This Item Is the Archived Peer-Reviewed Author-Version Of

This Item Is the Archived Peer-Reviewed Author-Version Of

This item is the archived peer-reviewed author-version of: Upper secondary and first-year university students explanations of animal behaviour : to what extent are Tinbergens four questions about causation, ontogeny, function and evolution, represented? Reference: Pinxten Rianne, Desclée Mathieu, Eens Marcel.- Upper secondary and first-year university students explanations of animal behaviour : to what extent are Tinbergens four questions about causation, ontogeny, function and evolution, represented? International journal of science education - ISSN 0950-0693 - (2016), p. 1-23 Full text (Publishers DOI): http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1080/09500693.2016.1239139 To cite this reference: http://hdl.handle.net/10067/1361570151162165141 Institutional repository IRUA Cover page Upper Secondary and First-year University Students’ explanations of Animal Behaviour: To what extent are Tinbergen’s Four Questions, about Causation, Ontogeny, Function and Evolution, represented? Rianne Pinxten a,b*, Mathieu Descléea and Marcel Eensb aFaculty of Social Sciences, Didactica research unit, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium. bDepartment of Biology, Behavioural Ecology and Ecophysiology group, University of Antwerp, Wilrijk, Belgium. Word count: 10315 *Corresponding author: Rianne Pinxten, Faculty of Social Sciences, Research group Didactica, University of Antwerp, Venusstraat 35, 2000 Antwerp, Belgium. Email: [email protected], Tel. ++32 (0)3 265 22 92 Mathieu Desclée, Faculty of Social Sciences, Research group Didactica, University of Antwerp, Venusstraat 35, 2000 Antwerp, Belgium. Email: [email protected], Tel. ++32 (0)493 68 13 18 Marcel Eens, Department of Biology, Behavioural Ecology and Ecophysiology group, University of Antwerp, Universiteitsplein 1, B-2610 Wilrijk, Belgium. Email: [email protected], Tel. ++32 (0)3 265 22 84 Biographical information: Rianne Pinxten is professor at the University of Antwerp. She has been working as a teacher trainer for more than 15 years, more specifically in the field of didactics of the natural sciences. Her research interests are in teacher education, STEM education, evolution and animal behaviour education. She is also affiliated to the Behavioural Ecology and Ecophysiology group of the Department of Biology for more than 30 years, where she teaches behavioural biology (including a practical course on observing and measuring animal behaviour), behavioural ecology and behavioural physiology. Here, her core research is in behavioural ecology and behavioural endocrinology, focusing on reproduction and sexual selection in birds. Mathieu Desclée obtained a master in biology and is a student biology teacher. Marcel Eens is head of the Behavioural Ecology and Ecophysiology group at the University of Antwerp. His main research interest is the study of the causes and consequences of variation in 1 behavioural traits among individuals, with most studies being carried out within an ethological framework, focusing on all four major aspects of animal behaviour: causation, development, function and evolution. He uses an integrative and multidisciplinary approach to study reproduction and sexual selection in different animal groups, but primarily in birds. He is also interested in the application of behavioural ecology to environmental and biodiversity problems. He teaches behavioural biology, behavioural ecology, behavioural physiology, behaviour: seminar and skills, applied and conservation behaviour, bioacoustics and ornithology. 2 In 1963, the Nobel Prize-winning ethologist Niko Tinbergen proposed a framework for the scientific study of animal behaviour by outlining four questions that should be answered to have a complete understanding: causation, ontogeny, function and evolution. At present, Tinbergen’s framework is still considered the best way to guide animal behavioural research. Given the importance in science instruction of demonstrating how scientists actually work and ask questions, we investigated to what extent Tinbergen’s questions are addressed in biology textbooks in secondary education in Flanders, Belgium and represented in students’ explanations of behaviour in general and of specific animal behaviours. The latter was investigated using open-response items posed to 70 upper-secondary and 149 first-year university students. Our results revealed that teaching of animal behaviour mainly emphasizes ontogeny and, to a lesser extent, causation, and that Tinbergen’s framework is not explicitly referred to. Students, in both groups, typically addressed only one or two questions, with the majority addressing causation or both causation and ontogeny when explaining behaviour in general, but function or causation and function when explaining specific