draft Guy; University of Montreal, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine flock size, house sparrow, passer domesticus, vigilance, individual Keyword: variation, plasticity https://mc06.manuscriptcentral.com/cjz-pubs Page 1 of 25 Canadian Journal of Zoology 1 Individual vigilance profiles in flocks of house sparrows (Passer domesticus) Hakima Boujja-Miljour, Patrick A. Leighton and Guy Beauchamp Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, University of Montréal, Canada E-mail addresses: H. Boujja-Miljour ([email protected]) P. A. Leighton ([email protected]) G. Beauchamp ([email protected]) Correspondence: Guy Beauchamp. Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, University of Montréal, 3200 rue Sicotte, St-Hyacinthe,Draft Québec, Canada J2S 2M2. Phone: 450-773- 8521 (#8478); Fax: 450-778-8129. https://mc06.manuscriptcentral.com/cjz-pubs Canadian Journal of Zoology Page 2 of 25 2 Boujja-Miljour, H., Leighton, P. A. & Beauchamp, G. Individual vigilance profiles in flocks of house sparrows (Passer domesticus). Abstract: Individual vigilance against threats typically decreases with group size. However, group size often explains a small amount of variation in vigilance suggesting that other factors such as individual differences might contribute. For instance, individuals could maintain different vigilance levels overall and also respond differently to variation in group size. We investigated individual variation in vigilance and its patterns of plasticity in flocks of house sparrows (Passer domesticus (L. 1758)). We carried out observations at one provisioned site, and used multiple observations of the same individuals (n = 14) in flocks of different sizes over two consecutive months. The typical decline in vigilance with flock size occurred at the population level. Controlling for food density, flock size, time of year, and sex, we documented consistent individual differences in various measurements of vigilance. Plasticity of vigilance adjustments to variation in flock size occurred for theDraft frequency of high vigilance postures. Male sparrows with larger bibs, which signal higher dominance status, tended to spend less time vigilant and obtained food at a higher rate supporting a state-dependent explanation for the origin of individual vigilance profiles. Individual differences can contribute to explaining the large scatter in the relationship between vigilance and group size in many species. Key words: bib size, flock size, house sparrow, individual variation, Passer domesticus plasticity, vigilance. https://mc06.manuscriptcentral.com/cjz-pubs Page 3 of 25 Canadian Journal of Zoology 3 Introduction Group living is widespread in animals and carries both costs and benefits. In terms of resource acquisition, for instance, individuals that forage in groups can find food faster, but can also experience higher competition (Ward and Webster 2016). Group living can also reduce predation risk. For instance, group foragers can maintain a lower vigilance against potential threats without incurring a higher risk of predation (Pulliam 1973). As group size increases, more eyes and ears are available to detect predators (Caraco et al. 1980), and information about detection can rapidly spread in the group to alert neighbours about the incoming threat (Lima 1995). Furthermore, the odds that a predator targets a specific individual are lower in a larger group (Bertram 1978). This group-size effect on vigilance prevails in many species of animals (Caro 2005). Models of antipredator vigilance predict the decline in vigilance as a function of group size for an average individual (BeauchampDraft 2015). Group size, however, explains a relatively small amount of the variation in vigilance in many species (Blumstein 1996; Beauchamp 2008). Plots of the relationship between vigilance and group size also reveal important heterogeneity (Beauchamp 2013). These findings suggest that the group-size effect on vigilance might vary for different subsets of individuals. For instance, males might maintain more vigilance against neighbours than females during the reproductive season and show a shallower decline of vigilance with group size (Childress and Lung 2003; Li et al. 2012). A shallower group-size effect on vigilance is also expected for individuals with more energy reserves (McNamara and Houston 1992). Heterogeneity in the group-size effect on vigilance might also reflect individual differences. Indeed, regardless of group size, some individuals tend to be consistently more vigilant than others. Recent studies have documented stable individual vigilance profiles