The Time-Course of the Generation Effect

The Time-Course of the Generation Effect

Memory & Cognition 1998,26 (1), /35-142 The time-course ofthe generation effect RODERICK W. SMITH and ALICEF. HEALY University ojColorado, Boulder, Colorado The generation effect, in which items generated by following some rule are remembered better than stimuli that are simply read, has been studied intensely over the past two decades. To date, how­ ever, researchers have largely ignored the temporal aspects of this effect. In the present research, we used a variable onset time for the presentation of the to-be-remembered material, thus providing the ability to determine at what point during processing the generation effect originates. The results in­ dicate that some benefit from generation attempts occurs even when subjects have only a few hun­ dred milliseconds in which to process the stimulus, but that more of the benefit occurs later. This finding suggests that the generation effect results from continuous or multiple discrete stages of in­ formation accrual or strengthening of memory traces over time, rather than from a single discrete increment upon final generation. The generation effect, first described by Slamecka and erations, but that recall was much lower for stimuli that Graf (1978), is a robust phenomenon in which recall or had only been read. Recognition tests showed intermedi­ recognition ofa stimulus list is enhanced ifa person must ate levels ofthe generation effect for unsolved antonyms generate the list by using some rule (e.g., rhyming, in this research. Slarnecka and Fevreiski interpreted these synonym-antonym relations, multiplication) as opposed findings as evidence that generation in this task involved to simply reading the list. Theories describing this phe­ at least two processes: generation ofa semantic code and nomenon include effort (see, e.g., Griffith, 1976; Me­ subsequent association ofthis code with a surface repre­ Farland, Frey,& Rhodes, 1980) or arousal (Jacoby, 1978); sentation (an English word, in this case). According to this semantic activation (e.g., Graf, 1980; McElroy & Sla­ interpretation, the generation failures were largely fail­ mecka, 1982); relational processes (e.g., Donaldson & ures in the second stage; the first stage was successful, Bass, 1980; Rabinowitz & Craik, 1986); multiple-factor and the generation effect derived largely from this first explanations (e.g., Hirshman & Bjork, 1988; McDaniel, stage. Riegler, & Waddill, 1990; McDaniel, Waddill, & Ein­ Although Slamecka and Fevreiski's (1983) research sug­ stein, 1988); and a procedural account (e.g., Crutcher & gests that completion ofgeneration may not be necessary Healy, 1989; McNamara & Healy, 1995). for the generation effect to occur, it leaves open the ques­ To date, however, researchers have largely ignored the tion of whether the memorial advantage for generated time-course ofthe generation effect. It is not clear whether items results from a single discrete change, a small num­ the benefit from generation is derived from a continual ber of discrete changes, or a continuous change. All of strengthening ofmemory traces during the generation pro­ these patterns have been observed in other phenomena. cess as a whole, from a small number ofdiscrete changes The speed-accuracy decomposition (SAD) technique during the course ofgeneration, or from a single discrete (Meyer, Irwin, Osman, & Kounios, 1988; see also Smith, change in memory representation at the time when gen­ Kounios, & Osterhout, 1997), for example, measures eration is completed. Slamecka and Fevreiski (1983) did partial information, or the ability of subjects to provide study the related question of whether recall or recogni­ an accurate response before processing has been com­ tion would be improved in the event of generation fail­ pleted. This measure is related to discreteness or conti­ ures. They manipulated the difficulty ofan antonym gen­ nuity, in that certain types ofcontinuous processes would eration and found that subsequent recall levels for stimuli be expected to produce rising levels of partial informa­ that had been attempted but not fully generated were tion over time, whereas certain types ofdiscrete processes roughly as high as the levels for completely successful gen- would not. Specifically, processes that continuously trans­ mit information to subsequent processes, to use Miller's (1988) terminology, would likely produce partial infor­ This research was supported in part by Army Research Institute mation, whereas those that transmit information dis­ Contract MDA903-93-K-00IO to the University of Colorado (A.H.