Marine Ecology Progress Series 631:157

Marine Ecology Progress Series 631:157

Vol. 631: 157–164, 2019 MARINE ECOLOGY PROGRESS SERIES Published November 21 https://doi.org/10.3354/meps13159 Mar Ecol Prog Ser OPENPEN ACCESSCCESS Aposematism in pink warty sea cucumbers: independent effects of chromatic and achromatic cues Amanda Y. H. Lim*, Ian Z. W. Chan*,**, Luis R. Carrasco, Peter A. Todd Department of Biological Sciences, National University of Singapore, 14 Science Drive 4, Singapore 117557 ABSTRACT: The importance of chromatic and achromatic cues in aposematism is well estab- lished, but in situ experiments investigating their comparative roles within a single warning signal remain rare. In a predation experiment, we examined their relative importance in pink warty sea cucumbers Cercodemas anceps Selenka, 1867, by asking (1) Do chromatic and achromatic cues have significant effects on attack rate? (2) Are their effects independent? (3) Does one have more influence on attack rate than the other? Using a multispectral imaging-based technique, we fabri- cated 4 types of clay models based on the hues, chroma and luminance levels of the animals and the background. Models were deployed in sets of 4 at 2 sites (n = 65 sets) for 3 days, and the num- ber of imprints from predation attempts was recorded. A generalised linear mixed model analysis showed that both types of cues had significant but independent effects on attack rates and that chromatic cues had a greater effect compared to achromatic ones. This study, the first manipula- tive investigation into holothurian aposematism, demonstrates the potential for chromatic and achromatic cues to play distinct roles in animal signalling, and highlights the importance of accu- rate experimental models. KEY WORDS: Warning colouration · Holothurian · Marine predation · Conspicuousness · Multispectral imaging · Cercodemas anceps 1. INTRODUCTION 1986) by naïve predators, and reduces recognition errors (Guilford 1986) and increases distinctiveness Aposematism, the association between warning (Merilaita & Ruxton 2007) for experienced predators. signals and prey unprofitability, is a well known phe- Conspicuousness, however, is itself poorly defined in nomenon with a long history of study, especially in the literature (Harvey & Paxton 1981) and is often terrestrial environments (e.g. Brodie 1993, Prudic et loosely used to refer to multiple related yet distinct al. 2007). However, less is known about aposematism properties of an animal’s appearance, which com- in marine species. Only opisthobranchs have been prises both chromatic and achromatic cues (Osorio & studied (e.g. Ritson-Williams & Paul 2007), while in Vorobyev 2005). other marine species with both conspicuous coloura- Chromatic cues consist of hue (or spectral position, tion and chemical defences, aposematism is gener- which is perceived as differences in light spectra) ally assumed without experimental verification — and chroma (or spectral purity; Osorio & Vorobyev since this claim was made by Rudman (1991), only a 2005). The achromatic cue in colour is luminance, handful of studies on nudibranch aposematism have also often referred to as apparent lightness or bright- been published (e.g. Aguado & Marin 2007). A key ness, which is a measure of intensity or light element of aposematic signalling is conspicuousness, reflectance (Osorio & Vorobyev 2005). Numerous which improves avoidance learning (Roper & Wistow previous studies have shown that animals with hues *These authors contributed equally to this work © The authors 2019. Open Access under Creative Commons by **Corresponding author: [email protected] Attribution Licence. Use, distribution and reproduction are un - restricted. Authors and original publication must be credited. Publisher: Inter-Research · www.int-res.com 158 Mar Ecol Prog Ser 631: 157–164, 2019 or chroma levels not common in their environment tion that appears conspicuous in its natural environ- display strong chromatic contrast against the back- ment, the tropical intertidal seagrass meadows rang- ground, contributing to recognition and avoidance ing from southern China to northern Australia. Like for experienced predators, e.g. domestic chickens most other holothurians, this species also possesses a Gallus gallus domesticus preying on larvae of the chemical defence in the form of cytotoxic triterpene seed bug Tropidothorax leucopterus (Gamberale- saponins (Cuong et al. 2015), which suggests an Stille 2001). Similarly, luminance contrast plays a aposematic function for its conspicuous colouration. critical role in signal recognition of simple monochro- However, the protective value of its appearance has matic lepidopteran eyespots by experienced avian not been confirmed experimentally. Aposematic sig- predators (Stevens et al. 2008). Signal learning and nals can be effective in marine environments (e.g. memory can also be influenced by hue itself irrespec- Ritson-Williams & Paul 2007), but signal efficacy may tive of chromatic contrast (Harvey & Paxton 1981) or differ in the aquatic medium compared to more well- by high luminance contrast against the background, studied terrestrial examples. For example, water e.g. in predators such as great tits Parus major (San- itself causes patchy lighting conditions (and may dre et al. 2010) and the colour-blind Chinese mantid thereby affect achromatic cues) and disproportion- Tenodera aridifolia sinensis (Prudic et al. 2007). Pred- ately attenuates light of longer wavelengths (red and ators have also been shown to innately avoid certain infrared) with increasing depth (affecting chromatic hues (Miller & Pawlik 2013) or high luminance in the cues) (Pegau et al. 1997), and the nature and concen- form of specular reflectance (glossiness) (Waldron et tration of suspended solids and organic matter within al. 2017). the water column also alter the ambient light field Although it is well known that animals use chro- (Gallegos & Moore 2000). Hence, it would be useful matic and achromatic cues for different purposes to demonstrate whether the appearance of C. anceps (Osorio & Vorobyev 2005), few studies have at - is indeed aposematic. tempted to tease apart their individual roles in the Our study aimed to investigate whether the ap- efficacy of the same signal (Sandre et al. 2010, Skel- pearance of C. anceps has an aposematic function, horn et al. 2016). For example, while Prudic et al. and if so, to disentangle the relative importance of (2007) suggested that both types of cues could to - chromatic and achromatic cues. We conducted a field- gether increase the efficiency of aposematic coloura- based predation experiment using modelling clay tion for predators with colour vision, this interaction replicas of C. anceps, which were fabricated to match has yet to be tested. Psychophysics-based studies target hues, chroma and luminance levels using a ro- (e.g. Osorio & Vorobyev 2005) have shown the per- bust procedure, and asked 3 questions: (1) Do chro- ception of chromatic and achromatic cues to be dis- matic and achromatic cues have significant effects on tinct in animal visual systems, as comprehensively attack rate? (2) Are their effects independent? (3) reviewed by Stevens (2007), giving rise to questions Does one have more influence on attack rate than the such as whether one may be more important than the other? We hypothesised that both types of cues would other, whether their relative importance may change in different situations, and whether their effects in - teract in an additive or synergistic manner. In order to create a more complete understanding of how ani- mals communicate information to one another visu- ally, these questions need to be answered, and it is hence necessary for studies to examine the effects of chromatic and achromatic cues separately. However, doing so has thus far proven difficult to achieve. One reason for this knowledge gap is the difficulty of reli- ably manipulating specific aspects of an organism’s appearance whilst keeping the other constant. This is an issue that we address here. Fig. 1. (a) Live pink warty sea cucumber, (b) bright pink-yel- One marine taxon containing conspicuously col - low model (with similar hues, chroma and luminance to the oured, unpalatable species are the holothurians. For actual animal), (c) dull pink-yellow model (with similar hues and chroma but lower luminance), (d) bright green-brown example, the pink warty sea cucumber Cercodemas model (with different hues and chroma but similar lumi- anceps Selenka, 1867 (Fig. 1a), from the family nance) and (e) dull green-brown model (with different hues Cucumariidae, has a bright pink and yellow coloura- and chroma and lower luminance) Lim et al.: Aposematism in pink warty sea cucumbers 159 have significant, interacting effects but that chromatic chroma (also known as saturation) was calculated cues would have a greater protective effect. following Agoston (2005): ⎧ ⎪0, ifL= 1 = ⎨ Chroma max(R,G,B)− min(R,G,B) (1) 2. MATERIALS AND METHODS ⎪ , otherwise ⎩⎪ 12−−L 1 2.1. Model fabrication where: max(R,G,B) is the largest of the R, G or B val- ues; min(R,G,B) is the smallest of the R, G or B values; Four types of models were fabricated using non- and L = [max(R,G,B) + min(R,G,B)] / 2. toxic modelling clay (Sargent Art Modeling Clay Objective RGB values provide an accurate represen- Plastilina, in ‘Cream’) and Americolor Soft Gel Paste tation of appearance as they measure the actual inten- food colouring to display chromatic cues (i.e. hues sity of the light coming from an object (Troscianko & and chroma levels) and achromatic cues (i.e. lumi- Stevens

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    8 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us