Field Guarding As a Crop Protection Method: Preliminary Implications for Improving Field Guarding Leah J

Field Guarding As a Crop Protection Method: Preliminary Implications for Improving Field Guarding Leah J

Human–Wildlife Interactions 14(3):519–530, Winter 2020 • digitalcommons.usu.edu/hwi Field guarding as a crop protection method: preliminary implications for improving field guarding Leah J. Findlay, Department of Anthropology, Durham University, Durham DH1 3LE, United Kingdom; and Primate & Predator Project, Alldays Wildlife & Communities Research Centre, P.O. Box 483, Alldays 0909, South Africa [email protected] Russell A. Hill, Department of Anthropology, Durham University, Durham DH1 3LE, United Kingdom; Primate & Predator Project, Lajuma Research Centre, P.O. Box 522, Louis Trichardt (Makhado) 0920, South Africa; and Department of Zoology, University of Venda, Thohoyandou 0950, South Africa Abstract: Negative interactions between crop farmers and wild primates are an issue of significant concern. Despite many crop farmers using field guards as a method of crop protection against foraging primates, there are very few published accounts of how effective this technique is and how it might be improved. To bridge this knowledge gap, we used direct observations from a hide to collect the behaviors of field guards, chacma baboons (Papio ursinus; baboons), and vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus pygerythrus; vervets) foraging in a 1-ha butternut squash (Cucurbita moschata) field for 4 months (May to August) in 2013 on a 564-ha commercial farm in the Blouberg District of South Africa. Only half of the crop- foraging events were chased by field guards, with vervets being chased much less frequently than baboons. Guards responded more often to events with greater primate numbers and to those that occurred earlier in the day. Guard delay in responding to crop-foraging events and baboon delay in responding to the guard both increased in the low productivity season. Baboon response delay also increased with more animals involved. Based on this case study, we suggest recommendations to improve the effectiveness of field guarding. This includes implementing an early warning alarm system, shortening field guard shifts, increasing guard numbers during the morning and low productivity season, and increasing the perceived fear of field guards, potentially by employing male guards or providing uniforms and deterrent accessories. Further evaluation in other local contexts will help determine how these findings can be adopted on a wider scale. Key words: baboon, Chlorocebus pygerythrus, crop damage, crop guarding, crop raiding, human–wildlife conflicts, mitigation, Papio ursinus, South Africa, vervet monkey, wildlife damage management The complex nature and increasing sever- 2020). As such, many agriculturalists attempt to ity of human–wildlife conflict has made it a deter primates from entering their crops using serious issue to wildlife management (Anand a number of methods. and Radhakrishna 2017). One of the most com- Strategies to keep primates and other wildlife mon causes of conflict is crop foraging (often away from crops must increase the risk of for- termed crop raiding), defined as wild animals aging enough to outweigh the nutritional ben- moving from their natural habitat onto agricul- efits of feeding on crops (Lee and Priston 2005, tural land to feed on the produce that humans Fehlmann et al. 2017) and/or use up the extra grow for their own consumption (Hill 2017a). time afforded to the animals by the increased Crop foraging results in economic and oppor- foraging efficiency of feeding on crops (Strum tunity costs for people and when retaliation is 1994, 2010; Hill 2017b). However, as primates lethal, increased mortalities for wildlife (Starin are highly intelligent and adaptable, farmers 1989, Mackenzie et al. 2015, Ango et al. 2016, often have little success preventing them from Anand et al. 2018). Primates are among the damaging their crops (Mason 1998, Warren most problematic species that damage crops, 2008, Pahad 2010, Mackenzie and Ahabyona with baboons (Papio spp.) often recorded as 2012). While many crop protection strategies the most damaging of all primates (Hill 1997, have been proposed, few have been evaluated Linkie et al. 2007, Hill 2018, Findlay and Hill for their effectiveness (Hill 2018); further work 520 Human–Wildlife Interactions 14(3) is therefore required to develop and evaluate the success of guarding. Guarding will only be effective solutions. effective if the animal being chased is afraid of Guarding is one of the most common mitiga- people. King and Lee (1987) suggest that a uni- tion strategies used by crop farmers (Naughton- formed guard known to primates as dangerous Treves 1997, Sekhar 1998, Arlet and Molleman is enough to make a group flee, while Strum 2010, Mackenzie and Ahabyona 2012). Crop (1994) suggests adding elements that animals guarding