• Principal Investigator). Correspondence should be sent to R. W.Smith, cretely would not. (This reasoning is described in greater who is now at the Institute for Research in Cognitive Science, Univer­ detail in Smith & Kounios, 1996.) sity of Pennsylvania, 3401 Walnut St., Suite 400C, Philadelphia, PA SAD research has provided evidence for processes 19104-6228 (e-mail: [email protected]), or to A. Healy, producing a slow growth or intermediate levels ofpartial Department ofPsychology, Campus Box 345, University of Colorado, information in subjects performing word recognition Boulder, CO 80309 (e-mail: [email protected]). (Meyer et al., 1988), study-test recognition memory (Rat­ -Accepted by previous editor, Geoffrey R. Loftus cliff, 1988), semantic memory access (Kounios, Osman, 135 Copyright 1998 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 136 SMITH AND HEALY & Meyer, 1987),and complex semanticrelations (Kounios, sociation: Information would be stored in one form but Montgomery, & Smith, 1994). In contrast, Smith and retrieved in another, as when a librarian shelves whole Kounios (1996) applied the same SAD technique to ana­ books but recovers them page by page. (See Crowder, gram problem solving and found little or no partial in­ 1976, pp. 264-273, for further discussion of incremen­ formation, suggesting that a single discrete shift rather tal vs. all-or-none learning.) than a continuous change was responsible for anagram In the present experiments, we investigated the time­ solution. course of the genesis of the generation effect. Because The use of metacognitive measures such asfeeling of the effect is defined in terms of recall or recognition knowing andfeeling ofwarmth (both here abbreviated as memory after an entire stimulus list has been presented, FOK) has produced results similar to those obtained with and because it is presumably produced at the time of SAD. In FOK research, subjects provide, at regular in­ study even though it is measured at the time of recall, tervals, estimates ofthe likelihood that they will produce time-of-test measures such as SAD and metacognitive an answer during a difficult "tip-of-the-tongue" recall or judgments cannot be applied to it. We therefore utilized problem-solving task. Metcalfe (1986a, 1986b) found a new extension ofa typical generation effect design. In that subjects' FOK ratings increased gradually over time a normal generation effect experiment, each to-be-re­ and predicted success in later recall of trivia questions, called item is presented to subjects immediately, typi­ but did not predict correct solution of insight problems cally with an associated cue; or only the cue is presented, or anagrams. Rather, FOK ratings for such problems did and subjects are required to produce the to-be-recalled not increase gradually over time in Metcalfe's (1986b) item. In the present experiments, we added intermediate research when the solution was correct, but incorrect so­ conditions, in which the associated cue was presented lutions were predicted by a preceding rise in FOK rat­ alone for a briefperiod, but the to-be-recalled item itself ings. Algebra problem solving, as studied by Metcalfe was presented after a delay shorter than subjects took to and Wiebe (1987), followed a pattern similar to that for generate the items in the pure-generate condition. These trivia questions. As with the SAD results, these studies intermediate conditions gave subjects time to work on suggest that some cognitive processes operate in a slow the production of the to-be-recalled item, but not so and continuous way, whereasothers produce rapid or much time that they would generally be able to complete possibly truly discrete changes. the solution before seeing it. Therefore, ifthe generation The findings from SAD and FOK studies lead to the effect is due entirely to a single discrete change in mem­ question of how the generation effect emerges over time. ory traces upon completed generation, subjects would In the studies cited above, measures of processing during gain no benefit from the intermediate conditions; but if retrieval or problem solving were used, whereas the gen­ the generation effect is the result of ongoing restructur­ eration effect presumably arises from processing during ing or elaboration during processing, the intermediate learning. The question is therefore whether the genera­ conditions would provide some, but not all, ofthe bene­ tion effect emerges in a relatively discrete fashion, as do fit of the full-generate condition. the solutions to anagram and insight problems or in a rel­ Because the existing explanations of the generation atively continuous fashion, as in retrieval in memory­ effect are largely mute with regard to its time-course, the related tasks. Note that discrete and continuous represent present experiments were not intended to help us distin­ two ends ofa continuum, as discussed by Miller (1988), guish between these theories; rather, they provide infor­ and it is possible for a process to exist in an intermediate mation that may be useful as one attempts to elaborate state, ifsome moderate number ofsteps are used to pro­ the existing theories and to tie the effect to other mem­ cess or transmit information. In a mathematical sense, ory phenomena, as detailed in the General Discussion. such a process would be considered discrete, but it is use­ The task selected for these experiments was multipli­ ful in psychology to distinguish such a process from those cation problem solution. This task has been studied in which use only one or some smaller number ofinterme­ the past (see, e.g., Crutcher & Healy, 1989; Gardiner & diate states or stages.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    8 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us