MATING BEHAVIOR of the MALE GREATER RHEA Author(S) :THAÏS L

MATING BEHAVIOR of the MALE GREATER RHEA Author(S) :THAÏS L

MATING BEHAVIOR OF THE MALE GREATER RHEA Author(s) :THAÏS L. CODENOTTI and FERNANDO ALVAREZ Source: The Wilson Bulletin, 113(1):85-89. 2001. Published By: The Wilson Ornithological Society DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1676/0043-5643(2001)113[0085:MBOTMG]2.0.CO;2 URL: http://www.bioone.org/doi/full/10.1676/0043-5643%282001%29113%5B0085%3AMBOTMG %5D2.0.CO%3B2 BioOne (www.bioone.org) is a nonprofit, online aggregation of core research in the biological, ecological, and environmental sciences. BioOne provides a sustainable online platform for over 170 journals and books published by nonprofit societies, associations, museums, institutions, and presses. Your use of this PDF, the BioOne Web site, and all posted and associated content indicates your acceptance of BioOne’s Terms of Use, available at www.bioone.org/page/terms_of_use. Usage of BioOne content is strictly limited to personal, educational, and non-commercial use. Commercial inquiries or rights and permissions requests should be directed to the individual publisher as copyright holder. BioOne sees sustainable scholarly publishing as an inherently collaborative enterprise connecting authors, nonprofit publishers, academic institutions, research libraries, and research funders in the common goal of maximizing access to critical research. Wilson Bull., 113(1), 2001, pp. 85±89 MATING BEHAVIOR OF THE MALE GREATER RHEA THAIÈS L. CODENOTTI1 AND FERNANDO ALVAREZ2,3 ABSTRACT.ÐDuring mating, and during the phases known as solitary male, harem formation, and harem, male Greater Rheas (Rhea americana) became vigilant to assure the elimination of competitors. High rates of harem defense and male expulsion were observed at the start, with levels decreasing gradually through the phases of mating, and male-male ®ghting mainly limited to the phase of harem formation. Courtship was nonexistent early in the mating period, reached a high level during the phases of harem formation and harem, then declined markedly just before harem breaking. The rates of all male social activities decreased signi®cantly just before harem breaking. Received 2 June 2000, accepted 17 January 2001. Greater Rheas (Rhea americana) living in Rheas living in a grassy ¯oodplain in the open grasslands of Argentina are typically po- Buenos Aires province of Argentina was re- lygynandric, i.e., the males are simultaneously stricted to the middle of the breeding season polygynous, while the females are sequential- (November and December 1970, and October ly polyandrous (Bruning 1973, 1974; Hand- and November 1972). Consequently he had to ford and Mares 1985). In an agricultural area rely on reports from local residents for infor- of Rio Grande do Sul, in southern Brazil, mation on the early phases of mating. Ac- Greater Rheas occupy grassland and cultivat- cording to those reports and Bruning's obser- ed land, and exhibit their characteristic poly- vations, adult males were the ®rst to leave the gynandric mating system, without ever chang- winter ¯ocks as aggression increased early in ing to monogamy, even when the sex ratio is the breeding season (this corresponds to our near unity. solitary male phase of mating). Contrary to Greater Rheas in the present study area (Rio our observations (Codenotti 1995, 1997), Grande do Sul) pass through three phases dur- Bruning (1974) reported the formation of ing mating: (A) Solitary male. From the mid- small female groups (2±15 members) which dle of winter and through spring, mainly from then became harems by the addition of an August to November, adult males leave the adult male. According to Bruning (1974), the mixed ¯ocks, the latter including 4±35 ani- primarily masculine behaviors of courtship mals of both sexes and all ages. The solitary and aggression occurred only and mostly, re- males follow the mixed ¯ock for approxi- spectively, during the breeding season. At the mately 25 d, at a distance of 30±300 m, usu- onset of this period, and prior to acquiring a ally keeping within sight of the mixed ¯ock. harem, males engaged in aggressive interac- (B) Harem formation. Also mainly from the tions and calling, and from that time the rate middle of winter and through spring, the iso- of courtship increased steadily. lated males return to the mixed ¯ocks where In regard to Bruning's (1974) and Coden- they stay for about one month, competing ag- otti's (1995, 1997) reports, the mating process gressively with other adult males, courting, of Greater Rheas may be best understood by and ®nally associating with a female sub- concentrating on the behavior of reproducing group. (C) Harem. Mainly during spring, the adult males. We approached that aim in the male with a subgroup of 2±12 females leaves present study by