Influences of Semantic and Syntactic Incongruence on Readiness

Influences of Semantic and Syntactic Incongruence on Readiness

View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLEprovided by Frontiers - Publisher Connector published: 27 May 2014 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00296 Influences of semantic and syntactic incongruence on readiness potential in turn-end anticipation Hendrik Wesselmeier*, Stefanie Jansen and Horst M. Müller Experimental Neurolinguistics Group, Collaborative Research Center “Alignment in Communication” (SFB 673), Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany Edited by: Knowing when it is convenient to take a turn in a conversation is an important task John J. Foxe, Albert Einstein College for dialog partners. As it appears that this decision is made before the transition point of Medicine, USA has been reached, it seems to involve anticipation. There are a variety of studies in the Reviewed by: literature that provide possible explanations for turn-end anticipation.This study particularly Vanessa Taler, University of Ottawa, Canada focuses on how turn-end anticipation relies on syntactic and/or semantic information during John F.Shelley-Tremblay, University of utterance processing, as tested with syntactically and semantically violated sentences. South Alabama, USA With a combination reaction time and EEG experiment, we used the onset latencies of the *Correspondence: readiness potential (RP) to uncover possible differences in response preparation. Although Hendrik Wesselmeier, Experimental the mean anticipation timing accuracy (ATA) values of the behavioral test were all within a Neurolinguistics Group, Collaborative Research Center “Alignment in similar time range (control sentences: 108 ms, syntactically violated sentences: 93 ms and Communication” (SFB 673), Bielefeld semantically violated sentences: 116 ms), we found evidence that response preparation is University, P.O. Box 100131, Bielefeld indeed different for syntactically and semantically violated sentences in comparison with 33501, Germany e-mail: hendrik.wesselmeier@uni- control sentences. Our preconscious EEG data, in the form of RP results, indicated a bielefeld.de response preparation onset to sentence end interval of 1452 ms in normal sentences, 937 ms in sentences with syntactic violations and 944 ms in sentences with semantic violations. Compared with control sentences, these intervals resulted in a significant RP interruption for both sentence types and indicate an interruption of preconscious response preparation. However, the behavioral response to sentence types occurred at comparable time points. Keywords: turn-end anticipation, spoken language, EEG, syntactic errors, semantic errors, readiness potential INTRODUCTION determined by the interlocutor during the turn itself. The com- Human communication usually occurs with a bi-directional infor- plex task of determining the next transition-relevant place and mation exchange, which can be performed coevally or alternately. preparing what to say requires highly synchronized turn-taking An example of simultaneous communication would be “mimic to ensure that virtually no gap will occur when the next speaker signals”(non-verbal) in which communication partners are simul- begins to speak. For example, this highly precise interplay can be taneously smiling. In the visual modality, a smile can be answered observed in two phenomena: back-channeling and turn-taking. with a smile while the communication partner is still smiling, i.e., Back-channeling is the precise use of brief interjections (e.g.,“aha” the smiles can overlap without interfering with each other. How- or “yeah”) that are not treated as an attempt to take a turn but ever, in a more complex information exchange such as spoken rather used to signal attention, understanding and assent. They language, a sequential organization of the signals is required. In can occur in the short gaps within the interlocutor’s turn but also contrast to non-verbal signals, it is socially restrictive not to have can overlap. For turn-taking, however, the decision concerning two interlocutors speaking at the same time because speaking and when to speak depends on several higher cognitive and motiva- listening simultaneously is not easily possible. Indeed, in a conver- tional considerations. Figure 1 shows an example of back-channels sation, we need to process and understand a perceived utterance and turn-taking in a natural dialog. before we can react by replying. Thus, in conversations between Turn-taking may depend on situational factors, such as the two or more interlocutors, the organization of taking turns is fun- relevance and pragmatic necessity of a possible utterance, how damental such that predominately only one partner speaks at a confident the interlocutors are about the reliability of the informa- time. Sacks et al. (1974) already