A Behavioural Syndrome in the Field Cricket Gryllus Integer: Intrasexual Aggression Is Correlated with Activity in a Novel Envir

A Behavioural Syndrome in the Field Cricket Gryllus Integer: Intrasexual Aggression Is Correlated with Activity in a Novel Envir

Blackwell Publishing LtdOxford, UKBIJBiological Journal of the Linnean Society0024-4066© 2007 The Linnean Society of London? 2007 91? 475482 Original Article BEHAVIOURAL SYNDROMES IN FIELD CRICKETS R. KORTET and A. HEDRICK Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2007, 91, 475–482. With 5 figures A behavioural syndrome in the field cricket Gryllus integer: intrasexual aggression is correlated with activity in a novel environment RAINE KORTET1,2* and ANN HEDRICK3 1Behaviour, Ecology and Evolution Team (Integrative Ecology Unit), Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Helsinki, PO Box 65(Viikinkaari 1), FI-00014 University of Helsinki, Finland 2Section of Ecology, Department of Biology, University of Oulu, PO Box 3000, 90014 University of Oulu, Finland 3Neurobiology, Physiology and Behaviour, University of California, Davis, One Shields Avenue, Davis, CA 95616, USA Received 27 March 2006; accepted for publication 1 September 2006 Behavioural syndromes, or suites of correlated behaviours across different contexts and situations, have recently drawn attention from evolutionary biologists. In the field cricket Gryllus integer, males are aggressive with one another and fight vigorously over females and territories. We examined whether aggressiveness with other males was correlated with activity in a potentially dangerous context (a novel environment) in laboratory-raised virgin males. Aggressiveness was measured as fighting ability against a weight-matched opponent. First, we measured each cricket’s latency to become active in a novel environment and latency to emerge from a refuge within a novel environment. Next, we determined which of two weight-matched males was more aggressive, by pitting the males together in an agonistic contest and counting the number of fights won by each male. More aggressive males, who won more fights, had shorter latencies to become active when placed in a novel environment and shorter latencies to emerge from a safe refuge. These results suggest that a behavioural syndrome exists in G. integer, in which more aggressive males are also more active in general, and possibly less cautious towards predation risk. © 2007 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2007, 91, 475–482. ADDITIONAL KEYWORDS: antipredator behaviour – boldness – personality – intrasexual dominance. INTRODUCTION because of their ‘bold’ or incautious behaviour in the presence of predators (Hedrick & Riechert, 1989, Recent studies support the existence of behavioural 1990; Riechert & Hedrick, 1993). Aggressiveness syndromes, comprising suites of correlated behaviours towards conspecifics that is correlated with danger- across different situations or contexts (Hedrick, 2000; ously bold or noncautious behaviour under predation Gosling, 2001; Sih, Kats & Maurer, 2003; Sih et al., risk has been demonstrated in a number of species, 2004b; Sih, Bell & Johnson, 2004a; Carere & Eens, including stickleback fish (Huntingford, 1976, 1982; 2005). One of the most familiar behavioural syn- Bell, 2005) and fishing spiders (Johnson & Sih, 2005). dromes in animals is an aggressiveness/activity syn- The existence of behavioural syndromes may offer an drome, in which individuals behave more or less explanation for apparently ‘non-adaptive’ behaviour, aggressively/are more or less active across many con- such as sexual cannibalism, high levels of activity texts (Sih et al., 2004a,b). For example, in funnel-web under predation risk, and hyper-aggressiveness that spiders, some individuals are more aggressive with decreases mating success by startling or driving away conspecifics than others, attack prey more vigorously, potential mates (Sih et al, 2004a; Bell, 2005; Sih & and expose themselves to higher risk of predation Watters, 2005). Moreover, the persistence of behav- ioural ‘types’ within a species (e.g. ‘bold’ and ‘shy’ indi- *Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected].fi viduals) suggests that there can be more than one © 2007 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2007, 91, 475–482 475 476 R. KORTET and A. HEDRICK general adaptive strategy for individuals within that MATERIAL AND METHODS species (Dall, 2004). For example, aggressive individ- uals that are incautious in terms of predation risk and CRICKETS less aggressive individuals that are cautious in terms The crickets used in this experiment were the first of predation risk may have similar fitnesses (Riechert laboratory generation derived from wild-caught & Hedrick, 1993; Dall, Houston & McNamara, 2004; G. integer females (August 2003) collected from Dingemanse et al., 2004). Davis, California. In this population, males call from In species