Variation in the Acoustic Signaling of Picoides Woodpeckers

Variation in the Acoustic Signaling of Picoides Woodpeckers

City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works School of Arts & Sciences Theses Hunter College Fall 1-5-2018 Slave to the Rhythm: Variation in the Acoustic Signaling of Picoides Woodpeckers Alex Baer How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/hc_sas_etds/244 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] Running head: GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN WOODPECKER DRUMS 1 Slave to the Rhythm: Variation in the Acoustic Signaling of Picoides Woodpeckers by Alex Baer Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Animal Behavior and Conservation, Hunter College The City University of New York [2017] Thesis Sponsor: December 28, 2017 Peter Moller Date [Signature of Thesis Sponsor] Second Reader: December 28, 2017 Nicolas Mathevon Date [Signature of Second Reader] Third Reader December 28, 2017 Andrea Baden Date [Signature of Second Reader] GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN WOODPECKER DRUMS 2 Table of Contents Abstract 3 Introduction 4 Method Subjects and Materials 8 Procedure 10 Results 13 Discussion 16 Conclusion 21 Acknowledgements 22 References 23 Tables & Figures 28 Glossary of Key Terms 43 GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN WOODPECKER DRUMS 3 Abstract We explored rhythmic variation in acoustic signals by geography across four species of Picoides woodpeckers. We hypothesized that there would be an effect of geographic location on the cadence of drums and rattle-type calls. No significant effect was observed. However, drum cadence was observed to significantly impact conspecific recognition in playbacks. Keywords: Picoides, cadence, geographic variation GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN WOODPECKER DRUMS 4 Geographic Variation in Acoustic Signaling of Picoides Woodpeckers Using sound to communicate information from one organism to another is a ubiquitous characteristic in the animal kingdom as it is invaluable to engaging in social behavior (Hauser, 1996). Many groups of animals have evolved fascinating and unique communication systems, though few are as robust or as well-known as those of birds. Chief among these systems is that of acoustic communication: nearly all birds use some form of acoustic signals, and several taxa have developed complex acoustic sequences (Bradbury & Vehrencamp, 2011). As such, using birds as model organisms in the field of bioacoustics and the study of acoustic communication in animals has become widespread as sound recording technology has advanced (Merchant et al., 2015). Most birds communicate primarily through vocalizations, which are produced through the syrinx, a derived vocal organ that evolved in birds as early as the Late Cretaceous (Trevisan & Mindlin, 2009; Clarke et al., 2016). The most complex of these vocalizations are signals collectively referred to as ‘songs’, which are series of vocal sequences that are produced for a variety of social functions (Searcy & Andersson, 1986). Among birds, the most complex songs are produced by passerines, hence known as songbirds, a group that includes such vocally complex species as wrens, finches, and titmice (Beecher & Brenowitz, 2005). Songs are used to achieve two major functions: territory maintenance and mate attraction (Baker, 2001). Territory maintenance is largely achieved through male-male communication: song playback experiments have found that male songbirds are much slower to occupy territorial space in which many complex song types can be heard (Krebs, Ashcroft, & Webber, 1978). Research into how complex vocalizations such as song facilitates mate attraction is well-documented. Lehrman (1959) was the first to document this in birds: he showed that vocalizations from male ring doves (Streptopelia capicola) stimulate the GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN WOODPECKER DRUMS 5 release of the follicle-stimulating hormone (FSH) in female ring doves. Further studies found that female canaries (Serinus canarius) laid more eggs and built their nests faster when exposed to more complex song types (Kroodsma, 1976). Recent research suggests that in over half of all passerine species, female songbirds will readily produce song vocalizations as much as males (Odom et al., 2014). Additionally, it is well known that there are often geographical differences in song types in songbirds, which are sometimes referred to as ‘dialects’ (Marler & Tamura, 1962). In addition to inherently conveying the group identity of the singer or signaller, there is also evidence that dialects influence sexual selection. In a study of yellowhammers (Emberiza citrinella), a Eurasian songbird, females from Norway responded with more solicitation displays when exposed to their natal dialect than when they were exposed to