demonstrated that conversation- tion, and social status of the interlocutors. However, Wilson and alists tend to avoid silences and overlapping speech. To achieve Wilson (2005) claim the precise timing of the speech onset at the this highly coordinated timing, we need to know in advance when moment the speaker begins to speak is mechanistically governed. our interlocutor will finish his turn to prepare our response. Many Turn-taking appears to be robust across different languages studies have attempted to explain this ability and have formulated (Stivers et al., 2009). Infants develop turn-taking ability well before a number of hypotheses about the information that listeners use language acquisition (Beebe et al., 1988), suggesting that turn- to anticipate utterance endings (e.g., Grosjean, 1996; Magyari and taking ability is independent of linguistic development (Yoshida De Ruiter, 2012). Because the length and content of what is said and Okanoya, 2005). However, early turn-taking ability is unlikely in a turn is not specified in advance, this information must be to be influenced by syntax within an utterance or the semantics Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org May 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 296 | 1 Wesselmeier et al. Semantic and syntactic incongruence in turn-end anticipation FIGURE 1 | Example of a dialog with accurate turn-taking (within 250 ms) indicated by the gray bar and backchannel (bc) indicated by the dotted lines. of a message. Nevertheless, both of these factors are needed to the left frontal area of subjects 120–320 ms before the speech form a correctly communicated illocutionary act. To prepare their onset. In their task, subjects were asked to count numbers silently, response and to take their turn at the right moment, listeners need while at a random time, a visual cue appeared, after which the to know in advance when the current turn is going to end. What subjects counted the numbers aloud. As this pre-activation for type of information do listeners base their knowledge of a turn- word production is highly unlikely to occur before gap detection end on? The aspects under discussion are intonation (e.g., Caspers, is completed, it can only follow gap detection. Thus, these 120– 2003), prosody (e.g., Wells and Macfarlane, 1998), syntactic and 320 ms pre-activations for word production result in a 200 ms semantic information (e.g., Sacks et al.,1974; De Ruiter et al.,2006) gap as the minimal response time being insufficient for the reac- and pragmatic information (e.g., Ford and Thompson, 1996). tion model because even the minimum of a 120 ms pre-activation To test the possible influences on turn-end anticipation, De prior to word production would reduce a 200 ms gap as the min- Ruiter et al. (2006) manipulated the presence of the symbolic imal response time to 80 ms for gap detection. In the reaction (lexico-syntactic) content of utterances recorded in natural con- model (Heldner and Edlund, 2010), not taking higher cognitive versations. With non-recognizable words, participants’ button- processes for utterance preparation into account, a 120 ms pre- press reaction-time increased significantly. Their experimental activation during word production can only start after 200 ms for results revealed that the lexico-syntactic content of an utterance minimal gap detection. In other words, when turn-taking with is necessary for successful turn-end anticipation. Magyari and De gaps under at least 320 ms, listeners must be able to anticipate Ruiter (2008) attempted to determine how interlocutors use the when the upcoming transition relevant place will occur to pre- lexico-syntactic information to anticipate the ends of turns so pare their response. Given these conflicting findings that speak for accurately. They hypothesized that listeners can predict when a and against the general perspective of anticipation in turn-taking, turn is going to end because they can predict how it ends. Using a further investigation is required. gating task, they were able to demonstrate that participants were Turn-end anticipation is usually measured by behavioral best at predicting the final two words of those turns that were responses, for instance, anticipation timing accuracy (ATA),which anticipated most accurately in a previous experiment by De Ruiter reflects conscious behavioral processes. The ATA is the accuracy et al. (2006). of the interval from the sentence-offset to response. However, the Contrary to anticipation in turn-taking, a fast reaction of the possible similar ATA results between conditions can nevertheless listener can offer an alternative explanation for turn-taking. Held- fail to reflect the operation of different anticipatory performances, ner and Edlund (2010), for example, investigated

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    9 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us