When Cooperators Cheat

When Cooperators Cheat

NEWS & VIEWS EVOLUTION When cooperators cheat A study of a cuckoo species that usually shows cooperative nesting behaviour, but sometimes cheats at parenthood by laying eggs in others’ nests, reveals the benefits that have shaped the evolution of this parasitic tactic. See Letter p.96 ANDREW G. ZINK & JOHN M. EADIE a Solitary nesting and parasitic d Defer egg laying to next year he evolutionary conditions that drive egg laying are not favoured cheating versus cooperative nest- ing tactics in birds are a major focus b Cooperative nesting is favoured Tof interest in animal-behaviour research. On page 96, Riehl and Strong1 report a study of a cuckoo species called the greater ani c (Crotophaga major), which sometimes dis- plays a cheating behaviour when nesting called 2,3 conspecific brood parasitism , in addition to e Parasitic egg laying cooperative nesting behaviour. The authors in the same year tracked the identity of females and the eggs in Nest destroyed by predators their nests to assess the costs and benefits of Parasitic this alternative parasitic tactic. egg Conspecific brood parasitism occurs when a female lays her eggs in a nest belonging to another member of the same species, but does Parasite not provide any offspring care2,3. By contrast, in one specific form of cooperative breed- Hosts ing behaviour, the care and defence of the offspring in a nest are shared between two Figure 1 | Nesting strategies of a cuckoo species. Riehl and Strong1 report a study of the greater ani or more females (and, in some species, their (Crotophaga major) in which they tracked the identity of females and the eggs in their nests. a, b, At the mates)4. Crotophaga major is a rare example of start of the annual breeding season, a female bird has three nesting options. a, The two unfavoured options a species that exhibits both types of behaviour (that less than 2% of the birds chose) are solitary nesting, in which a single female and her mate care for the in the same population, providing an opportu- nest, and brood parasitism, in which a female lays eggs in a nest and departs without providing offspring nity to examine the evolutionary relationships care. b, The favoured option is cooperative nesting, in which a nest is shared by two or more females and between these two tactics. their mates. c, When cooperative nests were destroyed by predators, some birds deferred egg laying until they bred cooperatively in the next season (d), whereas others changed tactics to parasitic egg laying in the The authors tracked nests in the wild over same breeding season (e). Birds following options d or e had similar numbers of surviving offspring, and 11 breeding seasons, and used DNA analysis one of the two distinct behaviours was repeatedly chosen by the same individual. The equal fitness of these to identify the birds in each nest and deter- two behaviours suggests that they have evolved to be maintained as alternative tactics. mine the mother of each egg, using techniques that included non-destructive extraction of DNA from eggshell surfaces. Riehl and Strong why conspecific brood parasitism occurs5. for egg survival are so great (relative to the observed that females almost always show One possibility is the ‘super mother’ scenario lower survival of parasitic eggs associated cooperative breeding behaviour at the start in which females develop eggs in excess of the with host rejection) that it has evolved to be the of the breeding season. If predators destroyed optimal number for their own nest, and lay the default option; this would explain why parasit- a nest, the authors found that some affected extra eggs in other nests. This is a successful ism is pursued only after nest failure, rather females pursued a parasitic strategy in the same strategy in some birds and insects6–8. Another than as the sole option of choice or pursued breeding season, whereas others waited until the explanation, which has had little support9, is concurrently with cooperative nesting. next year’s breeding season to lay more eggs and that the females are specialized parasites that More than 300 bird species cooperatively nest cooperatively (Fig. 1). The authors report never construct their own nest. The third is breed, and 200 show conspecific brood para- that either cooperative breeding combined with that females are making the best of a bad situ- sitism5,9, but few species are found to exhibit parasitism after nest failure or solely cooperative ation in which parasitism is a last resort taken both. It has been proposed that cooperative breeding provided similar numbers of surviv- by a female that would otherwise have nested breeding and parasitism might represent ing offspring. The parasitic females laid more in the usual way10. Riehl and Strong provide extremes of offspring care by a female that eggs than the solely cooperative females, but the support for this hypothesis. contributes eggs