Pain in Aquatic Animals Lynne U

Pain in Aquatic Animals Lynne U

© 2015. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd | The Journal of Experimental Biology (2015) 218, 967-976 doi:10.1242/jeb.088823 REVIEW Pain in aquatic animals Lynne U. Sneddon* ABSTRACT injury and this is termed pain. The concept of pain occurring in Recent developments in the study of pain in animals have animals has been extensively debated, with some authors demonstrated the potential for pain perception in a variety of wholly suggesting only primates and humans can experience the adverse aquatic species such as molluscs, crustaceans and fish. This allows affective component as they possess a human (or similar in us to gain insight into how the ecological pressures and differential life primates) neocortex (Rose, 2002; Rose et al., 2014). Opposing this history of living in a watery medium can yield novel data that inform opinion, scientists suggest that the negative experience that the comparative physiology and evolution of pain. Nociception is the accompanies tissue damage is crucial in altering an animal’s simple detection of potentially painful stimuli usually accompanied by subsequent behaviour to perform protective and guarding reactions, a reflex withdrawal response, and nociceptors have been found in enabling the animal to avoid such stimuli in future, and for aquatic invertebrates such as the sea slug Aplysia. It would seem avoidance learning to occur (Sneddon et al., 2014). This implies that adaptive to have a warning system that allows animals to avoid life- the unpleasant internal state of experiencing pain goes hand in hand threatening injury, yet debate does still continue over the capacity for with its perception as it has to be such a strong aversive stimulus to non-mammalian species to experience the discomfort or suffering ensure animals will alter future behaviour and learn from the event. that is a key component of pain rather than a nociceptive reflex. If this were not the case, animals would continue to damage Contemporary studies over the last 10 years have demonstrated that themselves repeatedly, resulting in disease, loss of limbs and even bony fish possess nociceptors that are similar to those in mammals; mortality. It is unlikely that animals living in very different that they demonstrate pain-related changes in physiology and environments will have developed the same nociceptive or pain- behaviour that are reduced by painkillers; that they exhibit higher detecting neural machinery as humans. Evolution and life history brain activity when painfully stimulated; and that pain is more place very diverse pressures on different animal groups as well as important than showing fear or anti-predator behaviour in bony fish. exposing them to differing types of nociceptive stimuli. For The neurophysiological basis of nociception or pain in fish is example, fish living in an aquatic world can maintain buoyancy, demonstrably similar to that in mammals. Pain perception in so the risk of collision due to gravity is likely to be rare compared invertebrates is more controversial as they lack the vertebrate brain, with a terrestrial vertebrate (Sneddon, 2004). Therefore, evolution, yet recent research evidence confirms that there are behavioural ecology and life history may have shaped nociceptive and pain changes in response to potentially painful events. This review will systems in aquatic animals to meet the demands of their assess the field of pain perception in aquatic species, focusing on fish environment in quite a dissimilar way to terrestrial animals and selected invertebrate groups to interpret how research findings (Broom, 2001; Rutherford, 2002). can inform our understanding of the physiology and evolution of pain. Studies on the bird brain have challenged old dogma on brain Further, if we accept these animals may be capable of experiencing evolution and have shown that the theory of linear and progressive the negative experience of pain, then the wider implications of human evolution, where the cerebral cortex was believed to have evolved use of these animals should be considered. from lower to higher vertebrates, with birds at an intermediate stage, are incorrect (see Jarvis et al., 2005 for review). The avian cerebrum KEY WORDS: Animal pain, Crustaceans, Experimental ethics, Fish, was proposed to be relatively simple in structure and controlled Molluscs, Neurobiology, Nociceptors primitive behaviours (Ariëns Kappers et al., 1936). Modern experimentation demonstrates that the avian cortex developed Introduction: the occurrence of nociception and pain from pallial (or cortical-like), striatal and pallidal regions from a The ability to detect dangerous, damaging stimuli is adaptive common avian/reptilian ancestor. The avian pallial region is in terms of survival, and thus the evolution