with social dominance hierarchies, such cracks in the ground to attract sexually receptive as field crickets (Gryllus spp.), more aggressive males females, and females travel above ground to find achieve higher dominance status by winning fights males. Males sing at the entrance of the crack with (Alexander, 1961; Andersson, 1994; Berglund, Bis- their heads outside the crack entrance, but abruptly sazza & Pilastro, 1996; Rantala & Kortet, 2004). stop calling and run further into the crack if they Males fight for and defend territorial shelters and sense a predator approaching. Although they leave females (Alexander, 1961; Simmons, 1986a; Adamo & cracks to forage, they appear reluctant to leave Hoy, 1995; Hofmann & Schildberger, 2001; Kortet & cracks, presumably because this increases predation Hedrick, 2005; Väänänen, Kortet & Rantala, 2006), risk. Similar hiding behaviour occurs in many taxa and success in intrasexual competition can result in and is generally recognized as an antipredator tactic increased fertilization success for dominant males (Dill & Fraser, 1997; Hugie, 2003; Jennions et al., (Simmons, 1986b; Rantala & Kortet, 2004; Kortet & 2003; Hugie, 2004). Hedrick, 2005). Fighting success is a heritable trait in Wild-caught females had been inseminated in the at least one species of field cricket (Gryllus bimacula- field before capture. Laboratory crickets were main- tus De Geer; Wedell & Tregenza, 1999). tained at 25 ± 1 °C with food available (Purina chick Virtually no studies have investigated behavioural starter) and water ad libitum, under a 12 : 12 h light/ syndromes in field crickets (but see Hedrick, 2000) dark photoperiod. Experimental crickets were even though many behavioural traits, such as mating removed from bulk family boxes as nymphs (approxi- preferences, antipredator behaviour, aggression, and mately one quarter adult size) and reared individually social dominance have been studied in these animals (also with food and water available ad libitum) in (Adamo & Hoy, 1995; Hedrick & Weber, 1998; Gray & waxed cardboard cups. Individuals of both sexes were Cade, 1999; Hedrick, 2000; Wagner & Reiser, 2000; physically, but not acoustically, isolated from other Rantala & Kortet, 2003; Kortet & Hedrick, 2004, individuals to ensure virginity and control for experi- 2005). However, in a population of the field cricket ence (with the possible exception of acoustic experi- Gryllus integer Scudder in Davis, California, calling ence). The crickets were approximately 2 weeks behaviour is phenotypically correlated with antipreda- past the final adult moult on the first day of the tor behaviour. In this population, males call to attract experiment. receptive females, males differ in their durations of uninterrupted calling, and females prefer longer calls (Hedrick, 1986). Calling duration is heritable EXPERIMENTAL TRIALS ON ACTIVITY IN A NOVEL (Hedrick, 1994). Hedrick (2000) demonstrated that ENVIRONMENT males with longer calls were more cautious about pre- Experimental trials were conducted in a sound-proof, dation risk than males with shorter calls: they had temperature controlled dark room chamber longer latencies to emerge from a safe refuge in a novel (26 ± 1 °C), consisting of a computer, desk, and red fil- environment, and had longer latencies to resume call- ter light (25 W red incandescent bulb, 60 cm from the ing when interrupted by a predator cue. Aggressive- arena). The red light was used to mimic nocturnal con- ness of males was not measured in this study. ditions. Our methods for trials to assess the ‘cautious- In the present study, we examined whether a ness’ of individual crickets were as described general aggressiveness/activity syndrome exists in previously (Hedrick, 2000), and resembled methods G. integer, by testing whether ‘cautiousness’ about that have been used extensively to measure ‘fearful- becoming active and emerging from a safe refuge in a ness’ in rodents (Boissy, 1995). At the beginning of a novel environment was correlated with the fighting trial, an experimental male was placed in a clean ability of males. If an aggressiveness/activity behav- experimental vial (a clear plastic vial that was covered ioural syndrome exists in this population of G. integer, with a layer of clear tape to make it opaque, 4 cm we predicted that more aggressive males (those who in diameter × 65 mm in length, set upright in an won the majority of fights with a weight-matched experimental arena (Rubbermaid food storage con- opponent) would have shorter latencies to become tainer; length 17 cm × width 17 cm × height 10 cm). active and to emerge from a refuge than less aggres- The cricket inside the vial was given 2 min to accli- sive males. mate to the environment. After acclimation, experi- © 2007 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society,

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    9 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us