animal behaviours. The latter high prevalence of function may be largely due to intuitive teleological thinking. Evolution was generally completely neglected, even in university students who had recently completed an evolution course. Our results revealed that transfer of the concepts of ontogeny and evolution was (almost) absent, although the skill of knowledge transfer is an important goal in science education. We argue why Tinbergen’s framework should best be an integral part of any biology curriculum. Keywords: Animal Behaviour Education; Tinbergen’s four questions; Secondary Education; Knowledge Transfer 3 Introduction Science involves not only knowledge but also a process. Learning of science by students is thus better supported if textbooks and instruction address not only scientific knowledge, but also scientific practices in the field (Bednekoff, 2005; Wilke & Straits, 2005; DiGiuseppe, 2014). The purpose of emphasizing scientific practices during instruction is to help students develop deeper understanding of science by demonstrating to them how scientists in the domain work, how they ask questions and examine the hypotheses raised (Bell, Smetana, & Binns, 2005). Hence, an important goal in biology and science education is to demonstrate, and teach, students how to ask the right scientifically oriented questions, depending on the specific domain. Research on inquiry-based learning, which also involves students learning to ask questions and formulate hypotheses, has revealed that most students appear to have difficulties with this specific skill (Schwartz, Lederman, & Crawford, 2004). In the biology curriculum, demonstrating, and experiencing, scientific practices is in particular useful for an adequate understanding of physiology, animal behaviour or ecology (Kremer, Specht, Urhanhe, & Mayer, 2014). Animal behaviour provides a great opportunity to learn science because students can directly observe animals and draw on their everyday experiences when formulating hypotheses (Bednekoff, 2005). So, teachers can readily use observation of animals to emphasize how scientists ask questions and frame and test hypotheses about animal behaviour. With respect to the scientific study of animal behaviour, it is generally agreed upon that four different questions should be answered to have a complete understanding: causation, ontogeny, function and evolution (Bateson & Laland, 2013a; Tinbergen, 1963; see below for more details). Hence, students learning about animal behaviour should be informed about this four-questions’ framework as a tool to study animal behaviour. Emphasizing these questions and the practices leading to answering them during instruction, will also support a deeper understanding of the topic of animal behaviour itself. In addition, another important cognitive skill central to deep conceptual understanding in animal behaviour, and sciences in general, is knowledge transfer. The cognitive skill of knowledge transfer (i.e. the ability to apply knowledge learned in one context to other novel contexts or to use it generatively) is thus also a fundamental goal in science education (Mayer, 2002). However, science education research has shown that most students have numerous difficulties regarding knowledge transfer (Anderson & Schönborn, 2008). For example, Pugh, Koskey, and Linnenbrink-Garcia (2014) recently described the patterns of transfer displayed by high school biology students learning about natural selection over time and concluded that transfer and duration of transfer were fairly limited. Natural selection is an important major concept in biology. As part of the larger concept of evolution, 4 it is considered a threshold concept that integrates the learning of many sub-concepts and is necessary to make sense of the field of biology (Dobzhansky, 1973). In this study, we specifically focus, within the subdomain of animal behaviour, on two cognitive skills that are important in the context of science education: the ability to ask the right scientific questions, in particular the extent to which Tinbergen’s four questions are represented in biology textbooks and in students’ explanations of behaviour, and the ability to transfer knowledge. Animal behaviour is agreed upon as an important major domain or concept in Biology. It is at the heart of the National Research Council (NRC)’s grand challenges for biology (NRC, 2010), since all of these challenges have strong behavioural elements. The latter was emphasized in a recent National Science Foundation (NSF) white paper (NSF Workshop, 2012). Animal Behaviour and Tinbergen’s four questions Behaviour is one of the most important properties of living organisms. Elucidating the mechanisms underlying behaviour and understanding its evolution represent major intellectual

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    34 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us