of an early warning differentially organized compared with the mammalian pallium or system in animals seems intuitive. Indeed, the nociceptive system, cortex in that the avian region is nuclear and the mammalian cortex which detects noxious, harmful, injury-causing stimuli such as is layered. However, the pallia of the two groups perform similar extremes of temperature, high mechanical pressure and irritant functions and have comparable connectivity. Thus, the avian chemicals, has been identified in invertebrates (e.g. Drosophila and cortical regions are different from the mammalian or human cortex Caenorhabditis elegans; Tobin and Bargmann, 2004; Neely et al., but have evolved analogous roles. Thus, we should expect that other 2010; Im and Galko, 2012) through to humans (reviewed in non-mammalian vertebrate taxa may have brain structures that differ Sneddon et al., 2014). Nociception is the simple perception of a from the mammalian cortex anatomically, but that may have noxious event and is typically accompanied by a reflex withdrawal evolved to perform similar functions. This example demonstrates response away from the source of damage. In humans, negative how the study of disparate animal groups can enlighten us about the ‘feelings’ of discomfort or suffering are experienced alongside the evolution of the nervous system. In this review, the evidence for pain and nociception in wholly aquatic animals will be considered by discussing the differences with terrestrial groups to ascertain University of Liverpool, Institute of Integrative Biology, The BioScience Building, how the watery environment may have resulted in anatomical, Liverpool L69 7ZB, UK. neurobiological and functional differences. Unfortunately, there are *Author for correspondence ([email protected]) negligible data on aquatic mammals and aquatic birds, and very few The Journal of Experimental Biology 967 REVIEW The Journal of Experimental Biology (2015) 218, 967-976 doi:10.1242/jeb.088823 data published on aquatic forms of amphibians (see Guenette et al., to avoid subsequent encounters; it may also perform less well on 2013); therefore, this review will focus upon the aquatic animal competing tasks, or act inappropriately when in pain such that pain groups where there is significant empirical evidence, specifically is an attention-dominating state. Sneddon et al. (2014) propose that fish, cephalopods and crustaceans. these criteria should not be considered in isolation but that specific Definitions of animal pain have proposed that there are two key animal groups must fulfil both sets to be considered capable of criteria animals must fulfil to be considered capable of experiencing experiencing pain (Table 1). Thus, this provides a framework to pain (Table 1; Sneddon et al., 2014). The first is that whole-animal identify how far aquatic animals meet these criteria, and where responses to a noxious, potentially painful event differ from those to future studies are needed if evidence is currently lacking. This innocuous stimulation. More specifically, it is suggested that: review will consider the evidence in fishes and selected invertebrate animals must have the neural apparatus to perceive damaging groups (crustaceans and molluscs), where there are substantial data stimuli with nociceptors, often free nerve endings that specifically from studies exploring nociception and pain, to discuss why detect harmful stimuli; the information is conveyed to the central differences occur between these animal groups and terrestrial nervous system (CNS) from the periphery; central processing occurs animals that may inform our understanding of the evolution and involving brain areas that innervate motivational and emotional comparative neurobiology and physiology of this important survival behaviour and learning; physiological responses are altered that may system (Table 2). be linked to a stress response; behavioural alterations are not simple reflexes, with long-term responses including protective behaviours Pain in fish and avoidance as part of the response; and finally all of these Prior to 2002 it was believed that fish could not perceive pain reactions should be reduced by the use of analgesics or painkillers because nociceptors, receptors that preferentially detect potentially (although note that these may only be effective in mammals and painful stimuli, had not been identified (Rose, 2002). However, specific compounds may not necessarily work in invertebrates with using electrophysiology and neuroanatomical approaches, different receptors, etc.; e.g. Barr and Elwood, 2011). The first nociceptors were subsequently identified in a teleost or bony fish, invertebrate species in which nociceptors were identified was the the rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    10 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us