involves maintaining human presence perceive as life-threatening improves guard- at crop fields and chasing animals away when ing by increasing risk. As such, guards carrying they enter the fields to forage. Crop guarding stones, slingshots, or other accessories are more requires low financial investment (Wang et al. successful at deterring crop-foraging animals 2006) but is labor and time intensive (Hill 2005, (Osborn and Hill 2005). Men are more effective Lee and Priston 2005), and it carries the risk of at deterring primates than women or children guards being harmed by the wildlife they chase with primates retreating more readily when and contracting diseases such as malaria when approached by male guards (Strum 1994, Hill spending the extra time outdoors, especially if 1997, Strum 2010, Lemessa et al. 2013). guarding at night (Tchamba 1996, Osborn and Hill The success of crop guarding is also deter- 2005). Guarding can also lead to missed opportu- mined by how it is performed. For maximum nity costs, such as children being held back from effectiveness, guards need to take an active school to protect crops (Mackenzie et al. 2015). approach, patrolling fields and making noise Crop guarding is often perceived by farmers (Nijman and Nekaris 2010, Strum 2010, Hill as one of the most effective methods at reducing and Wallace 2012, Hill 2018). Chasing must crop damage by wildlife (Studsrød and Wegge be vigorous to use up the extra time primates 1995, Sekhar 1998, Arlet and Molleman 2010, gain from foraging on crops (Strum 1994). Thapa 2010), yet few studies have systemati- Guarding improves when performed continu- cally investigated its effectiveness (Riley 2007, ously throughout the cropping season, particu- Warren 2008, Hill and Wallace 2012, Schweitzer larly prior to and during harvests, and when et al. 2017). Furthermore, despite being the it is intensified during crop foragers’ activity favored strategy by many farmers, crop guard- peaks (Hill 2000, Lee and Priston 2005, Ango ing does not provide 100% protection against et al. 2016). Increasing the number of guards wildlife crop damage (Sekhar 1998, Hill 2000, should also increase effectiveness (Admassu Nyirenda et al. 2018), yet there is no published 2007). Lastly, cooperation between farmers literature detailing how to improve it. Lastly, can reduce costs and time investment required most current literature focuses on subsistence to guard (Marchal and Hill 2009, Hedges and farming, with no published studies about crop Gunaryadi 2010), while sharing of information guarding on commercial farms. between farmers about crop-foraging animals While most crop-foraging literature focuses has also been shown to benefit crop protection on subsistence farming (Tchamba 1996, Siex strategies (Ango et al. 2016). and Struhsaker 1999, Nahallage et al. 2008, In a paper on primate crop-foraging behav- Waters 2015), primate crop damage on commer- ior on a commercial farm in South Africa cial farms, where both large corporate farms (Findlay and Hill 2020), we showed that chacma and family farms send produce to national and baboons (Papio ursinus; baboons) caused more international markets, is also a major problem crop damage than vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus and presents challenges of its own. While com- pygerythrus; vervets), foraged on crops more mercial farmer livelihoods may not be com- in the mornings than the afternoons, and their pletely at risk from crop damage, as can be rates of crop foraging were influenced primar- the case with subsistence farmers (Naughton- ily by natural vegetation productivity, increas- Treves 1997), access to staff and technology ing significantly when normalized difference means they often have a greater impact on or vegetation index (NDVI) values dropped below even can eradicate crop foragers from their area 0.32. Vervet monkey rates of crop foraging (Lamarque et al. 2008). were primarily influenced by the presence of Several studies, however, have provided baboons. Recommendations to improve current anecdotal evidence on factors that may affect deterrent methods were also suggested, namely Field guarding as crop protection • Findlay and Hill 521 We selected a representative farm in northern Blouberg for our case study that was also the focus for our research exploring the behavior of the crop-foraging primates (Findlay and Hill 2020). Briefly, the study farm was 564 ha in size, with 80 ha for crops, and was typical to the area in terms of size, crops grown, and farming and mitigation activities. Crops had been produced on this farm for 14 years, and crop-foraging primates had been subjected to shooting for many years (commercial farmer, personal com- munication). The remaining property was used for game farming of a variety of antelope spe- cies (including Hippotragus spp., Tragelaphus

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    12 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us