The Fusiform Face Area Is Selective for Faces Not Animals

The Fusiform Face Area Is Selective for Faces Not Animals

Brain imaging neuroreport NeuroReport 10, 183±187 (1999) TO test whether the human fusiform face area (FFA) responds not only to faces but to anything human or The fusiform face area is animate, we used fMRI to measure the response of the FFA to six new stimulus categories. The strongest selective for faces not responses were to stimuli containing faces: human faces (2.0% signal increase from ®xation baseline) and human animals heads (1.7%), with weaker but still strong responses to whole humans (1.5%) and animal heads (1.3%). Re- Nancy Kanwisher,CA Damian Stanley sponses to whole animals (1.0%) and human bodies without heads (1.0%) were signi®cantly stronger than and Alison Harris responses to inanimate objects (0.7%), but responses to animal bodies without heads (0.8%) were not. These Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, results demonstrate that the FFA is selective for faces, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 79 Amherst not for animals. NeuroReport 10:183±187 # 1999 Lip- pincott Williams & Wilkins. Street, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA and Massachusetts General Hospital Nuclear Magnetic Resonance Center, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA Key words: Category-speci®c impairments; Extrastriate cortex; Face perception; FFA; fMRI; Fusiform face area; Visual cortex CACorresponding Author Introduction is as strong as the response to human faces [6]. Second, a recent report found that the face area can Numerous behavioral and physiological studies have be activated by animal stimuli even when their heads provided evidence that the brain contains special- are occluded [8]. Third, de®cits in identifying ani- purpose mechanisms that are selectively involved in mals are often [9] (though not always [10,11]) the perception of faces. Recent evidence from neu- associated with prosopagnosia. roimaging in humans has demonstrated a region in Even if the FFA itself does not respond strongly the fusiform gyrus called the fusiform face area, or to animals, there is evidence that other cortical FFA [1], which responds both strongly [2,3] and regions may be specialized for processing animals or selectively [1,4,5] to faces. That is, the response of living things. Several imaging studies [12±14] have the FFA to faces is at least twice as large as the reported regions showing greater responses to ani- response to non-face stimuli such as inanimate mals than inanimate objects (though one [15] found objects, ¯owers, houses, and human hands. On the this for only black and white, not colored stimuli), other hand the FFA response has been shown to and a number of neuropsychological cases with generalize across a wide variety of face stimuli, and category-speci®c impairments for animals but not is of similar magnitude for front-view faces, pro®le tools [10,13,16] have been reported. Although the faces, inverted grey-scale faces [7], two-tone `Moon- animal-speci®c impairments usually affect higher- ey' faces, cat faces and cartoon faces, despite sub- level semantic tasks, in at least one case [10] a de®cit stantial differences in the low-level visual features for recognizing animals (but not faces) was reported present in these stimuli [6]. Given both the speci®- using the object decision task, which does not city and generality of the FFA response, the most require naming or understanding. parsimonious hypothesis is that this region is selec- The primary aim of the present study was to test tively involved in face perception. whether the FFA responds selectively to faces as However, it remains possible that the FFA might previously claimed, or if instead it responds more respond more generally to anything animate, or generally to animals. Because animals have faces, perhaps to anything human. The very weak response which have already been shown to activate the FFA, which has been reported to hands [1] and to photo- [6] three different kinds of animal pictures were graphs of the backs of human heads [6] suggest that used as stimuli: photographs of whole animals, ani- stimuli containing non-face human body parts are mals without their heads showing, and animals with not suf®cient to drive the area strongly. However, only the head visible. A second purpose of the study there are several reasons to think that photographs was to test whether any region of the ventral of animals might be. First, the response to cat faces occipitotemporal pathway shows a stronger response 0959-4965 # Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Vol 10 No 1 18 January 1999 183 neuroreport N. Kanwisher, D. Stanley and A. Harris to animals than to inanimate objects. We also meas- two or more functional localizer scans containing ured the fMRI response to photographs of humans, epochs of faces and either objects or houses, and