Hyperlexia: Systematic Review, Neurocognitive Modelling, and Outcome

Hyperlexia: Systematic Review, Neurocognitive Modelling, and Outcome

Accepted Manuscript Title: Hyperlexia: Systematic review, neurocognitive modelling, and outcome Authors: Alexia Ostrolenk, Baudouin Forgeot d’Arc, Patricia Jelenic, Fabienne Samson, Laurent Mottron PII: S0149-7634(16)30639-X DOI: http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.029 Reference: NBR 2839 To appear in: Received date: 18-10-2016 Revised date: 4-4-2017 Accepted date: 30-4-2017 Please cite this article as: Ostrolenk, Alexia, Forgeot d’Arc, Baudouin, Jelenic, Patricia, Samson, Fabienne, Mottron, Laurent, Hyperlexia: Systematic review, neurocognitive modelling, and outcome.Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.029 This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. 1 Hyperlexia: systematic review, neurocognitive modelling, and outcome Author names and affiliations Alexia Ostrolenka [email protected] Baudouin Forgeot d’Arca,b,c [email protected] Patricia Jelenica [email protected] Fabienne Samsona [email protected] Laurent Mottrona,b,c* [email protected] a Centre d’excellence en Troubles Envahissants du Développement de l’Université de Montréal (CETEDUM), Hôpital Rivière-des-Prairies, 7070 Boulevard Perras, H1E 1A4, Montréal, QC, Canada b Centre de recherche de l’Institut Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l’Université de Montréal, Québec, Canada c Département de Psychiatrie, Université de Montréal, H3T 1J4, Québec, Canada *Corresponding author 2 Highlights Hyperlexia is characterised by strong decoding skills and delayed comprehension. Hyperlexia is strongly associated with autism (84% of cases are on the spectrum). Autistic children learn to read through an inverted, non-communicative, pathway. Hyperlexia is mediated by the extended use of the perceptual expertise system. Favouring hyperlexia may be tested during intervention to foster communication. Abstract OSTROLENK, A., FORGEOT D’ARC, B., JELENIC, P., SAMSON, F., MOTTRON, L. - Hyperlexia: systematic review, neurocognitive modelling, and outcome. NEUROSCI BIOBAHAV REV XX(X) XXX-XXX, 2017 – Hyperlexia is defined as the co-occurrence of advanced reading skills relative to comprehension skills or general intelligence, the early acquisition of reading skills without explicit teaching, and a strong orientation toward written material, generally in the context of a neurodevelopmental disorder. In this systematic review of cases (N = 82) and group studies (including 912 participants of which 315 are hyperlexic), we address: whether the hyperlexic profile is associated with autism and why, whether models of non-autistic reading can teach us about 3 hyperlexia, and what additional information we can get from models specific to autistic cognitive functioning. We find that hyperlexia, or a hyperlexic-like profile, characterises a substantial portion of the autistic spectrum, in which the subcomponents of the typical reading architecture are altered and dissociated. Autistic children follow a chronologically inverted path when learning to read, and make extended use of the perceptual expertise system, specifically the visual word form recognition systems. We conclude by discussing the possible use of hyperlexic skills in intervention. Keywords: Hyperlexia, Autism, Reading, Decoding, Review, Enhanced Perceptual Functioning model, Visual Word Form Area 4 Table of contents 1. Introduction and definition ...................................................................................... 5 2. A systematic review: method .................................................................................... 6 2.1. Literature search .......................................................................................................... 6 2.2. Selection criteria ........................................................................................................... 7 2.3. Results ............................................................................................................................ 9 3. Prevalence and relation with autism ....................................................................... 9 3.1. Prevalence of hyperlexia in autism ............................................................................. 9 3.2. Prevalence of autism in hyperlexia ........................................................................... 11 4. Typical reading ........................................................................................................ 12 4.1. Neurocognitive models of reading ............................................................................. 13 4.1.1. Visual word form recognition .............................................................................. 13 4.1.2. Grapheme-to-phoneme conversion. ..................................................................... 15 4.1.3. Semantic access ................................................................................................... 16 4.2. Reading acquisition in typical development ............................................................. 17 5. Cognitive processes in autistic hyperlexia ............................................................. 19 5.1. Autism, hyperlexia, and the three steps of reading ................................................. 20 5.1.1. Visual perception ................................................................................................. 20 5.1.2. Grapheme-to-phoneme conversion ...................................................................... 25 5.1.3. Semantic access ................................................................................................... 28 5.2. Reading acquisition in hyperlexic children and developmental pathway ............. 31 6. Discussion ................................................................................................................. 33 6.1. Hyperlexia and autism ............................................................................................... 34 6.2. Models of hyperlexia .................................................................................................. 36 6.2.1. Why do we need a new model for hyperlexia? .................................................... 36 6.2.2. Towards a perception-oriented model for hyperlexia .......................................... 39 6.3. Development ................................................................................................................ 40 6.4. Intervention issues ...................................................................................................... 42 References Figure Captions 5 1. Introduction and definition The term “hyperlexia” was coined in 1967 by Silberberg and Silberberg (1968; 1967). Four features consistently describe hyperlexia: (1) the presence of an accompanying neurodevelopmental disorder; (2) advanced reading skills, relative to comprehension skills or general intelligence; (3) an early acquisition of reading skills without explicit teaching; and (4) a strong orientation toward reading material (Needleman, 1982). The same year, Rutter and Lockyer noticed that the same proportion of both their groups of “psychotic children” and control children could read at a normal level for their age, despite very little schooling for the “psychotic” group. They described the example of a boy who could read well above the average level, although he had been excluded from school at the age of six (Rutter and Lockyer, 1967). However, cases of hyperlexia were described as early as 1930 (Phillips), i.e. before the introduction of both hyperlexia and autism as a syndrome. Mentions of the hyperlexic profile can be found even earlier, in 1918, when Hollingworth and Winford wrote, “Cases where a generally stupid (sic) child is innately gifted with special ability to master the mechanics of reading, for example, are no doubt as frequent as cases where a generally capable child learns them with difficulty.” (Hollingworth and Winford, 1918, cited in Thompson, 1966, p.24). Fifty years after the term was first introduced, hyperlexia is often reported as one of the savant abilities in autism. However, its neurocognitive underpinning and how it relates to autistic cognition and typical reading acquisition, are yet to be established. Literature on hyperlexia consists of a large number of descriptive reports, combined with a small number of empirical studies testing specific 6 assumptions about its prevalence and mechanisms. Beyond the established cross- sectional differences in decoding and understanding abilities, the distinct, developmental pathway of hyperlexics remains to be characterized. The last academic review on hyperlexia was published 14 years ago (Grigorenko et al., 2003). Multiple case and group studies have been published in the last decade, including the first imaging studies of hyperlexia, making a new review necessary. Based on a systematic review, we first report the current state of knowledge on the prevalence of hyperlexia and its relation with autism. We present the cognitive processes underlying reading in non-hyperlexic, mature readers, and the sequence of typical

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    70 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us