dialects from nearby Denmark (Baker et al., 1987). However, some groups of birds can produce sounds mechanically, or independent of the syrinx, and may do so using their feathers or bills (Koch, Krakauer, & Patricelli, 2015). Soma and Mori (2015) discovered that bill-clicking behavior in the Javan Sparrow (Lonchura oryzivora) followed similar syntactic rules as their song. Among non-passerine birds which produce mechanical signals, woodpeckers are arguably the most well known. In addition to vocalizing, woodpecker individuals communicate by drumming on hard surfaces, e.g. tree trunks or limbs; this is done independently of the tapping associated with feeding or boring into tree bark (Gibson, 2006). Stark et al. (1998) outlined four principal variables of drums: the number of drums in a series, the length of the drum series, the cadence or rhythm (defined as the number of drums divided by the total time), and the interdrum interval. These variables provide a robust quantifiable framework for bioacoustic analyses of woodpecker drumming. GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN WOODPECKER DRUMS 6 Compared to their songbird cousins, the vocalizations of woodpeckers are relatively simple (Lucas et al., 2002). Of concern is whether such drumming is analogous to the songs of passerines. As noted above, birdsong achieves two major functions: territory maintenance through male-male communication, and mate attraction through male-female communication. As several early ornithologists and ethologists noted, woodpecker drumming is most often heard during the breeding season; for many North American species, this occurs in the spring months, e.g. March through May (de Kiriline Lawrence, 1967). In a study of drumming patterns in male and female Red-Bellied Woodpeckers (Melanerpes carolinus), male and female individuals drummed the most just prior to pairing up (Wilkins & Ritchison, 1999). Similar to songbirds, both male and female woodpeckers drum equally readily, though early researchers focused on communication from male individuals (de Kiriline Lawrence, 1967). In his exhaustive review of acoustic communication in Picoides woodpeckers, de Kiriline Lawrence (1967) also notes that because both sexes of Downy (Picoides pubescens) and Hairy Woodpeckers (Picoides villosus) may establish territories, female individuals may drum just as frequently as males. Just as vocal signals can convey species or individual identity, research suggests that the drum sequences of woodpeckers may also convey species-specific information. In a study of 11 syntopic and allotopic woodpecker species, Stark et al. (1998) found that drum cadence, or rhythm, was the principal acoustic variable that separated syntopic species. Similarly, playback experiments with four species of Picoides woodpeckers found that individuals were more likely to respond to drum sequences if they shared similar drum cadence (Dodenhoff et al., 2001). Winkler and Short (1978) noted that while the pitch of a drum sequence may vary considerably due to drumming substrate, ambient temperature, or barometric pressure, Hairy and Downy GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN WOODPECKER DRUMS 7 Woodpecker individuals responded to drums regardless of pitch. Our knowledge, however, about just how mechanical drum sequences code for species identity is rather limited. Though dialects can have significant effects on the evolution of species, e.g. by driving sexual selection of mates, no research has been devoted to woodpeckers on this topic. Furthermore, studies on avian dialects have been restricted to their vocal signals and it is not known if sounds produced by another mechanism, in this case drumming, could also be affected by the geographic isolation of populations. Prior research has discovered some evidence for geographic variation in the Hairy Woodpecker (Winkler & Short, 1978), and the Black-Backed Woodpecker (Picoides arcticus) (Stark, 2002). Though the genus Picoides has seen some comparatively recent taxonomic adjustments to account for paraphyly, the phylogenetic relationships of Picoides themselves are quite well known, and are supported by genetic assays (Benz et al., 2006; Moore et al., 2006) and acoustic analyses (Mathevon, personal communication, 2015). Picoides have wide geographic ranges, with each species being found across much of North America in similar environments throughout temperate and boreal forests with the Downy Woodpecker also having adapted to rural, suburban as well as urban areas such as Manhattan with relative ease (personal observation). Despite such widespread ranges, most Picoides species are quite sedentary. Over the some 25-year span of his study, de Kiriline Lawrence (1967) observed the transition from migrant Downy and Hairy

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    45 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us