either whole or with only head or body showing, four scans on the critical new test materials. Each both as comparison cases for the animal conditions subject performed two of the test scans in a passive and to test whether the FFA responds to compo- viewing task, and two in a `1-back' task in which nents of human forms other than faces. they were instructed to press a button whenever they saw two identical pictures in a row (1±3 Materials and Methods repetitions occurred in each epoch). Each scan lasted 5 min and 36 s and consisted of sixteen 16-second Subjects: Five healthy normal adults (four women), epochs with ®xation periods interleaved as shown in ages 21±39, with normal or corrected-to-normal Fig. 1b. During each epoch, 20 different photo- vision, volunteered or participated for payment. All graphs of the same type were shown. Each photo- subjects gave informed written consent, and all graph was presented for 300 ms followed by a blank procedures were approved by MIT and MGH hu- interval of 500 ms. There were two epochs for each man subjects committees. of the eight stimulus types within each scan; order was counterbalanced over two versions of each Stimuli: All stimuli consisted of greyscale photo- experiment (ABCD±EFGH±HGFE±DCBA for graphs or photorealistic drawings 300 3 300 pixels in version 1 and HGFE±DCBA±ABCD±EFGH for size (which ®t within a square of ,108 on a side). version 2). The raw data from version 1 were Original photographs of animals and people were reordered so that the time courses from the two digitally edited in Adobe Photoshop to create head versions were compatible and could be averaged only and body only versions (Fig. 1a). Note that together. These procedures are described in more stimuli were not resized after editing; the head only detail in previous papers [1,17]. and body only components constitute non-normal- ized subsets of the corresponding whole stimulus. Scanning procedures: Scanning was carried out on There were six main experimental conditions, cre- the 3 T GE scanner (modi®ed by ANMR to per- ated by crossing animals/people 3 whole/head only/ form Echo Planar Imaging) at the MGH-NMR body only. Two other conditions, front-view face Center in Charlestown, MA. A custom bilateral photos and inanimate objects, were also included in surface coil (built by Tommy Vaughn) provided a the experiment to provide benchmarks of maximal high signal-to-noise ratio in posterior brain regions. and minimal FFA responses to visual stimuli. High resolution anatomical and functional images were collected using twelve 6 mm near-coronal slices Experimental procedures: Each subject was run on extending from the occipital pole into the posterior a Animal Animal Whole Human Human Whole Objects Bodies Heads Animals Bodies Heads Humans Faces Passive 0.7 0.8 1.2 0.9 1.0 1.5 1.4 1.7 1-Back 0.6 0.9 1.4 1.0 1.1 1.9 1.5 2.3 Average 0.7 0.8 1.3 1.0 1.0 1.7 1.5 2.0 b F AB HHWA WHAH HB OO HBAH WH WAHH AB F 2 1.5 1 0.5 % Signal Change 0 FIG. 1. (a) Examples of the eight different stimulus types and the average percentage signal increase from ®xation in the FFA for each averaged over all subjects. Data from passive and 1-back runs, and the average of the two, are given separately. Percentage signal change was calculated individually for each subject using that subject's ®xation activation as baseline and then averaging across subjects. (b) The time course of the percentage change in MR signal intensity in the FFA over the period of the scan. O, objects; AB, animal bodies; AH, animal heads; WA, whole animals; HB, human bodies; HH, human heads; WH, whole humans; F, faces; black dot indicates ®xation epochs. 184 Vol 10 No 1 18 January 1999 Selectivity of the fusiform face area neuroreport third of the temporal lobe. Standard imaging proce- No interactions reached signi®cance (all F , 2.5, all dures were used (gradient echo pulse sequence, p . 0.15). Two-factor ANOVAs compared pairs of TR 2s, TE 30 ms, ¯ip angle 908, 1808 offset conditions crossed with task; the results of these 25 ms, 168 images/slice). A bite bar was used to analyses are summarized in Table 1. For these minimize head motion. analyses, none of the interactions of condition 3 task reached signi®cance. Data analysis: Each subject's FFA was identi®ed from the functional localizer scans as the set of all Other activated areas: For each subject, we also contiguous voxels in the mid-fusiform gyrus that computed Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistics on each of showed signi®cantly greater activation to greyscale the voxels scanned to test whether the response in front-view faces compared to houses or to common that region was greater during viewing of (whole) objects in a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test using a animals than during viewing of objects.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    5 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us