to a nest already occupied by death rate of parasitic eggs was higher than that Why C. major individuals are not nor- another female2. Cooperative breeding might of non-parasitic eggs, owing to host rejection. mally parasitic breeders except as a response have evolved directly from parasitism if a host The authors found that any given individual to nest predation, or why they do not pursue provides incentives to entice a female parasite used just one of these two alternative breeding both para sitism and cooperative nesting in to remain at the nest and cooperate3. However, tactics repeatedly over many cases of nest loss. the absence of nest predation, is a mystery. Riehl and Strong show that the relationship Three explanations are usually given for Perhaps the benefits of cooperative breeding between cooperative breeding and parasitism is 34 | NATURE | VOL 567 | 7 MARCH 2019 ©2019 Spri nger Nature Li mited. All ri ghts reserved. ©2019 Spri nger Nature Li mited. All ri ghts reserved. NEWS & VIEWS RESEARCH more complicated than was previously thought, 4. Riehl, C. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 1728–1735 (2011). 9. Lyon, B. E. & Eadie, J. M. in Avian Brood Parasitism because parasitism seems to have evolved 5. Lyon, B. E. & Eadie, J. M. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 39, (ed. Soler, M.) 105–123 (Springer, 2017). as part of an existing system of co operative 343–363 (2008). 10. Petrie, M. & Moller, A. P. Trends Ecol. Evol. 6, 6. Åhlund, M. & Andersson, M. Nature 414, 600–601 315–320 (1991). breeding, rather than the other way around. (2001). 11. Loeb, M. L. G. Am. Nat. 161, 129–142 (2003). Intriguingly, the same single factor of high lev- 7. Lyon, B. E. Anim. Behav. 46, 911–928 (1993). 12. Andersson, M. Am. Nat. 189, 138–152 (2017). els of nest predation drives both behaviours. 8. Zink, A. G. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 54, 406–415 Cooperative breeding is favoured over solitary (2003). This article was published online on 27 February 2019. nesting (in which a single female and her mate care for the nest) because of predator pressure4. The idea that genetic relatedness between VIROLOGY individuals can affect the evolution of social interactions has had a central role in our understanding of cooperative breeding in many species, and some models2,3 suggest that Receptor bats for the kinship might also have a role in the evolution of brood parasitism. A brood parasite might actively target kin to increase the survival of next flu pandemic host eggs by buffering their predation risk as a result of adding parasite eggs11, or hosts might How bat influenza viruses infect cells has been unclear. The discovery that they accept eggs of non-nesting kin because that is bind to a cell receptor that is present in many different species raises concerns the parasite’s only opportunity to reproduce12. about their potential risk to humans. See Letter p.109 Parasitism might, therefore, sometimes have a cooperative aspect, blurring the distinction between cooperative breeding and parasit- SEWENDY S. BARCLAY Wild birds are the natural reservoir of most ism when kin are involved. However, Riehl influenza A viruses. Avian flu viruses infect and Strong show that kinship does not play ats are excellent hosts for viruses: they birds by binding to sialic acid receptors on a part in the parasitism of C. major, because are numerous, accounting for 20% of all the host cells (Fig. 1). The cells that line the the relatedness of the hosts and parasites was mammals on Earth, and prolific, existing human respiratory tract also display sialic not greater than that in the general popula- Bin colonies of up to 20 million individuals. Bats acid receptors, but these are slightly different tion. This meant that the authors could focus harbour dangerous pathogens that can spread from the receptors in birds. Avian flu viruses on the evolution of nesting tactics without to domestic animals and humans. Ebola, SARS can acquire the capacity to pass through the having to consider the influence of kinship. and Nipah viruses have all crossed from bats to air between humans when they undergo Why specific C. major females pursue humans, either directly or through intermedi- mutations in haemagglutinin, a glycoprotein, parasitism is unknown. The observation ate hosts1. The discovery in 2012 (ref. 2) that which forms the spikes on the virus particle that individual females consistently used this bats harbour influenza A viruses was alarm- that interact with the sialic acid receptors on tactic each time their nest failed, whereas others ing, because flu viruses are notoriously adept at host cells. The requirement for optimal recep- did not, suggests that there might be a heritable crossing from animals into humans and caus- tor binding is a major barrier to infection basis.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    2 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us