analyzing the variation of the the mixed ¯ock to reside in a particular area reproductive males' behaviors in relation to for about 30±45 d, where nesting and joint the phases of mating. laying take place (Codenotti 1995, 1997). Bruning's (1974) ®eld study of Greater STUDY AREA AND METHODS The 1680-ha study site was located at 288 49 S, 528 1 Univ. de Passo Fundo, Caixa postal 611, 99001- 29 W in the northern portion of Rio Grande do Sul, 970 Passo Fundo/RS, Brazil. Brazil, and was characterized by a humid subtropical 2 EstacioÂn BioloÂgica de DonÄana, C.S.I.C., Apt. pos- climate. About 70% of the area was dedicated to farm- tal 1056, 41080 Sevilla, Spain. ing, and most of the remainder was meadow (natural 3 Corresponding author; E-mail: alvarez@ebd. and cultivated). The Greater Rhea population in the csic.es study area varied during the period of study from about 85 86 THE WILSON BULLETIN x Vol. 113, No. 1, March 2001 100±125 individuals, and the number of adult females is a low-pitched, intense guttural sound made up of per male was 2.1 in 1990, 1.4 in 1991, and 1.1 in 1992. two syllables. Audio recording was done with a Sony During a total of 82 weekly sampling journeys from digital recorder and a Sennheiser ME88 directional mi- January 1991 to December 1992 (14 such weekly jour- crophone with frequency range of 20±20,000 Hz, and neys were skipped due to bad weather) a ®xed route the digital sonagraph used was Uniscan II. Sonagram in the study area was followed, and the numbers of of booming call is presented in Codenotti et al. (1995). birds integrated into mixed ¯ocks (made up of animals Only one sound, or a bout of two sounds with an in- of both sexes and all ages) or harems, as well as sol- terval of 2±3 s between emissions, are produced. Then itary, were recorded. In addition, during a total of 127 the male remains silent for about 20 min before calling sampling journeys during the mating season (August again. On several occasions we were able to detect the to February of 1990 to 1992, at 08:00±12:00 and 14: sound up to a distance of 3 km. While facing toward 00±20:00 DST), 363 h of approximately equally dis- each other at close range, two males may call simul- tributed 15-min focal observations were made of the taneously in the context of competition for females, behaviors of the 16 breeding adult males that were sometimes just before ®ghting or before starting a individually recognized by differences in body size chase, but not during the actual ®ght or chase. Often and plumage (Codenotti 1995). Rates of behaviors the harem owner calls towards a male competitor 3± were then analyzed in relation to the three phases of 20 m away from him, the receiver being either visible mating and to the ®rst and second halves of the du- or hidden by obstacles. There is some variation in the ration of the harem phase for each male (Friedman features of the calls of different males, and our obser- two-way analysis of variance by ranks, Wilcoxon vations suggest that stronger males tend to vocalize matched-pairs signed-ranks test, and sequential Bon- more often than others. (8) Harem defenseÐthe harem ferroni test; Siegel and Castellan 1988, Sokal and owner, with wings open and ¯exed forward (thus ex- Rohlf 1995, with the aid of StatView 4.1 for Apple- posing body and penis), facing another male at a dis- Macintosh). The ranges of dates for the different phas- tance of 3±20 m. He then moves to place himself be- es of mating among the 16 subjects were: 20 July to tween the intruder and his females, while trying to 23 December for the solitary male phase, 25 July to drive the harem away from the opponent. (9) Male- 31 December for harem formation, and 6 September male ®ghtingÐoften after calling, two adult males ap- to 15 January for the harem phase. proaching and facing each other with neck and head The patterns of behavior were as follows (for more feathers bristled and both wings (or seldom only one detail see Brito 1949; Raikow 1968, 1969; and Cod- wing) raised. They then run toward each other. During enotti et al. 1995): (1) Comfort behaviorÐgrooming the ®ght they hit each other with their feet and peck and pecking at own feathers, shaking, opening wings, at each other while interlocking their necks. They may stretching, yawning, and also including neutral pos- also chase each other while clattering the upper and tures (standing, sitting, lying down). (2) FeedingÐ lower jaws. (10) Male expulsionÐas the result of a pecking the soil, vegetation, or insects (on ground or ®ght, the winner chasing the loser away from the area. ¯ying), and also including drinking. (3) LocomotionÐ walking and running. (4) Alert postureÐmotionless or RESULTS walking slowly, with neck stretched up and head for- ward.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    6 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us