in mammals (Carter et al. 2009; Pangle and Holekamp 2010; Dannock et al. 2013; Edwards et al. 2013; Hoogland et al. 2013; Favreau et al. 2014) and in birds (Rieucau et al. 2010; Couchoux and Cresswell 2012; Roche and Brown 2013). In addition to consistent differences in overall vigilance, individuals could also show differences in https://mc06.manuscriptcentral.com/cjz-pubs Canadian Journal of Zoology Page 4 of 25 4 the level of adjustment of vigilance to group size. Various terms describe this component of individual responses including plasticity but also flexibility, responsiveness, coping style or reactivity (Wolf et al. 2008). Two studies revealed consistent individual differences in patterns of plasticity with respect to a gradient in predation risk (Mathot et al. 2011; Couchoux and Cresswell 2012). Nevertheless, the results are mixed for vigilance studies with evidence for individual patterns of plasticity in one case (Carter et al. 2009) but not in the others (Rieucau et al. 2010; Dannock et al. 2013; Favreau et al. 2014). What might explain consistent individual differences in vigilance and plasticity? One explanation relies on state-dependent behaviour. Individuals adjust their behaviour according to their state (e.g. energy reserves), which varies consistently among individuals, and differences between individuals tend to increase or to decrease when predation risk changes thus inducing different levels of plasticity amongst individuals along the predation risk gradient (SihDraft et al. 2015). Another explanation emphasizes negative frequency-dependent payoffs associated with different behavioural options. Flexible individuals that adjust their vigilance to predation risk can benefit the whole group because vigilance payoffs are shared through collective detection. This mechanism allows the coexistence of flexible and less flexible individuals with respect to how vigilance is adjusted to predation risk (Mathot et al. 2011). Overall, few studies have investigated individual vigilance profiles as a function of group size. Thus far, the evidence for consistent individual differences in vigilance and for plasticity in the group-size effect on vigilance is mixed. In addition, it is not clear in most cases which mechanism underlies individual variation. To evaluate individual consistency and plasticity, we used multiple observations of the same individuals in groups of different sizes (Dingemanse et al. 2010). We sought evidence for statistical heterogeneity in intercept values, which evaluates repeatability of vigilance at the time scale of the study, and heterogeneity in slope values, which indicates individual variation in plasticity. https://mc06.manuscriptcentral.com/cjz-pubs Page 5 of 25 Canadian Journal of Zoology 5 We investigated individual vigilance profiles in flocks of the house sparrow (Passer domesticus (L. 1758)), a model species for vigilance (Anderson 2006). Sparrows typically feed on seeds in small flocks and often interrupt their feeding to scan the surroundings for signs of danger. Their lateral eyes allow detection of threats when foraging head down (Fernández-Juricic et al. 2008), but postures with the head up are commonly used when handling resources and scanning for threats (Liker and Barta 2002; Tisdale and Fernández-Juricic 2009). Vigilance in house sparrows decreases with flock size (Barnard 1980; Studd et al. 1983; Elgar et al. 1984; Lima 1987), and also varies as a function of sex and temperature (Beveridge and Deag 1987) and food density (Johnson et al. 2001). Males possess a black bib whose size correlates with dominance and age (Møller 1988; Veiga 1993; Liker and Barta 2001; Laucht et al. 2011), two state- dependent variables that might explain how stable individual vigilance profiles arise in males of this species. Materials and Methods Draft Study area We conducted the study between May 27th and August 5th in 2016 on the balcony of a third-floor apartment in the city of Montréal (Québec, Canada: 45.27° N, 73.33° W). The apartment building was located on a small street with few disturbances. Trees across the street provided a refuge for the feeding sparrows. We placed a 1m X 1m feeder on the balcony flush with the railings so that the sparrows could land directly on the feeder to access resources. We supplied the feeder with a large amount of white millet seeds on a daily basis to attract sparrows. A large number (>50) of sparrows regularly visited the feeder. Prior to the start of the study, we captured sparrows directly

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    26 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us